Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T05:32:52.224Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Marital status, brain health, and cognitive reserve among diverse older adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 November 2024

Ji Hyun Lee*
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Community Health, Montana State University, Bozeman, MT, USA
Kiana A. Scambray
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
Emily P. Morris
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
Ketlyne Sol
Affiliation:
Social Environment and Health Program, Survey Research Center, Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
Jordan D. Palms
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
Afsara B. Zaheed
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
Michelle N. Martinez
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Houston, Houston, TX, USA
Nicole Schupf
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center, Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer’s Disease and the Aging Brain, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, NY, USA
Jennifer J. Manly
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center, Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer’s Disease and the Aging Brain, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, NY, USA
Adam M. Brickman
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center, Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer’s Disease and the Aging Brain, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, NY, USA
Laura B. Zahodne
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
*
Corresponding author: Ji Hyun Lee; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Being married may protect late-life cognition. Less is known about living arrangement among unmarried adults and mechanisms such as brain health (BH) and cognitive reserve (CR) across race and ethnicity or sex/gender. The current study examines (1) associations between marital status, BH, and CR among diverse older adults and (2) whether one’s living arrangement is linked to BH and CR among unmarried adults.

Method:

Cross-sectional data come from the Washington Heights-Inwood Columbia Aging Project (N = 778, 41% Hispanic, 33% non-Hispanic Black, 25% non-Hispanic White; 64% women). Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) markers of BH included cortical thickness in Alzheimer’s disease signature regions and hippocampal, gray matter, and white matter hyperintensity volumes. CR was residual variance in an episodic memory composite after partialing out MRI markers. Exploratory analyses stratified by race and ethnicity and sex/gender and included potential mediators.

Results:

Marital status was associated with CR, but not BH. Compared to married individuals, those who were previously married (i.e., divorced, widowed, and separated) had lower CR than their married counterparts in the full sample, among White and Hispanic subgroups, and among women. Never married women also had lower CR than married women. These findings were independent of age, education, physical health, and household income. Among never married individuals, living with others was negatively linked to BH.

Conclusions:

Marriage may protect late-life cognition via CR. Findings also highlight differential effects across race and ethnicity and sex/gender. Marital status could be considered when assessing the risk of cognitive impairment during routine screenings.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of International Neuropsychological Society

Introduction

Marital status is an important but often overlooked sociodemographic factor that could shape cognitive health in later adulthood. As one of the closest persons in one’s social network, spouses are uniquely positioned to provide immediate social support, social engagement opportunities, and other resources in the daily lives of older adults. Indeed, married individuals are generally healthier than their unmarried counterparts (Carr & Springer, Reference Carr and Springer2010; Lillard & Waite, Reference Lillard and Waite1995), including having a lower risk of cognitive impairment and dementia (Sommerlad et al., Reference Sommerlad, Ruegger, Singh-Manoux, Lewis and Livingston2018). Existing theories of how marital status can protect or harm one’s health could be applied to cognition. The marital resource model suggests that marriage provides health advantages through the availability of social, psychological, and financial resources (Rendall et al., Reference Rendall, Weden, Favreault and Waldron2011; Waite & Gallagher, Reference Waite and Gallagher2000). On the other hand, the stress model posits that the experience of marital dissolution (e.g., divorce or bereavement) can cause emotional distress and require readjustment in the household, financial, and social aspects of life (Hughes & Waite, Reference Hughes and Waite2009; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Brown and Hammersmith2017), which can compromise cognitive functioning (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Lin, Vielee, Mellencamp and Meeks2021; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Zhang, Choi, Langa and Carr2020). While a growing body of literature documents dementia risk related to marital status, less is understood about the mechanisms through which marriage can influence late-life cognition. In this study, we sought to extend the literature on marital contexts of cognitive health by examining two potential mechanisms underlying links between marital status and cognitive functioning in later life: brain health and cognitive reserve. In addition, we examined whether living with other people can provide cognitive protection for older adults without spouses and whether the mechanisms operate similarly across race and ethnicity and sex/gender.

Marital status and the risk of cognitive impairment

Prospective population-level studies have generally found an elevated risk of dementia among unmarried individuals, compared to those who are married. For example, a meta-analysis found a higher risk of dementia for widowed or lifelong single persons (20 and 42% higher, respectively) compared to married individuals (Sommerlad et al., Reference Sommerlad, Ruegger, Singh-Manoux, Lewis and Livingston2018). Older adults who experienced marital dissolution were more likely to experience cognitive impairment (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Lin, Vielee, Mellencamp and Meeks2021; Hakansson et al., Reference Hakansson, Rovio, Helkala, Vilska, Winblad, Soininen, Nissinen, Mohammed and Kivipelto2009; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Zhang, Burgard and Needham2019; Nakahori et al., Reference Nakahori, Sekine, Yamada, Tatsuse, Kido and Suzuki2021; Skirbekk et al., Reference Skirbekk, Bowen, Håberg, Jugessur, Engdahl, Bratsberg, Zotcheva, Selbæk, Kohler, Weiss, Harris, Tom, Krokstad, Stern and Strand2022; Sundström et al., Reference Sundström, Westerlund and Kotyrlo2016; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Liu and Choi2021). In most studies, the heightened risk of dementia among unmarried individuals was not fully explained by lower education levels (Sommerlad et al., Reference Sommerlad, Ruegger, Singh-Manoux, Lewis and Livingston2018), lower income (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Zhang, Burgard and Needham2019; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Liu and Choi2021), or worse health conditions (Nakahori et al., Reference Nakahori, Sekine, Yamada, Tatsuse, Kido and Suzuki2021; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Liu and Choi2021).

In a similar vein, other studies showed that being married is protective for maintaining cognitive functioning, including memory and language abilities. For example, married individuals showed better episodic memory than those who were never married, and a slower decline in memory than never married and widowed persons (Mousavi-Nasab et al., Reference Mousavi-Nasab, Kormi-Nouri, Sundström and Nilsson2012). Being married/partnered was associated with a slower decline in episodic memory even when accounting for other social network characteristics (Zahodne et al., Reference Zahodne, Ajrouch, Sharifian and Antonucci2019). Others found that widowed individuals exhibit worse language levels compared to their married counterparts but experience slower declines in visuospatial functioning (Ying et al., Reference Ying, Vonk, Sol, Brickman, Manly and Zahodne2020), revealing heterogeneous association between marital status and different cognitive domains. These findings suggest that there are protective effects of being married, but marital status may be linked to cognitive health in more nuanced ways.

An important limitation of most previous studies is that marital status was not examined in conjunction with one’s living arrangement. Many older adults who were previously married re-enter into another marital/partnership union (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Lin, Hammersmith and Wright2018) or move in with their adult children (Seltzer & Friedman, Reference Seltzer and Friedman2014). Such living arrangement could provide social resources similar to those purported to underlie the protective effects of marriage (Lee & Kim, Reference Lee, Kim and Grossberg2022). Older adults who live alone may face elevated risk of dementia, possibly due to lower economic resources (Desai et al., Reference Desai, John, Stott and Charlesworth2020). Thus, examining both marital status and living arrangement together could help to isolate the “active ingredients” of these factors that are closely linked to cognitive health in late life and reveal practical intervention opportunities, as living arrangement may be more modifiable than marital status.

Brain health and cognitive reserve

Previous studies primarily focused on documenting the association between marital status and dementia risk, but very little is known about brain mechanisms underlying these associations. Marriage may provide protection against cognitive impairment through two potential pathways: brain health and cognitive reserve. One of the ways to measure brain health (BH) is via neuroimaging data on structural integrity of the brain obtained by MRI. At any point in time, structural biomarkers of the brain can represent a confluence of genetic factors, developmental factors, and avoidance of neuropathology (e.g., brain maintenance; (Nyberg et al., Reference Nyberg, Lövdén, Riklund, Lindenberger and Bäckman2012). On the other hand, cognitive reserve (CR) is an active model in that it refers to the adaptability (e.g., capacity, efficiency, compensation) of functional neural networks in the face of aging and disease (Barulli & Stern, Reference Barulli and Stern2013; Stern, Reference Stern2012; Stern et al., Reference Stern, Arenaza-Urquijo, Bartrés-Faz, Belleville, Cantilon, Chetelat, Ewers, Franzmeier, Kempermann, Kremen, Okonkwo, Scarmeas, Soldan, Udeh-Momoh, Valenzuela, Vemuri and Vuoksimaa2020). CR can be shaped by lifetime exposure to various experiences, such as education, occupation, or social and leisure activities (Stern, Reference Stern2012).

So far, very few empirical studies have examined marital status in the context of BH or CR. One recent study (Sharifian et al., Reference Sharifian, Zaheed, Morris, Sol, Manly, Schupf, Mayeux, Brickman and Zahodne2021) showed that being married/partnered was not associated with cortical thickness in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) signature regions (i.e., brain health), nor did it moderate the link between cortical thickness and global cognition (i.e., test of cognitive reserve). However, this study did not disaggregate the various non-married statuses (e.g., never married versus divorced), consider living arrangement, or examine multiple indicators of brain health (e.g., hippocampal volume, white matter hyperintensities). Thus, examining associations between marital status, living arrangement, multiple structural MRI indicators, and cognitive reserve could shed light on potential mechanisms for protecting cognitive health.

Differential associations across race and ethnicity and sex/gender

Despite strong evidence for race and ethnicity differences in both marital status (Bloome & Ang, Reference Bloome and Ang2020) and cognitive health (Chen & Zissimopoulos, Reference Chen and Zissimopoulos2018), very few studies explored racial and ethnic differences in links between marital status and cognition. One study found that divorce and widowhood were linked to a higher risk of dementia for both older Black and White adults, but the negative effect of marital dissolution on dementia risk was stronger for Black adults than White adults (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Liu and Choi2021). This finding supports the possibility that race and ethnicity may be another critical moderator of the effect of marital status on BH and/or CR.

Further, recent studies examined sex/gender variation in the association between marital status and cognition. For example, Chen et al. (Reference Chen, Wu, Wang, Zeng, Huang, Lv, Niu, Meng, Cai, Shen, Gang, You, Lv, Ren, Shi and Ji2022) found that never being married was linked to higher risk of dementia similarly for both men (Odds ratio [OR] = 2.2) and women (OR = 2.1). However, divorce and widowhood in midlife was associated with 2.8 times higher risk of dementia only among men (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Wu, Wang, Zeng, Huang, Lv, Niu, Meng, Cai, Shen, Gang, You, Lv, Ren, Shi and Ji2022). Marital dissolution in midlife was associated with 2.75 times higher risk of dementia for men only. Similarly, being divorced or never married had a stronger negative effect on cognition for men compared to women (Kim, Reference Kim2021; Xu et al., Reference Xu, Wei, Cheng, Wang, Li, Li, Sun, Du, Sheng, Liu, Tao and Yang2021). In contrast, other studies found that lifelong marital histories were associated with better episodic memory only among women (Zaheed et al., Reference Zaheed, Sharifian, Morris, Kraal and Zahodne2021). These mixed findings may suggest that resources and stressors related to marital status operate differently across men and women to influence brain and cognitive health.

The current study

The present study examined (1) which marital statuses are associated with BH and/or CR among diverse older adults, and (2) whether one’s living arrangement is associated with BH and/or CR among those who are not currently married. Within these two aims, we further explored whether marital status, living arrangement, or their associations with cognition differ across race and ethnicity and sex/gender. We hypothesize that, compared to currently married older adults, previously married and never married older adults will show lower BH (i.e., lower cortical thickness, lower total gray matter volume, lower hippocampal volume, and more white matter hyperintensities) and/or lower CR (i.e., lower memory reserve). We also hypothesize that, among non-married older adults (e.g., previously married and never married), those who are living with others will have higher BH and CR compared to those living alone.

Method

Participants and procedure

Cross-sectional data were drawn from the Washington Heights-Inwood Columbia Aging Project (WHICAP;(Manly et al., Reference Manly, Schupf, Tang and Stern2005; Tang et al., Reference Tang, Cross, Andrews, Jacobs, Small, Bell, Merchant, Lantigua, Costa, Stern and Mayeux2001). WHICAP is an ongoing, longitudinal study of a community-based sample of older adults residing in northern Manhattan, New York. To establish the WHICAP cohort, Medicare-eligible individuals aged 65 and older who were fluent in English and/or Spanish were recruited starting in 1992. Participants were evaluated at baseline and followed up every 18 to 24 months with medical, neurological, and neuropsychological tests in their preferred language (English or Spanish). Beginning in 2011, a random subset of participants was invited to join in a longitudinal 3T magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) study. This study complied with the ethical rules for human experimentation stated in the Declaration of Helsinki. The study procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board at the Columbia University Medical Center, and all participants gave written informed consent.

To create an analytic dataset for the study, independent variables, cognitive performance, and covariates were drawn from the WHICAP wave closest to the MRI scan date. The analytic sample was limited to those without a research diagnosis of dementia and had valid data on MRI variables and independent variables of interest. Descriptive characteristics of the final sample of 778 participants are shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Sample characteristics (N = 778)

Note: aUnstandardized residual score after regressing out total intracranial volume (TICV).

Measures

Independent variables

Marital status and living arrangement were the independent variables. In WHICAP interviews, participant’s current marital status was asked in a single question with response options including married, widowed, divorced, separated, and never married. We categorized those who were widowed, divorced, or separated as previously married. Dummy variables were created to compare across three marital status groups (i.e., married, previously married, and never married).

Next, the living arrangement was assessed by asking the participant to specify everyone who they live with. Two groups were identified within the unmarried subsample: living alone and living with others (i.e., children, parent, family, or friend).

Brain health outcomes

Structural MRI indicators of brain health included cortical thickness in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) signature regions (Dickerson et al., Reference Dickerson, Bakkour, Salat, Feczko, Pacheco, Greve, Grodstein, Wright, Blacker, Rosas, Sperling, Atri, Growdon, Hyman, Morris, Fischl and Buckner2009), hippocampal volume, total gray matter volume, and total white matter hyperintensity (WMH) volume. Images were obtained on a Philips Achieva 3.0 T MRI scanner at Columbia University Medical Center. Specifications are described elsewhere (Turney et al., Reference Turney, Lao, Rentería, Igwe, Berroa, Rivera, Benavides, Morales, Rizvi, Schupf, Mayeux, Manly and Brickman2023).

Regional cortical thickness, left and right hippocampal volumes, total gray matter volume, and total intracranial volume (TICV) were quantified using FreeSurfer version 6.0 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu) with T1-weighted scans. A cortical thickness composite score was calculated by averaging the cortical thickness across hemispheres in nine regions that typically evidence AD-related neurodegeneration (Dickerson et al., Reference Dickerson, Bakkour, Salat, Feczko, Pacheco, Greve, Grodstein, Wright, Blacker, Rosas, Sperling, Atri, Growdon, Hyman, Morris, Fischl and Buckner2009). The regions of interest include rostral medial temporal lobe, inferior parietal lobe, inferior frontal lobe, inferior temporal lobe, temporal pole, precuneus, supramarginal gyrus, superior parietal lobe, and superior frontal lobe. Next, hippocampal volumes (summed across left and right hemispheres) and total gray matter volumes (divided by 100) were corrected for TICV such that the unstandardized residual scores were used after regressing against TICV (Pa et al., Reference Pa, Aslanyan, Casaletto, Rentería, Harrati, Tom, Armstrong, Rajan, Avila-Rieger, Gu, Schupf, Manly, Brickman and Zahodne2022). Lastly, total WMH volumes were quantified from T2-weighted FLAIR images (Brickman et al., Reference Brickman, Muraskin and Zimmerman2009; Brickman et al., Reference Brickman, Sneed, Provenzano, Garcon, Johnert, Muraskin, Yeung, Zimmerman and Roose2011). In brief, images were skull stripped and voxel intensity was fit with a Gaussian curve. Voxel intensities greater than 2.1 SDs above the imaging study sample mean were labeled as WMH. The total WMH volume was log-transformed to normalize their distribution for the analysis (Pa et al., Reference Pa, Aslanyan, Casaletto, Rentería, Harrati, Tom, Armstrong, Rajan, Avila-Rieger, Gu, Schupf, Manly, Brickman and Zahodne2022).

Cognitive reserve outcomes

Memory reserve was used as a primary indicator of domain-specific cognitive reserve (Reed et al., Reference Reed, Mungas, Farias, Harvey, Beckett, Widaman, Hinton and DeCarli2010; Zahodne et al., Reference Zahodne, Manly, Brickman, Siedlecki, DeCarli and Stern2013). In each core visit, WHICAP participants underwent a neuropsychological battery that assessed four domains of cognition, including episodic memory (Siedlecki et al., Reference Siedlecki, Manly, Brickman, Schupf, Tang and Stern2010; Stern, Reference Stern1992). Previous factor analysis confirmed measurement invariance between those who took the test in English and Spanish (Siedlecki et al., Reference Siedlecki, Manly, Brickman, Schupf, Tang and Stern2010). Episodic memory was assessed with immediate recall, delayed recall, and delayed recognition trials from the Selective Reminding Test (Buschke & Fuld, Reference Buschke and Fuld1974). A composite score was derived by computing z-scores of these tasks using the means and standard deviation of the baseline WHICAP sample and averaging them across tasks (Zahodne et al., Reference Zahodne, Manly, Brickman, Narkhede, Griffith, Guzman, Schupf and Stern2015). Finally, memory reserve was quantified as the residual variance of the memory composite score after regressing out the four BH indicators (i.e., cortical thickness in AD signature regions, TICV-adjusted hippocampal volume, TICV-adjusted total gray matter volume, and WMH). The resulting residual value represents the discrepancy between one’s memory performance compared to what is predicted from the degree of brain atrophy or injury (Reed et al., Reference Reed, Mungas, Farias, Harvey, Beckett, Widaman, Hinton and DeCarli2010; Zahodne et al., Reference Zahodne, Manly, Brickman, Narkhede, Griffith, Guzman, Schupf and Stern2015; Zahodne et al., Reference Zahodne, Manly, Brickman, Siedlecki, DeCarli and Stern2013). Higher residual values indicate a larger reserve.

Covariates

Demographic covariates include age (in years, at neuropsychological evaluation), education level (in years, self-reported), sex/gender (1= female, 0 = male, self-reported), and race and ethnicity. Respondent’s self-identification of race and ethnicity was coded into three mutually exclusive categories (i.e., non-Hispanic White, non-Hispanic Black, and Hispanic) which were dummy coded with non-Hispanic White as the reference group. Physical health (i.e., disease burden and functional impairment) and an additional indicator of socioeconomic status (i.e., income) were used as covariates in sensitivity analyses. Disease burden was computed as the sum of medical problems endorsed by the participant across 15 chronic conditions (i.e., hypertension, diabetes, heart disease, stroke, arthritis, COPD, thyroid, liver, renal, ulcer, peripheral vascular disease, cancer, Parkinson’s disease, essential tremor, and multiple sclerosis). Functional impairment was measured with six items asking if the participant can perform activities of daily living (ADLs); a sum score was used with higher score indicating greater impairment (Manly et al., Reference Manly, Tang, Schupf, Stern, Vonsattel and Mayeux2008). Self-rated monthly household income was operationalized as a 12-category variable (1 = $450 or less to 12 = more than $4,000) which was used as a continuous variable in the model.

Analytic strategy

Descriptive statistics were examined, and unadjusted race and ethnicity and sex/gender differences of study variables were analyzed using independent-sample T-tests and chi-square tests. A series of linear regression analyses were conducted to examine the associations between marital status and (1) BH or (2) CR. Models controlled for demographic covariates (i.e., age, sex/gender, education, race and ethnicity). Using dummy variables, the reference group was rotated to compare all three marital status categories. For example, two dummy variables were created for previously married (1) and never married (1) where the married (0) is the reference group. In the following model, similar dummy variables were utilized where the previously married group was the reference group. Separate race and ethnicity-stratified and sex/gender-stratified analyses were conducted. To answer the second aim, the sample was restricted to previously and never married participants. Two linear regression models examined the association between living arrangement and (1) BH or (2) CR, controlling for demographic covariates and marital status. Stratified models explored race and ethnicity and sex/gender differences.

Sensitivity analyses examined whether associations between marital status and BH or CR remained significant after accounting for living arrangement, disease burden, functional impairment, and household income. All analyses were conducted in SPSS Version 28. Two-sided p-values were statistically significant at .05.

Results

Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1. About half of the sample was previously married (49.6%), followed by currently married/partnered (35.2%), and never married (15.2%).

As shown in Figure 1 and summarized in Supplementary Table 1, there were differences in marital status across race and ethnicity (χ2 (4, 778) = 72.01, p = <.001) and sex/gender (χ2 (2, 778) = 84.67, p = <.001). For non-Hispanic White (hereafter White) participants, the most common marital status was married (48%), with similar proportions previously and never married (30 and 22%, respectively). In contrast, the most common marital status for non-Hispanic Black (hereafter Black) and Hispanic participants was previously married (56 and 57%, respectively). One-fifth of Black participants were never married (21%), while very few Hispanic participants were never married (6%). In terms of sex/gender, more than half of men (55%) were currently married compared to only 24% of women. More than half (61%) of women were previously married.

Figure 1. Marital status by race and ethnicity and sex/gender.

In terms of demographic and health covariates, Hispanic participants were older (eta-squared = .02, hereafter η 2) and had higher functional impairment (η 2 = .04) than White or Black participants, who were similar to each other. White participants had the highest education (η 2 = .43), and income (η 2 = .33), followed by Black participants, then Hispanic participants. White participants had lower disease burden (η 2 = .04) than Black or Hispanic participants, who were similar to each other. Across sex/gender, women had less education (Cohen’s d = .17, hereafter d), had more disease burden (d = .28), and had less income (d = .18) than men.

In terms of BH/CR outcomes, White participants had the highest cortical thickness (η 2 = .02) and greater gray matter volume (η 2 = .05) compared to Black or Hispanic participants, who were similar. Hippocampal volume did not differ across race and ethnic groups. Black participants had greater WMH volume (η 2 = .01) than White or Hispanic participants, who were similar. White participants had greater CR, followed by Black, then Hispanic participants (η 2 = .11). Across sex/gender, women had greater cortical thickness (d = .20), lower gray matter volume (d = .41), lower hippocampal volume (d = .24), and greater CR (d = .25) than men. Men and women did not differ in WMH volume.

Marital status

The associations between marital status and each BH and CR outcomes in the full sample are presented in Table 2. Marital status was not associated with any BH indicator. However, marital status was linked to CR such that previously married participants had lower CR than their married counterparts (β = −0.08, p = .036). Never married participants did not differ in CR compared to their married counterparts (β = −0.04, p = .329) nor previously married counterparts (β = 0.02, p = .524).

Table 2. Associations between marital status and brain health or cognitive reserve

Note: Estimates represent b = unstandardized coefficient [Lower Bound, Upper Bound 95% confidence interval], β = standardized coefficient, * p < .05. The models controlled for age, sex/gender, education, and race and ethnicity. aUnstandardized residual score after regressing out total intracranial volume (TICV). bUnstandardized residual episodic memory composite score after regressing out cortical thickness in AD signature regions, TICV-adjusted hippocampal volume, TICV-adjusted total gray matter volume, and WMH.

These patterns were not consistent across race and ethnicity or sex/gender. In race and ethnicity-stratified models (Table 3), previously married participants had lower CR than married counterparts among White (β = −0.21, p = .006) and Hispanic (β = −0.08, p = .032) participants, but not among Black participants (β = 0.07, p = .352). When stratified by sex/gender (Table 4), both previously married women (β =−0.12, p = .012) and never married women (β = −0.12, p = .016) had lower CR compared to married women. There were no associations between marital status and CR among men.

Table 3. Race and ethnicity-stratified models of associations between marital status and brain health and cognitive reserve

Note: Estimates represent b = unstandardized coefficient [Lower Bound, Upper Bound 95% confidence interval], β = standardized coefficient, * p < .05. The models controlled for age, sex/gender, and education.

Table 4. Sex/gender-stratified models of associations between marital status and brain health and cognitive reserve

Note: Estimates represent b = unstandardized coefficient [Lower Bound, Upper Bound 95% confidence interval], β = standardized coefficient, * p < .05. The models controlled for age, race and ethnicity, and education.

A series of sensitivity analyses performed. In a model that categorized marital status into four groups (i.e., divorced/separated and widowed were distinguished), among White and Hispanic participants, the lower CR effects of previously married groups were driven by the divorced group. Among women, both divorced and widowed participants showed lower CR than married counterparts (Supplementary Table 2). Additionally, associations described in the main model remained significant after controlling for living arrangement, disease burden, functional impairment, and household income (Supplementary Table 3).

Role of living arrangement among non-married older adults

Among those who were previously married (n = 386), their current living arrangement was not associated with their BH or CR (Table 5). Among those who were never married (n = 118), participants living with others had lower cortical thickness than those living alone (β = −0.19 p = .030) but were similar in other indicators of BH and in CR. Stratified models of never married subsample (Supplementary Table 4) revealed that lower cortical thickness of those living with others was only found among Hispanic participants (β = −0.56, p = .029). Additionally, living with others was only associated with smaller total gray matter volume (β = −0.27, p = .046) and hippocampal volume (β = −0.40, p = .004) among never married White participants.

Table 5. Associations between living arrangement (living with others compared to living alone) and brain health and cognitive reserve among unmarried adults

Note: Models controlled for age, sex/gender, education, race and ethnicity. * p < .05.

Discussion

The present study of older adults examined whether marital status is associated with indicators of BH and CR, as well as the association between living arrangement and these outcomes among currently unmarried individuals. Marital status was associated with CR, but not BH, in this sample. Compared to being married, being previously married (i.e., divorced, widowed, and separated) was associated with lower CR in the full sample, among White and Hispanic participants and among women, but not among Black participants or men. Only among women, never being married was also linked to lower CR compared to being currently married. These associations persisted even after controlling for living arrangement, disease burden, functional impairment, and household income. Currently living with others was associated with worse BH among never married individuals, but not among previously married individuals. Overall, these results show that marital status is a salient sociodemographic factor associated with cognitive health, especially in the cognitive reserve of older adults.

Marital status and cognitive reserve

We found that previously married older adults had lower CR than their married counterparts in general. This finding complements many of the previous studies that examined marital loss and dementia risk among older adults (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Lin, Vielee, Mellencamp and Meeks2021; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Zhang, Burgard and Needham2019, Reference Liu, Zhang, Choi, Langa and Carr2020; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Zhang and Zhang2021; Sommerlad et al., Reference Sommerlad, Ruegger, Singh-Manoux, Lewis and Livingston2018; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Liu and Choi2021). There are several reasons why marital status may be associated with CR. In line with the marital resource model, the dissolution of marriage via divorce, widowhood, or separation would be linked to considerable disruption in socioeconomic resources that were accessible during marriage. It is notable that the negative effect of experiencing marital dissolution on cognitive reserve persisted even after controlling for income, which is also consistent with the existing studies (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Zhang, Choi, Langa and Carr2020; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Liu and Choi2021). This finding suggest that the benefit of being married is not fully explained by access to financial resources. Although, we note that we only examined current household income. Given that our sample is older adults, it is possible that wealth and assets may be more influential indicators of financial resources and may also better capture lifelong access to shared financial resources via marriage. It is also possible that benefits of marriage come from non-financial aspects such as interpersonal processes. Having a spouse can provide cognitive stimulation and social resource sharing that can build and maintain CR. Living with a spouse or partner often involves cognitively demanding conversations rooted in shared experiences as well as perspective taking. For example, collaborative social interactions that involve social cognition (e.g., empathy, mentalizing, symbolic interaction) engage executive functions (Ybarra et al., Reference Ybarra, Burnstein, Winkielman, Keller, Manis, Chan and Rodriguez2008; Ybarra et al., Reference Ybarra, Winkielman, Yeh, Burnstein and Kavanagh2011). These frequent spousal interactions could be an important source of social and cognitive stimulation (Fratiglioni et al., Reference Fratiglioni, Paillard-Borg and Winblad2004), which is a key mechanism in building and maintaining CR (Barulli & Stern, Reference Barulli and Stern2013). In addition, the benefits of individual-level resources such as education can be shared in couples. It is shown that one’s spouse’s education level is associated with a higher level of and slower decline in cognitive functioning independent of their own education (Xu, Reference Xu and Neupert2019). Education is a primary source of CR that attenuates the association of brain pathology and cognition (Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Wilson, Schneider, Evans, Mendes de Leon, Arnold and Bienias2003); marriage could promote CR by extending the benefits of education to both partners.

The stress model may further help to explain the negative association between marital dissolution and CR. Often deemed as one of the most stressful life events (Miller & Rahe, Reference Miller and Rahe1997), death of the spouse involves grief that can greatly affect mental health, especially in the acute phase (Lin & Brown, Reference Lin and Brown2020). In divorce and separation, lower CR may not be stemming from the end of marriage itself but from the extended exposure to negative marital quality that would have proceeded it (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Zhang and Zhang2021). Practical stressors such as a decrease in income or managing responsibilities alone could lead to chronic stress that could also hinder maintenance of CR. In the reversed case, marriage may be protective of CR because intimacy and emotional support provided by one’s spouse can buffer the effects of external stressors. To shed light on the processes underlying associations between marital dissolution and CR, future studies could investigate the role of mediators such as loneliness or loss of social network, which are linked to worse cognitive functioning (Harrington et al., Reference Harrington, Vasan, Kang, Sliwinski and Lim2023).

Regarding sex/gender differences, detrimental effects of marital dissolution and never entering marriage on CR were only found among women. Some studies have reported similar findings, such that one’s marital history was impactful on memory trajectory only for women (Zaheed et al., Reference Zaheed, Sharifian, Morris, Kraal and Zahodne2021). However, the literature presents mixed findings, with some studies indicating that men may be more susceptible to cognitive decline associated with marital disruption (Feng et al., Reference Feng, Ng, Yap, Li, Lee, Håkansson, Kua and Ng2014; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Liu and Choi2021; van Gelder et al., Reference van Gelder, Tijhuis, Kalmijn, Giampaoli, Nissinen and Kromhout2006). Again, the resource model may help to explain the salience of marital status for women’s CR. Women face substantial disruption in household income and assets after divorce or widowhood (Angel et al., Reference Angel, Jiménez and Angel2007). It is possible that older women have more limited access to resources outside of marriage than men due to structural socioeconomic inequities experienced over their lifetime (Angel et al., Reference Angel, Jiménez and Angel2007).

We found that marital status was not associated with CR among Black older adults. This finding is in contrast to a previous study that found stronger negative effects of divorce and widowhood on dementia risk for Black older adults compared to White older adults (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Liu and Choi2021). However, the discrepancy of findings may stem from differences in outcome (i.e., risk of being classified as having dementia vs. a memory residual reflecting cognitive reserve) and marital categories (i.e., we combined divorced and widowed). It is possible that higher involvement with extended families and religious congregations (Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Chatters, Woodward and Brown2013) protect Black older adults from the negative effects of marital dissolution or never entering marriage. Future studies could explore other mediators such as social network engagement and participation to detangle the mechanisms of marital effects and its racial/ethnic differences.

Marital status and brain health

Contrary to our hypothesis, marital status was not associated with any indicators of BH in the full sample or among sociodemographic subgroups. This is consistent with Sharifian et al., (2022)’s null finding on cortical thickness. The fact that we did not find an effect of marital status on BH outcomes but only in CR suggests that the commonly reported marital status effects on cognition are more likely to be a functional process rather than a reflection of structural changes and neuropathology. Future studies with larger sample and a more direct indicator of neuropathology (e.g., amyloid) could be useful to corroborate our null findings.

The role of living arrangement

Contrary to our hypotheses, current living arrangement (i.e., living alone vs. living with others) were not associated with CR or BH for previously married older adults. Living with others was linked to lower cortical thickness compared to living alone among those who were never married. Because it is a cross-sectional study, we cannot rule out the possibility of reverse causation where declining cognition could have prompted older adults to live with others rather than remain living alone. Additionally, these results are based on a very small subsample and may not be reliable. Furthermore, living alone, particularly as a never married person, may not be an adequate indicator of social isolation or a lack of social stimulation because many older adults prefer to live near, rather than with, their families (Raymo et al., Reference Raymo, Pike, Liang and Brown2019). Future longitudinal studies should consider these decision processes to better capture the nuanced relationship between living arrangement and cognitive health for unmarried older adults.

Limitations and future directions

There are limitations to this study. First, the study used an assessment of marital status at a single time point, which does not capture the lifetime marital history. Growing evidence shows that many factors contribute to the complexity in links between marital status and cognitive impairment; duration of being unmarried (Zaheed et al., Reference Zaheed, Sharifian, Morris, Kraal and Zahodne2021), timing of divorce and widowhood (Zhang et al., 2022), and relationship quality within marriage (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Zhang and Zhang2021) have been shown to have cognitive implications. Second, the residual method of quantifying CR may capture unmeasured aspects of brain health. However, we addressed this limitation by including structural MRI indicators beyond those included in foundational studies (Reed et al., Reference Reed, Mungas, Farias, Harvey, Beckett, Widaman, Hinton and DeCarli2010; Zahodne et al., Reference Zahodne, Manly, Brickman, Siedlecki, DeCarli and Stern2013). Third, living arrangement of the WHICAP sample in Northern Manhattan may not adequately represent older adults living in suburban/rural regions. Additional research is needed to determine whether these results would generalize beyond this specific urban context. However, there are many strengths of the study which include the examination of multiple MRI based BH biomarkers, examination of BH and CR within the same sample, analysis of heterogeneity across racial/ethnic and sex/gender groups of similar sizes, disaggregation of unmarried individuals into those who are previously versus never married, consideration of living arrangement in addition to marital status, and the use of a community-based sample.

Conclusions

The current study showed that marital dissolution and never being married may be harmful to cognitive health by hindering the development or maintenance of CR for many groups of older adults. Lack of evidence for BH suggests that CR may be a more prominent pathway linking marital status to cognition. Different patterns of association across race/ethnicity and sex/gender point to differential impact of life experiences on cognitive health. Our findings can lead future research seeking to identify modifiable social resources relevant to dementia risk, help practitioners to recognize high-risk individuals based on marital status, and inform the development of targeted interventions.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617724000638.

Acknowledgements

Data collection and sharing for this project was supported by the Washington Heights-Inwood Columbia Aging Project (R01AG054520, R01AG072474, P01AG07232, R01AG037212, RF1AG054023, R56AG034189, R01AG034189) funded by the National Institute on Aging (NIA). This manuscript has been reviewed by WHICAP investigators for scientific content and consistency of data interpretation with previous WHICAP publications. We acknowledge the WHICAP study participants and the WHICAP research and support staff for their contributions to this study. Ji Hyun Lee was supported by Alzheimer’s Association [AARFD-22-970207]; Emily P. Morris was supported by NIA [F31AG077758]; Afsara Zaheed was supported by NIA [F31AG067717]; Ketlyne Sol was supported by NIA [P30AG059300; K01AG073588, R01AG082307], National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences [KL2TR002241; UL1TR002240], and the Antonia Lemstra Fund at the Michigan Alzheimer’s Disease Resource Center.

Competing interests

The authors report no conflicts of interest.

References

Angel, J. L., Jiménez, M. A., & Angel, R. J. (2007). The economic consequences of widowhood for older minority women. The Gerontologist, 47, 224234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barulli, D., & Stern, Y. (2013). Efficiency, capacity, compensation, maintenance, plasticity: Emerging concepts in cognitive reserve. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 502509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennett, D. A., Wilson, R. S., Schneider, J. A., Evans, D. A., Mendes de Leon, C. F., Arnold, S. E., & Bienias, J. L. (2003). Education modifies the relation of AD pathology to level of cognitive function in older persons. Neurology, 60, 19091915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloome, D., & Ang, S. (2020). Marriage and union formation in the United States: Recent trends across racial groups and economic backgrounds. Demography, 57, 17531786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brickman, A. M., Muraskin, J., & Zimmerman, M. E. (2009). Structural neuroimaging in Altheimer’s disease: Do white matter hyperintensities matter? Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 11, 181190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brickman, A. M., Sneed, J. R., Provenzano, F. A., Garcon, E., Johnert, L., Muraskin, J., Yeung, L.-K., Zimmerman, M. E., & Roose, S. P. (2011). Quantitative approaches for assessment of white matter hyperintensities in elderly populations. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 193, 101106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, S. L., Lin, I.-F., Hammersmith, A. M., & Wright, M. R. (2018). Later life marital dissolution and repartnership status: A national portrait. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 73, 10321042.Google ScholarPubMed
Brown, S. L., Lin, I.-F., Vielee, A., Mellencamp, K. A., & Meeks, S. (2021). Midlife marital dissolution and the onset of cognitive impairment. The Gerontologist, 61, 10851094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buschke, H., & Fuld, P. A. (1974). Evaluating storage, retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and learning. Neurology, 24, 10191019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carr, D., & Springer, K. W. (2010). Advances in families and health research in the 21st century. Journal of Marriage and Family; Minneapolis, 72, 743761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, C., & Zissimopoulos, J. M. (2018). Racial and ethnic differences in trends in dementia prevalence and risk factors in the United States. Alzheimer’s & Dementia : Translational Research & Clinical Interventions, 4, 510520.Google ScholarPubMed
Chen, Z. C., Wu, H., Wang, X. D., Zeng, Y., Huang, G., Lv, Y., Niu, J., Meng, X., Cai, P., Shen, L., Gang, B., You, Y., Lv, Y., Ren, Z., Shi, Z., & Ji, Y. (2022). Association between marital status and cognitive impairment based on a cross-sectional study in China. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 37, gps.5649.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desai, R., John, A., Stott, J., & Charlesworth, G. (2020). Living alone and risk of dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Research Reviews, 62, 101122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickerson, B. C., Bakkour, A., Salat, D. H., Feczko, E., Pacheco, J., Greve, D. N., Grodstein, F., Wright, C. I., Blacker, D., Rosas, H. D., Sperling, R. A., Atri, A., Growdon, J. H., Hyman, B. T., Morris, J. C., Fischl, B., & Buckner, R. L. (2009). The cortical signature of Alzheimer’s disease: Regionally specific cortical thinning relates to symptom severity in very mild to mild AD dementia and is detectable in asymptomatic amyloid-positive individuals. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 497510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feng, L., Ng, X.-T., Yap, P., Li, J., Lee, T.-S., Håkansson, K., Kua, E.-H., & Ng, T.-P. (2014). Marital status and cognitive impairment among community-dwelling chinese older adults: The role of gender and social engagement. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders Extra, 4, 375384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fratiglioni, L., Paillard-Borg, S., & Winblad, B. (2004). An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. The Lancet Neurology, 3, 343353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hakansson, K., Rovio, S., Helkala, E.-L., Vilska, A.-R., Winblad, B., Soininen, H., Nissinen, A., Mohammed, A. H., & Kivipelto, M. (2009). Association between mid-life marital status and cognitive function in later life: Population based cohort study. BMJ, 339, b2462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrington, K. D., Vasan, S., Kang, J. E., Sliwinski, M. J., & Lim, M. H. (2023). Loneliness and cognitive function in older adults without dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 91, 12431259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, M. E., & Waite, L. J. (2009). Marital biography and health at mid-life. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 50, 344358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, Y. (2021). Gender differences in the link between marital status and the risk of cognitive impairment: Results from the korean longitudinal study of aging. International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 94, 415435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, S. R., Kim, L. S., & Grossberg, G. T. (2022). Coresidence of older parents and adult children increases older adults’ self-reported psychological well-being. International Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 2022, 5406196.Google ScholarPubMed
Lillard, L. A., & Waite, L. J. (1995). Til death do us part: Marital disruption and mortality. American Journal of Sociology, 100, 11311156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, I.-F., & Brown, S. L. (2020). Consequences of later-life divorce and widowhood for adult well-being: A call for the convalescence model. Journal of Family Theory & Review, 12, 264277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, I.-F., Brown, S. L., & Hammersmith, A. M. (2017). Marital biography, social security receipt, and poverty. Research on Aging, 39, 86110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, H., Zhang, Y., Burgard, S. A., & Needham, B. L. (2019). Marital status and cognitive impairment in the United States: Evidence from the national health and aging trends study. Annals of Epidemiology, 38, 2834.e2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, H., Zhang, Z., Choi, S.-W., Langa, K. M., & Carr, D. (2020). Marital status and dementia: Evidence from the health and retirement study. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 75, 17831795.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, H., Zhang, Z., & Zhang, Y. (2021). A national longitudinal study of marital quality and cognitive decline among older men and women. Social Science & Medicine, 282, 114151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manly, J. J., Schupf, N., Tang, M.-X., & Stern, Y. (2005). Cognitive decline and literacy among ethnically diverse elders. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 18, 213217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manly, J. J., Tang, M.-X., Schupf, N., Stern, Y., Vonsattel, J.-P. G., & Mayeux, R. (2008). Frequency and course of mild cognitive impairment in a multiethnic community. Annals of Neurology, 63, 494506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, M. A., & Rahe, R. H. (1997). Life changes scaling for the 1990s. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 43, 279292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mousavi-Nasab, S.-M.-H., Kormi-Nouri, R., Sundström, A., & Nilsson, L.-G. (2012). The effects of marital status on episodic and semantic memory in healthy middle-aged and old individuals. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 53, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakahori, N., Sekine, M., Yamada, M., Tatsuse, T., Kido, H., & Suzuki, M. (2021). Association between marital status and cognitive function in Japan: Results from the Toyama dementia survey. Psychogeriatrics : The Official Journal of the Japanese Psychogeriatric Society, 21, 627635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nyberg, L., Lövdén, M., Riklund, K., Lindenberger, U., & Bäckman, L. (2012). Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 292305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pa, J., Aslanyan, V., Casaletto, K. B., Rentería, M. A., Harrati, A., Tom, S. E., Armstrong, N., Rajan, K., Avila-Rieger, J., Gu, Y., Schupf, N., Manly, J. J., Brickman, A., & Zahodne, L. (2022). Effects of sex, APOE4, and lifestyle activities on cognitive reserve in older adults. Neurology, 99, e789e798.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raymo, J. M., Pike, I., Liang, J., & Brown, J. S. (2019). A new look at the living arrangements of older Americans using multistate life tables. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 74, e84e96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reed, B. R., Mungas, D., Farias, S. T., Harvey, D., Beckett, L., Widaman, K., Hinton, L., & DeCarli, C. (2010). Measuring cognitive reserve based on the decomposition of episodic memory variance. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 133, 21962209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rendall, M. S., Weden, M. M., Favreault, M. M., & Waldron, H. (2011). The protective effect of marriage for survival: A review and update. Demography, 48, 481506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seltzer, J. A., & Friedman, E. M. (2014). Widowed mothers’ coresidence with adult children. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 69, 6374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharifian, N., Zaheed, A. B., Morris, E. P., Sol, K., Manly, J. J., Schupf, N., Mayeux, R., Brickman, A. M., & Zahodne, L. B. (2021). Social network characteristics moderate associations between cortical thickness and cognitive functioning in older adults. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 1, 2383.Google Scholar
Siedlecki, K. L., Manly, J. J., Brickman, A. M., Schupf, N., Tang, M.-X., & Stern, Y. (2010). Do neuropsychological tests have the same meaning in Spanish speakers as they do in English speakers? Neuropsychology, 24, 402411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skirbekk, V., Bowen, C. E., Håberg, A., Jugessur, A., Engdahl, B., Bratsberg, B., Zotcheva, E., Selbæk, G., Kohler, H.-P., Weiss, J., Harris, J. R., Tom, S. E., Krokstad, S., Stern, Y., Strand, Børn H. (2022). Marital histories and associations with later-life dementia and mild cognitive impairment risk in the HUNT4 70+ study in Norway. Journal of Aging and Health, 35, 543–555.Google Scholar
Sommerlad, A., Ruegger, J., Singh-Manoux, A., Lewis, G., & Livingston, G. (2018). Marriage and risk of dementia: Systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 89, 231238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y. (1992). Diagnosis of dementia in a heterogeneous population: Development of a neuropsychological paradigm-based diagnosis of dementia and quantified correction for the effects of education. Archives of Neurology, 49, 453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurology, 11, 10061012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y., Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Bartrés-Faz, D., Belleville, S., Cantilon, M., Chetelat, G., Ewers, M., Franzmeier, N., Kempermann, G., Kremen, W. S., Okonkwo, O., Scarmeas, N., Soldan, A., Udeh-Momoh, C., Valenzuela, M., Vemuri, P., Vuoksimaa, E., & The Reserve, Resilience and Protective Factors PIA Empirical Definitions and Conceptual Frameworks Workgroup (2020). Whitepaper: Defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 16, 13051311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sundström, A., Westerlund, O., & Kotyrlo, E. (2016). Marital status and risk of dementia: A nationwide population-based prospective study from Sweden. BMJ Open, 6, e008565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tang, M.-X., Cross, P., Andrews, H., Jacobs, D. M., Small, S., Bell, K., Merchant, C., Lantigua, R., Costa, R., Stern, Y., & Mayeux, R. (2001). Incidence of AD in African-Americans, Caribbean Hispanics, and Caucasians in northern Manhattan. Neurology, 56, 4956.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, R. J., Chatters, L. M., Woodward, A. T., & Brown, E. (2013). Racial and ethnic differences in extended family, friendship, fictive kin and congregational informal support networks. Family Relations, 62, 609624.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turney, I. C., Lao, P. J., Rentería, M. A., Igwe, K. C., Berroa, J., Rivera, A., Benavides, A., Morales, C. D., Rizvi, B., Schupf, N., Mayeux, R., Manly, J. J., & Brickman, A. M. (2023). Brain aging among racially and ethnically diverse middle-aged and older adults. JAMA Neurology, 80, 73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Gelder, B. M., Tijhuis, M., Kalmijn, S., Giampaoli, S., Nissinen, A., & Kromhout, D. (2006). Marital status and living situation during a 5-year period are associated with a subsequent 10-year cognitive decline in older men: The FINE study. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 61, P213–9.Google ScholarPubMed
Waite, L. J., & Gallagher, M. (2000). The case for marriage: Why married people are happier, healthier, and better off financially: Broadway books.Google Scholar
Xu, M., Neupert, S. (2019). Spousal education and cognitive functioning in later life. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 75, e141e150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xu, P.-R., Wei, R., Cheng, B.-J., Wang, A.-J., Li, X.-D., Li, H.-B., Sun, L., Du, J., Sheng, J., Liu, K.-Y., Tao, F.-B., & Yang, L.-S. (2021). The association of marital status with cognitive function and the role of gender in Chinese community-dwelling older adults: A cross-sectional study. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research, 33, 22732281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ybarra, O., Burnstein, E., Winkielman, P., Keller, M. C., Manis, M., Chan, E., & Rodriguez, J. (2008). Mental exercising through simple socializing: Social interaction promotes general cognitive functioning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 248259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ybarra, O., Winkielman, P., Yeh, I., Burnstein, E., & Kavanagh, L. (2011). Friends (and sometimes enemies) with cognitive benefits: What types of social interactions boost executive functioning? Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2, 253261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ying, G., Vonk, J. M. J., Sol, K., Brickman, A. M., Manly, J. J., & Zahodne, L. B. (2020). Family ties and aging in a multiethnic cohort. Journal of Aging and Health, 32, 14641474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zaheed, A. B., Sharifian, N., Morris, E. P., Kraal, A. Z., & Zahodne, L. B. (2021). Associations between life course marital biography and late-life memory decline. Psychology and Aging, 36, 557571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahodne, L. B., Ajrouch, K. J., Sharifian, N., & Antonucci, T. C. (2019). Social relations and age-related change in memory. Psychology and Aging, 34, 751765.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahodne, L. B., Manly, J. J., Brickman, A. M., Narkhede, A., Griffith, E. Y., Guzman, V. A., Schupf, N., & Stern, Y. (2015). Is residual memory variance a valid method for quantifying cognitive reserve? A longitudinal application. Neuropsychologia, 77, 260266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahodne, L. B., Manly, J. J., Brickman, A. M., Siedlecki, K. L., DeCarli, C., & Stern, Y. (2013). Quantifying cognitive reserve in older adults by decomposing episodic memory variance: Replication and extension. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19, 854862.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahodne, L. B., Wall, M. M., Schupf, N., Mayeux, R., Manly, J. J., Stern, Y., & Brickman, A. M. (2015). Late-life memory trajectories in relation to incident dementia and regional brain atrophy. Journal of Neurology, 262, 24842490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, Z., Liu, H., & Choi, S.-W. E. (2021). Marital loss and risk of dementia: Do race and gender matter? Social Science & Medicine, 275, 113808.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample characteristics (N = 778)

Figure 1

Figure 1. Marital status by race and ethnicity and sex/gender.

Figure 2

Table 2. Associations between marital status and brain health or cognitive reserve

Figure 3

Table 3. Race and ethnicity-stratified models of associations between marital status and brain health and cognitive reserve

Figure 4

Table 4. Sex/gender-stratified models of associations between marital status and brain health and cognitive reserve

Figure 5

Table 5. Associations between living arrangement (living with others compared to living alone) and brain health and cognitive reserve among unmarried adults

Supplementary material: File

Lee et al. supplementary material

Lee et al. supplementary material
Download Lee et al. supplementary material(File)
File 36.2 KB