Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T05:01:24.608Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Impaired Visual Scanning and Memory for Faces in High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders: It's Not Just the Eyes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 September 2011

Joseph Snow*
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland
John E. Ingeholm
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland
Ilana F. Levy
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland
Rachel A. Caravella
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland
Laura K. Case
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland
Gregory L. Wallace
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland
Alex Martin
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Joseph Snow, National Institute of Mental Health, 10 Center Drive, Room 3N222, Bethesda, MD 20892. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Prior studies suggest that autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are associated with a domain-specific memory impairment for faces. The underlying cause of this problem and its relation to impaired visual scanning of faces—particularly of the eyes—remains to be determined. We recorded eye movements while 22 high-functioning ASD and 21 typically developing (TD) adolescents encoded and later recognized faces and objects from a single, nonsocial object category (electric fans). Relative to TD subjects, ASD individuals had poorer memory for faces, but not fans. Correlational analyses showed significant relationships between recognition memory and fixations. Eye tracking during encoding revealed that TD subjects made more fixations to faces than fans, whereas ASD individuals did not differ in number of fixations made to each stimulus type. Moreover, although both the TD and ASD groups showed a strong preference for fixating the eyes more than the mouth, the ASD subjects were less likely than TD subjects to scan regions of the face outside of the primary facial features (i.e., eyes, nose, and mouth). We concluded that ASD individuals have a domain-specific memory impairment for faces relative to mechanical objects and that this impairment may be related to abnormal scanning during encoding. (JINS, 2011, 17, 1021–1029)

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Achenbach, T.M., Rescorla, L.A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles: An integrated system of multi-informant assessment. Burlington, VT: ASEBA.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) (4th ed.). Washington DC: APA.Google Scholar
Anderson, C.J., Colombo, J., Shaddy, J.D. (2006). Visual scanning and pupillary responses in young children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28(7), 12381256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bar-Haim, Y., Shulman, C., Lamy, D., Reuveni, A. (2006). Attention to the eyes and mouth in high-functioning children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 131137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, R.J., Frith, U., Smith, N., Abell, F., Cipolotti, L. (2002). Fractionation of visual memory: Agency detection and its impairment in autism. Neuropsychologia, 40(1), 108118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boraston, Z., Blakemore, S.J. (2007). The application of eye-tracking technology in the study of autism. Journal of Physiology, 581(3), 893898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boucher, J., Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face recognition in relatively able autistic-children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(5), 843859.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, G., Webb, S., Schellenberg, G.D., Dager, S., Friedman, S., Aylward, E., Richards, T. (2002). Defining the broader phenotype of autism: Genetic, brain, and behavioral perspectives. Development and Psychopathology, 14(3), 581611.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher-Watson, S., Leekam, S.R., Benson, V., Frank, M.C., Findlay, J.M. (2009). Eye-movements reveal attention to social information in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 47(1), 248257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher-Watson, S., Leekam, S.R., Findlay, J.M., Stanton, E.C. (2008). Brief report: Young adults with autism spectrum disorder show normal attention to eye-gaze information-evidence from a new change blindness paradigm. Journal of Autism Developmental Disorders, 38(9), 17851790.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grelotti, D.J., Gauthier, I., Schultz, R.T. (2002). Social interest and the development of cortical face specialization: What autism teaches us about face processing. Developmental Psychobiology, 40(3), 213225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harms, M.B., Martin, A., Wallace, G.L. (2010). Facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: A review of behavioral and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychology Review, 20(3), 290322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauck, M., Fein, D., Maltby, N., Waterhouse, L., Feinstein, C. (1998). Memory for faces in children with autism. Child Neuropsychology, 4(3), 187198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ishai, A., Pessoa, L., Bikle, P.C., Ungerleider, L.G. (2004). Repetition suppression of faces is modulated by emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(26), 98279832.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(9), 809816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lainhart, J.E., Bigler, E.D., Bocian, M., Coon, H., Dinh, E., Dawson, G., Volkmar, F. (2006). Head circumference and height in autism: A study by the Collaborative Program of Excellence in Autism. American Journal of Medical Genetics A, 140(21), 22572274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Le Couteur, A., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Rios, P., Robertson, S., Holdgrafer, M., McLennan, J. (1989). Autism diagnostic interview: A standardized investigator-based instrument. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19(3), 363387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., Risi, S. (1999). Autism diagnostic observation schedule-WPS edition. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Lord, C., Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., Ohman, A. (1998). The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces—KDEF, CD Rom from Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Psychology section. Stockholm: Karolinska Institute.Google Scholar
Mann, T.A., Walker, P. (2003). Autism and a deficit in broadening the spread of visual attention. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(2), 274284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osterling, J., Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(3), 247257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pelphrey, K., Sasson, N., Reznick, J., Paul, G., Goldman, B. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(4), 249261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosch, E.H. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 2748). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Rutherford, M.D., Clements, K.A., Sekuler, A.B. (2007). Differences in discrimination of eye and mouth displacement in autism spectrum disorders. Vision Research, 47(15), 20992110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutherford, M.D., Towns, A.M. (2008). Scan path differences and similarities during emotion perception in those with and without autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(7), 13711381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Speer, L.L., Cook, A.E., McMahon, W.M., Clark, E. (2007). Face processing in children with autism: Effects of stimulus contents and type. Autism, 11(3), 265277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spezio, M., Adolphs, R., Hurley, R., Piven, J. (2006). Analysis of face gaze in autism using “Bubbles”. Neuropsychologia, 45(1), 144151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sterling, L., Dawson, G., Webb, S., Murias, M., Munson, J., Panagiotides, H., Aylward, E. (2008). The role of face familiarity in eye tracking of faces by individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 16661675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, D.L., Goldstein, G., Minshew, N.J. (2005). Impaired memory for faces and social scenes in autism: Clinical implications of memory dysfunction. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20(1), 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yarbus, A.L. (1967). Eye movements during perception of complex objects. Eye Movements and Vision, 7, 171196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar