Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T19:53:19.017Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Executive function in patients with obstructive sleep apnea treated with continuous positive airway pressure

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 August 2010

ESTHER YUET YING LAU*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China
GAIL A. ESKES
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia Department of Medicine, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia
DEBRA L. MORRISON
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia Sleep Clinic and Laboratory, Queen Elizabeth II Health Sciences Centre, Halifax, Nova Scotia
MALGORZATA RAJDA
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia Sleep Clinic and Laboratory, Queen Elizabeth II Health Sciences Centre, Halifax, Nova Scotia
KATHLEEN F. SPURR
Affiliation:
School of Health Sciences, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia
*
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Esther Yuet Ying Lau, Add: K641, Knowles Building, Department of Psychology, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam Road, Hong Kong. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) is characterized by disrupted breathing and hypoxemia during sleep, daytime sleepiness, and changes in cognition and mood. One important question is regarding the reversibility of cognitive deficits after treatment with continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP). Here, we report the outcomes of CPAP treatment as measured by tests of attention and executive function. Thirty-seven individuals with moderate to severe OSA and compliant on CPAP treatment were studied with working memory tasks, neuropsychological testing, and overnight polysomnographic sleep study and compared to 27 healthy controls. CPAP improved the respiratory disturbance index, minimum and mean oxygen saturation (SpO2), subjective sleep quality, and daytime sleepiness ratings compared to pre-treatment values. In terms of current neurocognitive function, treated individuals with OSA performed at a comparable level to controls on basic working memory storage functions but still showed a significant reduction on tests of working memory requiring the central executive. The OSA group also performed worse on neuropsychological measures of complex attention, executive function, and psychomotor speed. While CPAP is an effective treatment for OSA in terms of ameliorating breathing disruption and oxygen desaturation during sleep, as well as daytime sleepiness, some cognitive deficits may be more resistant to treatment. (JINS, 2010, 16, 1077–1088.)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Alchanatis, M., Zias, N., Deligiorgis, N., Amfilochiou, A., Dionellis, G., & Orphanidou, D. (2005). Sleep apnea-related cognitive deficits and intelligence: An implication of cognitive reserve theory. Journal of Sleep Research, 14, 6975.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aloia, M.S., Arnedt, J.T., Davis, J.D., Riggs, R.L., & Byrd, D. (2004). Neuropsychological sequelae of obstructive sleep apnea-hypopnea syndrome: A critical review. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 772785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., Text Revision). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A.D. (1996a). Exploring the central executive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 528. doi: 10.1080/713755608CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A.D. (1996b). The fractionation of working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93, 1346813472. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.24.13468CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A.D. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 829839.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A.D., & Della Sala, S. (1996). Working memory and executive control. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 351, 13971404.Google ScholarPubMed
Barnes, M., Houston, D., Worsnop, C.J., Neill, A.M., Mykytyn, I.J., Kay, A., et al. . (2002). A randomized controlled trial of continuous positive airway pressure in mild obstructive sleep apnea. American Journal of Respiratory Critical Care Medicine, 165, 773780.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bédard, M.A., Montplaisir, J., Malo, J., Richer, F., & Rouleau, I. (1993). Persistent neuropsychological deficits and vigilance impairments in sleep apnea syndrome after treatment with continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP). Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 330341. doi: 10.1080/01688639308402567CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beebe, D.W., & Gozal, D. (2002). Obstructive sleep apnea and the prefrontal cortex: Towards a comprehensive model linking nocturnal upper airway obstruction to daytime cognitive and behavioral deficits. Journal of Sleep Research, 11, 116. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2002.00289.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonnet, M.H. (1993). Vigilance/sleepiness sequelae of sleep disordered breathing and sleep apnea: Cognitive effects of sleep and sleep fragmentation. Sleep, 16, S65S67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buysse, D.J., Reynolds, C.F. III, Monk, T.H., Berman, S.R., & Kupfer, D.J. (1989). The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Research, 28, 193213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coltheart, M. (1981a). MRC psycholinguistic database user manual: Version 1, from http://www.psych.rl.ac.uk/User_Manual_v1_0.htmlGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M. (1981b). The MRC psycholinguistic database. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33(A), 497505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P.A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Décary, A., Rouleau, I., & Montplaisir, J. (2000). Cognitive deficits associated with sleep apnea syndrome: A proposed neuropsychological test battery. Sleep, 23, 369381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Della Sala, S., Baddeley, A., Papagno, C., & Spinnler, H. (1995). Dual-task paradigm: A means to examine the central executive. In Grafman, J., Holyoak, K.J. & Boller, F. (Eds.), Structure and functions of the human frontal cortex (pp. 161171). New York: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Douglas, N.J. (1998). Systematic review of the efficacy of CPAP. Thorax, 53, 414415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duff, S.C., & Logie, R.H. (2001). Processing and storage in working memory span. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 3148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engleman, H.M., Kingshott, R.N., Martin, S.E., & Douglas, N.J. (2000). Cognitive function in the sleep apnea/hypopnea syndrome (SAHS). Sleep, 23, S102S108.Google ScholarPubMed
Engleman, H.M., Martin, S.E., Deary, I.J., & Douglas, N.J. (1994). Effect of continuous positive airway pressure treatment on daytime function in sleep apnoea/Hypopnea syndrome. Lancet, 343, 572575. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(94)91522-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engleman, H.M., Martin, S.E., Deary, I.J., & Douglas, N.J. (1997). Effect of CPAP therapy on daytime function in patients with mild sleep apnoea/hypopnoea syndrome. Thorax, 52, 114119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Felver-Grant, J.C., Bruce, A.S., Zimmerman, M., Sweet, L.H., Millman, R.P., & Aloia, M.S. (2007). Working memory in obstructive sleep apnea: Construct validity and treatment effects. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 3, 589594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferini-Strambi, L., Baietto, C., Di Giolia, M.R., Castaldi, P., Castronovo, C., Zucconi, M., et al. . (2003). Cognitive dysfunction in patients with obstructive sleep apnea (OSA): Partial reversibility after continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP). Brain Research Bulletin, 61, 8792. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(03)00068-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feuerstein, C., Naëgelé, B., Pepin, J.-L., & Levy, P. (1997). Frontal lobe-related cognitive functions in patients with sleep apnea syndrome before and after treatment. Acta Neurologica Beligica, 97, 96107.Google ScholarPubMed
Gevins, A.S., & Cutillo, B.A. (1993). Spatiotemporal dynamics of component processes in human working memory. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 87, 128143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gevins, A., Smith, M.E., Le, J., Leong, H., Bennett, J., Martin, N., et al. . (1996). High resolution evoked potential imaging of the cortical dynamics of human working memory. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 98, 327348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grunstein, R. (2005). Continuous positive airway pressure treatment for obstructive sleep apnea-hypopnea syndrome. In Kryger, M.H., Roth, T. & Dement, W.C. (Eds.), Principles and practice of sleep medicine (4th ed., pp. 10661080). Philadelphia: Elsevier Saunders.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartenbaum, N., Collop, N., Rosen, I.M., Phillips, B., George, C.F., Rowley, J.A., et al. . (2006). Sleep apnea and commercial motor vehicle operators: Statement from the joint task force of the American College of Chest Physicians, the American College of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, and the National Sleep Foundation. Chest, 130, 902905. doi: 10.1378/chest.130.3.902CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johns, M.W. (1991). A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: The Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep Research, 14, 540545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johns, M.W. (1992). Reliability and Factor Analysis of the Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep Research, 15, 376381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, K., & Harrison, Y. (2001). Frontal lobe function, sleep loss and fragmented sleep. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 5, 463475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, H.C., Young, T., Matthews, C.G., Weber, S.M., Woodard, A.R., & Palta, M. (1997). Sleep-disordered breathing and neuropsychological deficits. American Journal of Respiratory Critical Care Medicine, 156, 18131819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kingshott, R.N., Vennelle, M., Hoy, C.J., Engleman, H.M., Deary, I.J., & Douglas, N.J. (2000). Predictors of improvements in daytime function outcomes with CPAP therapy. American Journal of Respiratory Care Medicine, 161, 866871.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kushida, C.A., Littner, M., Morgenthaler, T., Alessi, C., Bailey, D., Coleman, J.J., et al. . (2005). Practice parameters for the indications for polysomnography and related procedures: An update for 2005. Sleep, 28, 299521.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, B., Robertson, I.H., Clare, L., Carter, G., Hong, J., Wilson, B.A., et al. . (2000). Rehabilitation of executive functioning: An experimental-clinical validation of goal management training. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 299312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lis, S., Krieger, S., Hennig, D., Christian, R., Kirsch, P., Seeger, W., et al. . (2008). Executive function and cognitive subprocesses in patients with obstructive sleep apnoea. Journal of Sleep Research, 17, 271. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00660.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montplaisir, J., Bédard, M.A., Richer, F., & Rouleau, I. (1992). Neurobehavioral manifestations in obstructive sleep apnea syndrome before and after treatment with continuous positive airway pressure. Sleep, 15(Suppl.), S17S19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naëgelé, B., Launois, S.H., Mazza, S., Feuerstein, C., Pépin, J.-L., & Lévy, P. (2006). Which memory processes are affected in patients with obstructive sleep apnea? An evaluation of 3 types of memory. Sleep, 29, 533544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naëgelé, B., Pepin, J.L., Levy, P., Bonnet, C., Pellat, J., & Feuerstein, C. (1998). Cognitive executive dysfunction in patients with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome (OSAS) after CPAP treatment. Sleep, 21, 392397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naëgelé, B., Thouvard, V., Pepin, J.L., Levy, P., Bonnet, C., Perret, J.E., et al. . (1995). Deficits of cognitive executive functions in patients with sleep apnea syndrome. Sleep, 18, 4352.Google ScholarPubMed
Patel, S.R., White, D.P., Malhotra, A., Stanchina, M.L., & Ayas, N.T. (2003). Continuous positive airway pressure therapy for treating sleepiness in a diverse population with obstructive sleep apnea. Archives of Internal Medicine, 163, 565571. doi: 10.1001/archinte.163.5.565CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rechtschaffen, A., & Kales, A. (1968). A manual of standardized terminology, techniques and scoring system for sleep stages of human subjects. Los Angeles: UCLA Brain Information Service/Brain Research Institute.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T.A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review 103, 403428. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.403CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sánchez, A.I., Buela-Casal, G., Bermúdez, M.P., & Casas-Maldonado, F. (2001). The effects of continuous positive air pressure treatment on anxiety and depression levels in apnea patients. Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 55, 641646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanner, B.M., Klewer, J., Trumm, A., Randerath, W., Kreuzer, I., & Zidek, W. (2000). Long-term treatment with continuous positive airway pressure improves quality of life in obstructive sleep apnoea syndrome. European Respiratory Journal, 16, 118122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sateia, M.J. (2003). Neuropsychological impairment and quality of life in obstructive sleep apnea. Clinical Chest Medicine, 24, 249259. doi: 10.1016/S0272-5231(03)00014-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saunamäki, T., & Jehkonen, M. (2007). A review of executive functions in obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 115, 111. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2006.00744.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, E.E., Jonides, J., & Koeppe, R.A. (1996). Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 1120. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.1.11CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, E.E., Jonides, J., Koeppe, R.A., Awh, E., Schumacher, E.H., & Minoshima, S. (1995). Spatial versus object working memory: PET investigations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 337356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sternberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human memory. Science, 153, 652654. doi: 10.1126/science.153.3736.652CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sternberg, S. (1969). Memory-scanning: Mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. American Scientist, 57, 421457.Google ScholarPubMed
Sturm, W., Willmes, K., Orgass, B., & Hartje, W. (1997). Do specific attention deficits need specific training? Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 7, 81103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, R.J., Rosen, B.R., Stern, C.E., Weiss, W., & Kwong, K.K. (2005). Functional imaging of working memory in obstructive sleep-disordered breathing. Journal of Applied Physiology, 98, 22262234. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.01225.2004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verstraeten, E., Cluydts, R., Pevernagie, D., & Hoffmann, G. (2004). Executive function in sleep apnea: Controlling for attentional capacity in assessing executive attention. Sleep, 27, 685693.Google ScholarPubMed
Weaver, T.E. (2001). Outcome measurement in sleep medicine practice and research. Part 2: Assessment of neurobehavioral performance and mood. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 5, 223236. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2001.0153CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler memory scale-revised manual. New York: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wilson, M.D. (1988). The MRC psycholinguistic database: Machine readable dictionary, Version 2. Behavioural Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 20, 611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar