Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T22:19:59.794Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Evaluation of the performance of normal elderly in a limb praxis protocol: Influence of age, gender, and education

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2009

KARLA RODRIGUES CAVALCANTE
Affiliation:
Department of Physical Therapy, University of Taubaté, Taubaté (SP), Brazil Department of Neurology, University of São Paulo School of Medicine, São Paulo (SP), Brazil
PAULO CARAMELLI*
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, University of São Paulo School of Medicine, São Paulo (SP), Brazil Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology Unit, Department of Internal Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, Federal University of Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte (MG), Brazil
*
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Paulo Caramelli, Department of Internal Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, Federal University of Minas Gerais, Avenue Alfredo Balena, 190—Room 246, Belo Horizonte (MG) 30.130-000, Brazil. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Limb praxis can be influenced by age, gender, and education. The present study investigated the influence of these variables on gesture production by healthy elderly subjects. We evaluated 96 individuals divided into two age groups (60–74 and 75–88 years). Each group contained 48 men and 48 women and was subdivided into four groups according to education: illiterates and 1–3, 4–7, and 8 or more years of education. Individuals were requested to carry out tasks on verbal command and imitation. There were no differences between the performance of men and women, while older individuals performed worse than their younger counterparts. Regarding educational level, three major groups emerged: illiterates, individuals with 1–7 years of education, and those with 8 or more years of education. In conclusion, age and education significantly influenced the performance of individuals in limb praxis tests. (JINS, 2009, 15, 618–622.)

Type
Brief Communications
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Brucki, S.M., Nitrini, R., Caramelli, P., Bertolucci, P.H., & Okamoto, I.H. (2003). Suggestions for utilization of the Mini-Mental State Examination in Brazil. Arquivos de Neuropsiquiatria, 61, 777–781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caramelli, P., Carthery-Goulart, M.T., Porto, C.S., Charchat-Fichman, H., & Nitrini, R. (2007). Category fluency as a screening test for Alzheimer’s disease in illiterate and literate patients. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 21, 65–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castro-Caldas, A., Petersson, K.M., Reis, A., Stone-Elander, S., & Ingvar, M. (1998). The illiterate brain: Learning to read and write during childhood influences the functional organization of the adult brain. Brain, 121, 1053–1063.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chipman, K. & Hampson, E. (2006). A female advantage in the serial production of non-representational learned gestures. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2315–2329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dobigny-Roman, N., Dieudonne-Moinet, B., Tortrat, D., Verny, M., & Forette, B. (1998). Ideomotor apraxia test: A new test of imitation of gestures for elderly people. European Journal of Neurology, 5, 571–578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dumont, C., Ska, B., & Joanette, Y. (2000). Conceptual apraxia and semantic memory deficit in Alzheimer’s disease: Two sides of the same coin? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 693–703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldenberg, G. (1995). Imitating gestures and manipulating a manikin—The representation of the human body in ideomotor apraxia. Neuropsychologia, 33, 63–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heath, M., Roy, E.A., Westwood, D., & Black, S.E. (2001). Patterns of apraxia associated with the production of intransitive limb gestures following left and right hemisphere stroke. Brain and Cognition, 46(1–2), 165–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manly, J.J., Jacobs, D.M., Sano, M., Bell, K., Merchant, C.A., Small, S.A., & Stern, Y. (1999). Effect of literacy on neuropsychological test performance in nondemented, education-matched elders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 5, 191–202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nitrini, R., Caramelli, P., Herrera, E. Jr, Charchat-Fichman, H., & Porto, C.S. (2005). Performance in Lurias fist-edge-palm test according to educational level. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 18, 211–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nitrini, R., Lefèvre, B.H., Mathias, S.C., Caramelli, P., Carrilho, P.E.M., Sauaia, N., Massad, E., Takiguti, C., Silva, I.O., Porto, C.S., Magila, M.C., & Scaff, M. (1994). Neuropsychological tests of simple application for dementia diagnosis. Arquivos de Neuropsiquiatria, 52, 457–465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nitrini, R., Caramelli, P., Herrera, E. Jr, Porto, C.S., Charchat-Fichman, H., Carthery, M.T., Takada, L.T., & Lima, E.P. (2004). Performance of illiterate and literate nondemented elderly subjects in two tests of long-term memory. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 634–638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peigneux, P. & van der Linden, M. (1999). Influence of ageing and educational level on the prevalence of body-part-as-objects in normal subjects. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, 522–547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peigneux, P., Van der Linden, M., Garraux, G., Laureys, S., Degueldre, C., Aerts, J., Del Fiore, G., Moonen, G., Luxen, A., & Salmon, E. (2004). Imaging a cognitive model of apraxia: The neural substrate of gesture specific cognitive processes. Human Brain Mapping, 21, 119–142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pereira, J.R., Reis, A.M., & Magalhães, Z. (2003). Neuroanatomia funcional. Anatomia das áreas ativáveis nos usuais paradigmas em ressonância magnética funcional. Acta Médica Portuguesa, 16, 107–116.Google Scholar
Pharr, V., Uttl, B., Stark, M., Litvan, I., Fantie, B., & Grafman, J. (2001). Comparison of apraxia in corticobasal degeneration and progressive supranuclear palsy. Neurology, 56, 957–963.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothi, L.J., Ochipa, C., & Heilman, K.M. (1991). A cognitive neuropsychological model of limb praxis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 443–458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ska, B., Caramelli, P., Croisile, B., Fontaine, F.S., Gilbert, B., & Pineau, H. (1997) Protocole d’Évaluation de la Production des Gestes. Montreal, QC, Canada: Laboratoire Th.-Alajouanine, Centre de Recherche de l’ Institut de Gériatrie de Montreal.Google Scholar
Ska, B. & Nespoulous, J.L. (1987). Pantomimes and aging. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 9, 754–766.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiederholt, W.C., Cahn, D., Butters, N.M., Salmon, D.P., Kritz-Silverstein, D., & Barrett-Connor, E. (1993). Effects of age, gender and education on selected neuropsycological test in an elderly community cohort. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 41, 639–647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar