Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T04:17:01.017Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Environment and object mental images in patients with representational neglect: Two case reports

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 March 2010

LIANA PALERMO*
Affiliation:
Dipartimento di Psicologia-39, “Sapienza” Università di Roma, Rome, Italy Centro Ricerche di Neuropsicologia, I.R.C.C.S. Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy
RAFFAELLA NORI
Affiliation:
Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università degli Studi di Bologna, Bologna, Italy
LAURA PICCARDI
Affiliation:
Centro Ricerche di Neuropsicologia, I.R.C.C.S. Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy Dipartimento di Scienze della Salute, Università degli Studi dell’Aquila, L’Aquila, Italy
FIORELLA GIUSBERTI
Affiliation:
Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università degli Studi di Bologna, Bologna, Italy
CECILIA GUARIGLIA
Affiliation:
Dipartimento di Psicologia-39, “Sapienza” Università di Roma, Rome, Italy Centro Ricerche di Neuropsicologia, I.R.C.C.S. Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy
*
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Liana Palermo, Dipartimento di Psicologia 39, Facoltà Psicologia 1, Sapienza Università di Roma, Via dei Marsi 78, 00185 Italy. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The aim of this study was to shed light on the nature of the imagery deficits in two patients with representational neglect and to determine whether representational neglect is affected by the content of the mental images the patients have to generate, inspect and manipulate. In particular, we submitted two patients with different types of representational neglect to a battery of visual mental imagery tests to assess the different kinds of imagery processes. We found that Patient 1, whose performance was asymmetrical on the O’Clock Test, performed poorly on tasks involving the mental generation, inspection and manipulation of objects but showed no deficit on tasks involving the mental generation, inspection and manipulation of environments. On the other hand, Patient 2, whose performance was asymmetrical on the Familiar Squares Description Test, performed poorly on tasks involving the mental generation, inspection and manipulation of environments, but not on tasks involving the mental generation, inspection and manipulation of objects. Our results demonstrate that environments and objects in the imagery domain can be represented separately and can be selectively affected by damage following brain lesions. (JINS, 2010, 16, 921–932.)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Aguirre, G.K., Zarahn, E., & D’Esposito, M. (1998). An area within human ventral cortex sensitive to “building” stimuli: Evidence and implications. Neuron, 21, 373383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartolomeo, P., D’Erme, P., & Gainotti, G. (1994). The relationship between visuospatial and representational neglect. Neurology, 44, 17101714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beschin, N., Cocchini, G., Della Sala, S., & Logie, R.H. (1997). What the eyes perceive, the brain ignores: A case of pure unilateral representational neglect, Cortex, 33, 326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bisiach, E., & Luzzatti, C. (1978). Unilateral neglect of representational space. Cortex, 14, 129133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlesimo, G.A., Buccione, I., Fadda, L., Graceffa, A., Mauri, M., Lorusso, S., et al. . (2002). Standardizzazione di due test di memoria per uso clinico: Breve Racconto e Figura di Rey. Nuova Rivista di Neurologia, 12, 13.Google Scholar
Carlesimo, G.A., Caltagirone, C., & Gainotti, G. (1996). The mental deterioration battery: Normative data, diagnostic reliability and qualitative analyses of cognitive impairment. The Group for the Standardization of the Mental Deterioration Battery. European Neurology, 36, 378384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corsi, P.M. (1972). Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain, Dissertation Abstracts International, 34(02), 891B, (University micro-lmsNo. AA105-77717).Google Scholar
Crawford, J.R., Garthwaite, P.H., Azzalini, A., Howell, D.C., & Laws, K.R. (2006). Testing for a deficit in single-case studies: Effects of departures from normality. Neuropsychologia, 44, 666677.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, J.R., & Garthwaite, P.H. (2002). Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: Confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences. Neuropsychologia, 40, 11961208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, J.R., & Howell, D.C. (1998). Comparing an individual’s test score against norms derived from small samples. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12, 482486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Della Sala, S., Logie, R.H., Beschin, N., & Denis, M. (2004). Preserved visuo-spatial transformations in representational neglect. Neuropsychologia, 42, 13581364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denis, M., Beschin, N., Logie, R.H., & Della Sala, S. (2002). Visual perception and verbal descriptions as source for generating mental representations: Evidence from representational neglect. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 19, 97112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, R., & Kanwisher, N. (1998). A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature, 392, 598601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, R.A., Parker, W.E., & Feiler, A.M. (2007). Where am I now? Distinct roles for parahippocampal and retrosplenial cortices in place recognition. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 61416149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farah, M.J., Hammond, K.M., Levine, D.N., & Calvanio, R. (1988). Visual and spatial mental imagery: Dissociable system of representation. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 439462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, J.W., Ekstrom, R.B., & Price, L. (1963). Manual for kit of reference tests for cognitive factors. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.Google Scholar
Grossi, D., Modaferri, A., Pelosi, L., & Trojano, L. (1989). On the different roles of the cerebral hemispheres in mental imagery: The “ O’ Clock Test” in two clinical cases. Brain and Cognition, 10, 1827.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guariglia, C., Padovani, A., Pantano, P., & Pizzamiglio, L. (1993). Unilateral neglect restricted to visual imagery. Nature, 364, 235237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guariglia, C., Piccardi, L., Iaria, G., Nico, D., & Pizzamiglio, L. (2005). Representational neglect and navigation in real space. Neuropsychologia, 43, 11381143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guariglia, C., & Pizzamiglio, L. (2006). Spatial navigation-cognitive and neuropsychological aspects. In Vecchi, T. & Bottini, G. (Eds.), Imagery and spatial cognition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Guariglia, C., & Pizzamiglio, L. (2007). The role of imagery in navigation: Neuropsychological evidence. In Mast, F. & Jäncke, L. (Eds.), Spatial processing in navigation, imagery and perception. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Haxby, J.V., Parasuraman, R., Lalonde, F., & Abboud, H. (1993). SuperLab: General purpose Macintosh software for human experimental psychology and psychological testing. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 25, 400405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hegarty, M., Montello, D.R., Richardson, A.E., Ishikawa, T., & Lovelace, K. (2006). Spatial abilities at different scales: Individual differences in aptitude-test performance and spatial-layout learning. Intelligence, 34, 151176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hillis, A.E., Newhart, M., Heidler, J., Barker, P.B., Herskovits, E.H., & Degaonkar, M. (2005). Anatomy of spatial attention: insights from perfusion imaging and hemispatial neglect in acute stroke. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 31613167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ishai, A., Ungerleider, L.G., & Haxby, J.V. (2000). Distributed neural systems for the generation of visual images. Neuron, 28, 979990.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janzen, G., & van Turennout, M. (2004). Selective neural representation of objects relevant for navigation. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 673677.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kosslyn, S.M. (1980). Image and mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S.M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural foundations of imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 635642.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kreiman, G., Koch, C., & Fried, I. (2000). Imagery neurons in the human brain. Nature, 16, 357361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Logie, R.H., Della Sala, S., Beschin, N., & Denis, M. (2005). Dissociating mental transformations and visuo-spatial storage in working memory: Evidence from representational neglect. Memory, 13, 430434.Google ScholarPubMed
Luzzatti, C., Vecchi, T., Agazzi, D., Cesa-Bianchi, M., & Vergani, C. (1998). A neurological dissociation between preserved visual and impaired spatial processing in mental imagery. Cortex, 34, 461469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Massironi, M., Antonucci, G., Pizzamiglio, L., Vitale, M.V., & Zoccolotti, P. (1988). The Wundt-Jastrow illusion in the study of spatial hemi-neglect, Neuropsychologia, 26, 161166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nico, D., Piccardi, L., Iaria, G., Bianchini, F., Zompanti, L., & Guariglia, C. (2008). Landmark based navigation in brain-damaged patients with neglect. Neuropsychologia, 46, 18981907.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nori, R., & Giusberti, F. (2006). Predicting cognitive styles from spatial abilities. American Journal of Psychology, 119, 6786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Novelli, G., Papagno, C., Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., Cappa, S.F., & Vallar, G. (1986). Tre test clinici di ricerca e produzione lessicale: taratura su soggetti normali. Archivio di Psicologica, Neurologia e Psichiatria, 47, 477506.Google Scholar
O’Craven, K.M., & Kanwisher, N. (2000). Mental imagery of faces and places activates corresponding stimulus-specific brain regions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 10131023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orsini, A., Grossi, D., Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1987). Verbal and spatial immediate memory span: Normative data from 1355 adults and 1112 children. Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 8, 539548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ortigue, S., Viaud-Delmon, I., Michel, C., Blanke, O., Annoni, J.M., Pegna, A., et al. . (2003). Pure imagery hemi-neglect of far space. Neurology, 60, 20002002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palermo, L., Piccardi, L., Nori, R., Giusberti, F., & Guariglia, C. (submitted). Is hemi-neglect involved in visual mental imagery? Deficits following lesions of the right and left hemisphere.Google Scholar
Piccardi, L., Bianchini, F., Zompanti, L., & Guariglia, C. (2008). Pure representational neglect and navigational deficits in a case with preserved visuo-spatial working memory. Neurocase, 14, 329342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pizzamiglio, L., Judica, A., Razzano, C., & Zoccolotti, P. (1989). Toward a comprehensive diagnosis of visual spatial disorders in unilateral brain damaged patients. Psychological Assessment, 5, 199218.Google Scholar
Rey, A. (1941). L’examen psychologique dans les cas d’encephalopathie traumatique. Archives de Psychologie, 28, 286340.Google Scholar
Rey, A. (1958). Memorisation d’ une serie de 15 mots en 5 repetitions. In Rey, A. (Ed.), L’ examen clinique en psycologie. Paris: Presses Universitaires des France.Google Scholar
Riddoch, M.J. (1990). Loss of visual imagery: A generation deficit. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 7, 249276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sassi, G. (1986 ). Prova la tua intelligenza giocando, 7°. Bergamo: BUR.Google Scholar
Sokal, R.R., & Rohlf, F.J. (1995). Biometry (3rd ed.). San Francisco, CA: W.H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Spinnler, H., & Tognoni, G. (1987). Standardizzazione e taratura italiana di test neuropsicologici. Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 8(Suppl.), 1120.Google Scholar
Street, R.F. (1931). A Gestalt completion test. Teachers College Record, 33, 280282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thurstone, L.L. (1937). Psychological tests for the study of mental abilities. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Tversky, B. (2003). Structures of mental spaces. How people think about space. Environment and Behavior, 35, 6680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vincenzi, G. (2000). Le immagini mentali: caratteristiche strutturali. Unpublished dissertation. University of Bologna, Italy.Google Scholar
Zimmermann, P., & Fimm, B. (1992). Testbatterie zur aufmerksamkeitsprüfung (TAP). Würselen: Psytest.Google Scholar