Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T04:33:47.531Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive Outcomes for Extremely Preterm/Extremely Low Birth Weight Children in Kindergarten

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 September 2011

Leah J. Orchinik
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio
H. Gerry Taylor*
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, Case Western Reserve University, Rainbow Babies and Children's Hospital, Cleveland, Ohio
Kimberly Andrews Espy
Affiliation:
Developmental Neuroscience Laboratory, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, Nebraska
Nori Minich
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, Case Western Reserve University, Rainbow Babies and Children's Hospital, Cleveland, Ohio
Nancy Klein
Affiliation:
Department of Education, Cleveland State University, Cleveland, Ohio
Tiffany Sheffield
Affiliation:
Developmental Neuroscience Laboratory, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, Nebraska
Maureen Hack
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, Case Western Reserve University, Rainbow Babies and Children's Hospital, Cleveland, Ohio
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: H. Gerry Taylor, W.O. Walker Building, Suite 3150, 10524 Euclid Ave., Cleveland, OH 44106. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Our objectives were to examine cognitive outcomes for extremely preterm/extremely low birth weight (EPT/ELBW, gestational age <28 weeks and/or birth weight <1000 g) children in kindergarten and the associations of these outcomes with neonatal factors, early childhood neurodevelopmental impairment, and socioeconomic status (SES). The sample comprised a hospital-based 2001–2003 birth cohort of 148 EPT/ELBW children (mean birth weight 818 g; mean gestational age 26 weeks) and a comparison group of 111 term-born normal birth weight (NBW) classmate controls. Controlling for background factors, the EPT/ELBW group had pervasive deficits relative to the NBW group on a comprehensive test battery, with rates of cognitive deficits that were 3 to 6 times higher in the EPT/ELBW group. Deficits on a measure of response inhibition were found in 48% versus 10%, odds ratio (95% confidence interval) = 7.32 (3.32, 16.16), p < .001. Deficits on measures of executive function and motor and perceptual-motor abilities were found even when controlling for acquired verbal knowledge. Neonatal risk factors, early neurodevelopmental impairment, and lower SES were associated with higher rates of deficits within the EPT/ELBW group. The findings document both global and selective cognitive deficits in EPT/ELBW children at school entry and justify efforts at early identification and intervention. (JINS, 2011, 17, 1067–1079)

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aarnoudse-Moens, C.S., Smidts, D.P., Oosterlaan, J., Duivenvoorden, H.J., Weisglas-Kuperus, N. (2009). Executive function in very preterm children at early school age. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 981993.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, P.J., De Luca, C.R., Hutchinson, E., Spencer-Smith, M.M., Roberts, G., Doyle, L.W. (2011). Attention problems in a representative sample of extremely preterm/extremely low birth weight children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36, 5773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, P.J., Doyle, L.W., & the Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group. (2003). Neurobehavioral outcomes of school-age children born extremely low birth weight or very preterm in the 1990s. Journal of the American Medical Association, 289, 32643272.Google ScholarPubMed
Avchen, R.N., Scott, K.G., Mason, C.A. (2001). Birth weight and school-age disabilities: A population-based study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 154, 895901.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aylward, G.P. (1992). The relationship between environmental risk and developmental outcome. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 13, 222229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron, I.S., Erickson, K., Ahronovich, M.D., Baker, R., Litman, F.R. (2011). Neuropsychological and behavioral outcomes of extremely low birth weight at age three. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36, 521.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron, I.S., Erickson, K., Ahronovich, M.D., Litman, F.R., Brandt, J. (2010). Spatial location memory discriminates children born at extremely low birth weight and late-preterm at age three. Neuropsychology, 24, 787794. doi:10.1037/a0020382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayless, S., Stevenson, J. (2007). Executive functions in school-age children born very prematurely. Early Human Development, 83, 247254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley scales of infant development (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Beery, K.E., Beery, N.A. (2004). The Beery-Buktenica development test of visual-motor integration: Beery VMI, administration, scoring, and teaching manual (5th ed.). Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., Mackinnon, C. (1994). Transition to school: Developmental trajectories and school experiences. Early Education and Development, 5, 106119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohm, B., Katz-Salamon, M. (2003). Cognitive development at 5.5 years of children with chronic lung disease of prematurity. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 88, F101F105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bohm, B., Katz-Salamon, M., Smedler, A.C., Forssberg, H. (2002). Developmental risks and protective factors for influencing cognitive outcome at 5 years of age in very-low-birthweight children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 44(8), 508516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bohm, B., Smedler, A-C., Forssberg, H. (2004). Impulse control, working memory and other executive functions in preterm children when starting school. Acta Paediatrica, 93, 13631371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruininks, R.H., Bruininks, B.D. (2005). BOT-2: Bruininks-Oseretsky test of motor proficiency (2nd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Bull, R., Espy, K.A., Wiebe, S.A. (2008). Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33, 205228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cahan, S., Cohen, N. (1989). Age versus schooling effects on intelligence development. Child Development, 60(5), 12391249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B.J., Galvan, A., Hare, T.A. (2005). Changes in cerebral functional organization during cognitive development. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 239244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, C.A., Pritchard, V.E., Woodward, L.J. (2010). Preschool executive functioning abilities predict early mathematics achievement. Developmental Psychology, 46, 11761191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dewey, D., Crawford, S.G., Creighton, D.E., Sauve, R.S. (1999). Long-term neuropsychological outcomes in very low birth weight children free of sensorineural impairments. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, 851865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dewey, D., Creighton, D.E., Heath, J.A., Wilson, B.N., Anseeuw-Deeks, D., Crawford, S.G. (2011). Assessment of developmental coordination disorder in children born with extremely low birth weights. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16, 4256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Espy, K.A., Bull, R., Martin, J., Stroup, W. (2006). Measuring the development of executive control with the shape school. Psychological Assessment, 18, 373381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Espy, K., Cwik, M. (2004). The development of a trail making test in young children: The TRAILS-P. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 18, 411422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Espy, K.A., Fang, H., Charak, D., Minich, N., Taylor, H.G. (2009). Growth mixture modeling of academic achievement in children of varying birth weight risk. Neuropsychology, 23, 460474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fanaroff, A.A., Stoll, B.J., Wright, L.L., Carlo, W.A., Ehrenkranz, R.A., Stark, A.R., Poole, W.K.; NICHD Neonatal Research Network. (2007). Trends in neonatal morbidity and mortality for very low birthweight infants. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 196, 147.e1147.e8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fay, T.B., Yeates, K.O., Wade, S.L., Drotar, D., Stancin, T., Taylor, H.G. (2009). Predicting longitudinal patterns of functional deficits in children with traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 23, 271282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Federal Financial Institutions Examinations Council Geocoding System. Retrieved from http://www.ffiec.gov/Geocode/default.htm. Accessed January 1, 2010–November 1, 2010.Google Scholar
Foster-Cohen, S.H., Friesen, M.D., Champion, P.R., Woodward, L.J. (2010). High prevalence/low severity language delay in preschool children born very preterm. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 31, 658667.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gidley Larson, G., Baron, I.S., Erickson, K., Ahronovich, M.D., Baker, R., Litman, F.R. (2011). Neuromotor outcomes at school age after extremely low birth weight: Early detection of subtle signs. Neuropsychology, 25, 6675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonzalez, L.M., Anderson, V.A., Wood, S.J., Mitchell, A., Harvey, A.S. (2007). The localization and lateralization of memory deficits in children with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia, 48, 124132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, R.M., Warren, J.R. (1997). Socioeconomic indexes for occupation: A review, update, and critique. Sociological Methodology, 27, 177298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, C., Dunn, J., White, A. (1998). Trick or treat?: Uneven understanding of mind and emotion and executive dysfunction in “Hard-to-manage” preschoolers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39, 981994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, S., Hennessy, E., Smith, R., Trikic, R., Wolke, D., Marlow, N. (2009). Academic attainment and special educational needs in extremely preterm children at 11 years of age: The EPICure study. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 94, F283F289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, J.S., Dole, N., Savitz, D.A., Herring, A.H. (2003). Modeling community-level effects on preterm birth. Annals of Epidemiology, 13, 377384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kulseng, S., Jennekens-Schinkel, A., Naess, P., Romundstad, P., Indredavik, M., Vik, T., Brubakk, A-M. (2006). Very-low-birthweight and term small-for-gestational-age adolescents: Attention revisited. Acta Paediatrica, 95, 224230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larroque, B., Ancel, P-Y., Marret, S., Marchand, L., Andre, M., Arnaud, C., Kaminski, M.; for the EPIPAGE Study group. (2008). Neurodevelopmental disabilities and special care of 5-year-old children born before 33 weeks of gestation (the EPIPAGE study): A longitudinal cohort study. The Lancet, 371, 813820.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawrence, E.J., Rubia, K., Murray, R.M., McGuire, P.K., Walshe, M., Allin, M., Nosarti, C. (2009). The neural basis of response inhibition and attention allocation as mediated by gestational age. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 10381050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, C.H., Clyman, R.I., Piecuch, R.E., Juster, R.P., Ballard, R.A., Behle, M.B. (1990). Effect of medical and social risk factors on outcome of prematurity and very low birth weight. Journal of Pediatrics, 116, 620626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lind, A., Korkman, M., Lehtonen, L., Lapinleimu, H., Parkkola, R., Matomake, J., Haataja, L. (2011). Cognitive and neuropsychological outcomes at 5 years of age in preterm children born in the 2000s. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 53, 256262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Litt, J., Taylor, H.G., Klein, N., Hack, M. (2005). Learning disabilities in children with very low birth weight: Prevalence, neuropsychological correlates, and educational interventions. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38, 130141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luoma, L., Herrgard, E., Martikainen, A., Ahonen, T. (1998). Speech and language development of children born at ≤32 weeks’ gestation: A 5-year prospective follow-up study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 40, 380387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marlow, N. (2004). Neurocognitive outcome after preterm birth. Archives of Disease in Childhood: Fetal and Neonatal Edition. 89, F224F228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marlow, N., Hennessy, E.M., Bracewell, M.A., Wolke, D. (2007). Motor and executive function at 6 years of age after extremely preterm birth. Pediatrics, 120, 793804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mather, N., Jaffe, L.E. (2002). Woodcock-Johnson III: Reports, guidelines and recommendations. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ment, L., Vohr, B., Allan, W., Katz, K., Schneider, K., Westerveld, M., Makuch, R. (2003). Change in cognitive function over time in very low-birth-weight infants. Journal of the American Medical Association, 289, 705711.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikkola, K., Ritari, N., Tommiska, V., Salokorpi, T., Lehtonen, L., Tammela, O., Fellman, V. (2005). Neurodevelopmental outcome at 5 years of age of a national cohort of extremely low birth weight infants who were born in 1996–1997. Pediatrics, 116, 13911400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulder, H., Pitchford, N.J., Marlow, N. (2010). Processing speed and working memory underlie academic attainment in very preterm children. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 95, F267F272. doi:10.1136/adc.209.167965.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narberhaus, A., Segarra, D., Caldu, X., Gimenez, M., Pueyo, R., Botet, F., Junque, C. (2008). Corpus callosum and prefrontal functions in adolescents with history of very preterm birth. Neuropsychologia, 46, 111116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nosarti, C., Giouroukou, E., Healy, E., Rifkin, L., Walshe, M., Reichenberg, A., Murray, R.M. (2008). Grey and white matter distribution in very preterm adolescents mediates neurodevelopmental outcome. Brain, 131, 205217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nosarti, C., Rubia, K., Smith, A.B., Frearson, S., Williams, S.C., Rifkin, L., Murray, R.M. (2006). Altered functional neuroanatomy of response inhibition in adolescent males who were born very preterm. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 48, 265271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patrianakos-Hoobler, A., Msall, M.E., Huo, D., Marks, J.D., Plesha-Troyke, S., Schreiber, M.D. (2009). Predicting school readiness from neurodevelopmental assessments at age 2 years after respiratory distress syndrome in infants born preterm. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 52, 379385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patrianakos-Hoobler, A., Msall, M.E., Marks, J.D., Huo, D., Schreiber, M.D. (2009). Risk factors affecting school readiness in premature infant with respiratory distress syndrome. Pediatrics, 124, 258267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pritchard, V.E., Clark, C.A., Liberty, K., Champion, P.R., Wilson, K., Woodward, L.J. (2009). Early school-based learning difficulties in children born very preterm. Early Human Development, 85, 215224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pritchard, V.E., Woodward, L.J. (2011). Preschool executive control on the Shape School task: Measurement considerations and utility. Psychological Assessment, 23, 3143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rimm-Kaufmann, S.E., Pianta, R.C., Cox, M.J. (2000). Teachers’ judgments of problems in the transition to kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 15, 147166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, G., Anderson, P.J., Doyle, L.W.; for the Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group. (2010). The stability of the diagnosis of developmental disability between ages 2 and 9 in a geographic cohort of very preterm children born in 1997. Archives of Disease in Children, 95, 786790.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, B., Asztalos, E.V., Roberts, R.S., Robertson, C.M., Sauve, R.S., Whitfield, M.F. (2003). Impact of bronchopulmonary dysplasia, brain injury, and severe retinopathy on the outcome of extremely low-birth-weight infants at 18 months: Results from the trial of indomethacin prophylaxis in preterms. Journal of the American Medical Association, 289, 11241129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simpson, A., Riggs, K.J. (2006). Conditions under which children experience inhibitory difficulty with a “button-press” Go/No-go task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94, 1826.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stjernqvist, K., Svenningsen, N.W. (1999). Ten-year follow-up of children born before 29 gestational weeks: Health, cognitive development, behaviour and school achievement. Acta Paediatrica, 88, 557562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoll, B.J., Hansen, N.I., Bell, E.F., Shankaran, S., Laptook, A.R., Walsh, M.C., Higgins, R.D. (2010). Neonatal outcomes of extremely preterm infants from the NICHD Neonatal Research Network. Pediatrics, 126, 443456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, M.C., McGrath, M.M. (2003). Perinatal morbidity, mild motor delay, and later school outcomes. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 45, 104112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, H.G. (2010). Academic performance and learning disabilities. In C. Nosarti, R.M. Murray, & M. Hack (Eds.), Neurodevelopmental outcomes of preterm birth: From childhood to adult life (pp. 195218). Cambridge: University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, H.G., Filipek, P.A., Juranek, J., Bangert, B., Minich, N., Hack, M. (2011). Brain volumes in adolescents with very low birth weight: Effects on brain structure and associations with neuropsychological outcomes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36, 96117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, H.G., Klein, N., Anselmo, M.G., Espy, K.A., Minich, N., Hack, M. (in press). Learning problems in kindergarten children with extremely preterm birth. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine.Google Scholar
Taylor, H.G., Klein, N., Drotar, D., Schluchter, M., Hack, M. (2006). Consequences and risks of <1000-g birth weight for neuropsychological skills, achievement, and adaptive functioning. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27, 459469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, H.G., Minich, N., Bangert, B., Filipek, P.A., Hack, M. (2004). Long-term neuropsychological outcomes of very low birth weight: Associations with early risks for periventricular brain insults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 9871004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volpe, J.J. (2009). Brain injury in premature infants: A complex amalgam of destructive and developmental disturbances. Lancet Neurology, 8, 110124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, R.K., Torgesen, C.A., Rashotte, C. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Austin, TX: Pro-ed.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1987). Manual for the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wiebe, S.A., Sheffield, T., Nelson, J.M., Clark, C.A., Chevalier, N., Espy, K.A. (2011). The structure of executive function in 3-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 436452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson-Costello, D., Friedman, H., Minich, N., Siner, B., Taylor, G., Schluchter, M., Hack, M. (2007). Improved neurodevelopmental outcomes for extremely low birth weight infants in 2000–2002. Pediatrics, 110, 3745.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolke, D., Samara, M., Bracewell, M., Marlow, N.; for the EPICure Study Group. (2008). Specific language difficulties and school achievement in children born at 25 weeks of gestation or less. Journal of Pediatrics, 152, 256262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodcock, R.C., McGrew, K.S., Mather, S. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of cognitive abilities. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.Google Scholar
Woodward, L.J., Clark, C.A., Pritchard, V.E., Anderson, P.J., Inder, T.E. (2011). Neonatal white matter abnormalities predict global executive function impairment in children born very preterm. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 2241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodward, L.J., Moor, S., Hood, K.M., Champion, P.R., Foster-Cohen, S., Inder, T.E., Austin, N.C. (2009). Very preterm children show impairments across multiple neurodevelopmental domains by age 4 years. Archives of Disease in Childhood: Fetal and Neonatal Edition, 94, F339F344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yudkin, P.L., Aboualfa, M., Eyre, J.A., Redman, C.W., Wilkinson, A.R. (1987). New birthweight and head circumference centiles for gestational ages 24 to 42 weeks. Early Human Development, 15, 4552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed