Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T03:35:12.802Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Associations of environmental and lifestyle factors with spatial navigation in younger and older adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 August 2022

Hannah Maybrier
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, Missouri 63130, USA
Ben Julian A. Palanca
Affiliation:
Center on Biological Rhythms and Sleep, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, Missouri 63130, USA Department of Anesthesiology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, Missouri 63130, USA Department of Psychiatry, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, Missouri 63130, USA Department of Biomedical Engineering, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, Missouri 63130, USA
Denise Head*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, Missouri 63130, USA Department of Radiology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, Missouri 63130, USA
*
Corresponding author: Denise Head, email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Advanced age is associated with prominent impairment in allocentric navigation dependent on the hippocampus. This study examined whether age-related impairment in allocentric navigation and strategy selection was associated with sleep disruption or circadian rest-activity fragmentation. Further, we examined whether associations with navigation were moderated by perceived stress and physical activity.

Method:

Sleep fragmentation and total sleep time over the course of 1 week were assayed in younger (n = 42) and older (n = 37) adults via wrist actigraphy. Subsequently, participants completed cognitive mapping and route learning tasks, as well a measure of spontaneous navigation strategy selection. Measurements of perceived stress and an actigraphy-based index of physical activity were also obtained. Circadian rest-activity fragmentation was estimated via actigraphy post-hoc.

Results:

Age was associated with reduced cognitive mapping, route learning, allocentric strategy use, and total sleep time (ps < .01), replicating prior findings. Novel findings included that sleep fragmentation increased with advancing age (p = .009) and was associated with lower cognitive mapping (p = .022) within the older adult cohort. Total sleep time was not linearly associated with the navigation tasks (ps > .087). Post-hoc analyses revealed that circadian rest-activity fragmentation increased with advancing age within the older adults (p = .026) and was associated with lower cognitive mapping across the lifespan (p = .001) and within older adults (p = .005). Neither stress nor physical activity were robust moderators of sleep fragmentation associations with the navigation tasks (ps > .113).

Conclusion:

Sleep fragmentation and circadian rest-activity fragmentation are potential contributing factors to age effects on cognitive mapping within older adults.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2022

Introduction

Existing research has focused on characterizing age-related differences in spatial navigation, with evidence of particular deficits in processes dependent upon the hippocampus (Lester et al., Reference Lester, Moffat, Wiener, Barnes and Wolbers2017). However, it is important to also consider factors that may contribute to the deficits, particularly ones that may be modifiable. While several factors may contribute to hippocampal aging and related impairments in spatial navigation, the role of age-related disruption in sleep quality remains unclear. Thus, the current study focused on understanding the role of sleep disturbance in age effects on spatial navigation.

The ability to navigate an environment to a goal location involves two separable strategies and neural systems (Burgess, Reference Burgess2008; Maguire, Reference Maguire1998; Wolbers et al., Reference Wolbers, Weiller and Büchel2004). Allocentric navigation strategies involve a flexible, world-centered (object-to-object) representation (i.e., a “cognitive map”) and are hippocampal dependent. Egocentric navigation strategies involve environmental information represented from the perspective of the navigator (i.e., route based) and are dependent on the caudate nucleus and medial parietal cortex. In dual-solution paradigms, there is individual variability in which strategy is predominately used, paralleling structural and functional variance in the hippocampus and caudate (e.g., Bohbot et al., Reference Bohbot, Lerch, Thorndycraft, Iaria and Zijdenbos2007; Iaria et al., Reference Iaria, Petrides, Dagher, Pike and Bohbot2003; Marchette et al., Reference Marchette, Bakker and Shelton2011). While both strategies may be used during navigation, there is also evidence of competitive interactions between the systems (Packard & McGaugh, Reference Packard and McGaugh1996; Poldrack & Packard, Reference Poldrack and Packard2003).

While advancing age is associated with deterioration of the neural substrates of both navigation strategies (Raz & Rodrigue, Reference Raz and Rodrigue2006), the magnitude of hippocampal atrophy exceeds that of caudate atrophy (Raz et al., Reference Raz, Ghisletta, Rodrigue, Kennedy and Lindenberger2010; Walhovd et al., Reference Walhovd, Westlye, Amlien, Espeseth, Reinvang, Raz, Agartz, Salat, Greve, Fischl, Dale and Fjell2011). Consistent with this, there is greater age-related decrement on tasks assessing the formation, retention, and use of a cognitive map relative to learning a route (Head & Isom, Reference Head and Isom2010; Iaria et al., Reference Iaria, Palermo, Committeri and Barton2009; Lester et al., Reference Lester, Moffat, Wiener, Barnes and Wolbers2017). Additionally, older adults exhibit a shift from allocentric to egocentric strategies in dual-solution paradigms consistent with competitive interactions between the systems (Rodgers et al., Reference Rodgers, Sindone and Moffat2012; Wiener et al., Reference Wiener, de Condappa, Harris and Wolbers2013). Lastly, associations between hippocampal volume and allocentric tasks have been observed in older adults (Daugherty et al., Reference Daugherty, Yuan, Dahle, Bender, Yang and Raz2015; Head & Isom, Reference Head and Isom2010).

Several lines of evidence support poor sleep quality as a potential contributor to these age effects on allocentric navigation. Total sleep deprivation is associated with reduced hippocampal plasticity, a shift from allocentric to egocentric strategy use and a differential deficit in allocentric compared to egocentric navigation task performance in rodents (Hagewoud et al., Reference Hagewoud, Havekes, Tiba, Novati, Hogenelst, Weinreder, Van der Zee and Meerlo2010; Murata et al., Reference Murata, Oka, Iseki, Mori, Ohe, Mine and Enjoji2018; Soto-Rodriguez et al., Reference Soto-Rodriguez, Lopez-Armas, Luquin, Ramos-Zuñiga, Jauregui-Huerta, Gonzalez-Perez and Gonzalez-Castañeda2016). Sleep fragmentation, a measure of brief arousals during a bout of sleep, is more ecologically valid and common than sleep deprivation. Experimentally induced sleep fragmentation negatively impacts hippocampal plasticity and hippocampal-dependent learning in rats (Guzman-Marin et al., Reference Guzman-Marin, Bashir, Suntsova, Szymusiak and McGinty2007; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Katsuyama, Duge, Sriram, Krushelnytskyy, Kim and de la Iglesia2016; Sportiche et al., Reference Sportiche, Suntsova, Methippara, Bashir, Mitrani, Szymusiak and McGinty2010; Tartar et al., Reference Tartar, McKenna, Ward, McCarley, Strecker and Brown2010). Furthermore, sleep fragmentation is associated with disruptions in electrophysiological signals that support hippocampal-dependent memory processes (Astori et al., Reference Astori, Wimmer and Lüthi2013; Fogel & Smith, Reference Fogel and Smith2011; Ondze et al., Reference Ondze, Espa, Dauvilliers, Billiard and Besset2003). The incidence of sleep fragmentation rises with advancing age (Ohayon et al., Reference Ohayon, Carskadon, Guilleminault and Vitiello2004) and is modestly associated with impaired cognitive performance in older adults (André et al., Reference André, Tomadesso, de Flores, Branger, Rehel, Mézenge, Landeau, de la Sayette, Eustache, Chetelat and Rauchs2019; Blackwell et al., Reference Blackwell, Yaffe, Ancoli-Israel, Redline, Ensrud, Stefanick, Laffan and Stone2011; Kaneshwaran et al., Reference Kaneshwaran, Olah, Tasaki, Yu, Bradshaw, Schneider, Buchman, Bennett, de Jager and Lim2019; Luik et al., Reference Luik, Zuurbier, Hofman, Van Someren, Ikram and Tiemeier2015). However, much of this work was conducted with clinical populations or limited to general psychometric measures. Thus, targeted research exploring the impact of sleep fragmentation on spatial navigation processes associated with the hippocampus in healthy older adults is necessary.

Circadian rest-activity rhythm fragmentation, in the form of daytime napping and/or nighttime arousals, may also contribute to age effects on allocentric navigation. The intradaily variability, an actigraphy-derived measure of rest-activity rhythm fragmentation, increases with older age (Huang et al., Reference Huang, Liu, Wang, Van Someren, Xu and Zhou2002; Luik et al., Reference Luik, Zuurbier, Hofman, Van Someren and Tiemeier2013; Musiek et al., Reference Musiek, Bhimasani, Zangrilli, Morris, Holtzman and Ju2018; Oosterman et al., Reference Oosterman, Van Someren, Vogels, Van Harten and Scherder2009). Furthermore, higher intradaily variability has been associated with medial temporal lobe atrophy and dysfunction across the lifespan (Van Someren et al., Reference Van Someren, Oosterman, Van Harten, Vogels, Gouw, Weinstein, Poggesi, Scheltens and Scherder2019). The impact of circadian disruption on navigation strategies remains unclear.

Relevant to our central goal of understanding the effects of sleep fragmentation on allocentric navigation is consideration of other lifestyle factors. Exercise and stress have both been shown to regulate hippocampal plasticity, with exercise conferring benefits and stress imparting a negative impact in rodents and humans. (Erickson et al., Reference Erickson, Voss, Prakash, Basak, Szabo, Chaddock, Kim, Heo, Alves, White, Wojcicki, Mailey, Vieira, Martin, Pence, Woods, McAuley and Kramer2011; Gianaros et al., Reference Gianaros, Jennings, Sheu, Greer, Kuller and Matthews2007; Kleemeyer et al., Reference Kleemeyer, Kühn, Prindle, Bodammer, Brechtel, Garthe, Kempermann, Schaefer and Lindenberger2016; Lucassen et al., Reference Lucassen, Meerlo, Naylor, van Dam, Dayer, Fuchs, Oomen and Czéh2010; Machida et al., Reference Machida, Takamiya, Amagasa, Murayama, Fujiwara, Odagiri, Kikuchi, Fukushima, Kouno, Saito, Yoshimine, Inoue and Shobugawa2021; Niemann et al., Reference Niemann, Godde and Voelcker-Rehage2014). As an initial step in building models of how these factors may play a role in age effects on hippocampal-dependent navigation, we considered interactive effects of stress and exercise with sleep fragmentation. That is, neurophysiological changes with aging could make older adults’ hippocampal system simultaneously more vulnerable to cumulative damage from sleep fragmentation and stress as well as more likely to benefit from protective factors like physical activity and exercise (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Christian and Kiecolt-Glaser2006; Miller & O’Callaghan, Reference Miller and O’Callaghan2005).

The current study collected one-week measures of sleep quality, perceived stress, and physical activity as assays of trait-like characteristics to address several novel aims. First, we examined associations between sleep disturbance and spatial navigation. We hypothesized that sleep fragmentation would have a particularly negative impact on allocentric navigation performance and be associated with a shift from allocentric strategy use to egocentric strategy use. A second goal was to determine the role of sleep disturbance in age-related differences in spatial navigation. We hypothesized that sleep fragmentation would mediate age-related effects on allocentric navigation. Our third aim was to determine whether perceived stress or physical activity moderates the effects of sleep disturbance. We hypothesized that individuals with greater perceived stress would evidence greater negative effects of sleep fragmentation, and that this moderating effect would be greater for older adults. Additionally, we hypothesized that greater physical activity would mitigate the negative effects of sleep fragmentation, and this moderating effect would be greater for older adults. Total sleep time was also examined as a secondary measure of sleep quality, with the hypotheses that shorter sleep duration would serve a similar role as greater sleep fragmentation.

The fourth aim was to determine whether circadian rest-activity rhythm fragmentation was associated with spatial navigation in post-hoc analyses. We hypothesized that intradaily variability would be negatively associated with allocentric navigation performance and strategy use and would mediate age effects on these tasks.

Methods

Participants

Younger adult participants (aged 18–25) were recruited via an undergraduate participant pool in the Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences at Washington University or Volunteers for Health (VFH), a research volunteer registry at Washington University. Older adult participants (aged 60–89) were recruited via VFH. Participants were compensated monetarily or with course credit. All participants were right-handed with normal or corrected vision. Participants with a current or past psychological disorder or neurological disorder (e.g., stroke, dementia, traumatic brain injury), current sleep disorder, reduced mobility, or use of sleep-promoting medications were excluded. In total, 80 participants were enrolled (42 younger adults (27 females); 37 older adults (19 females); see Supplemental Table 1). There was a non-significant trend for higher education in older adults (p = .08). One older individual was excluded due to sleep medication use. Participants provided written consent in accordance with Washington University Human Research Protection Office guidelines.

Experimental procedures

General

Participants had two lab visits approximately one week apart. During the first visit, participants were given an actigraphy watch and instructions on completing a sleep diary. During the second visit, participants completed experimental tasks. A psychomotor vigilance task was first. Spatial navigation task order was counterbalanced. Participants completed stress, physical activity, computer expertise, health, and sense of direction questionnaires via REDCap before or after the second visit. A summary of study measures is in Table 1 and descriptive statistics are in Supplemental Table 2.

Table 1. Summary of key tasks and variables

Measures

Sleep fragmentation, total sleep time, and intradaily variability

Sleep measures were obtained using Actigraph GT9X watch (ActiGraph; Pensacola, FL) with a tri-axial accelerometer. With the exception of bathing or swimming, participants continuously wore the watch on the non-dominant wrist for three–eight consecutive nights (82% with 5+ days; mean = 6.2; SD = 1.4). Movement sampling frequency was set to 30 Hz and binned in 60-s epochs without the low frequency extension filter (Feito et al., Reference Feito, Hornbuckle, Reid and Crouter2017). A wear-time validation algorithm was applied before further processing (Choi et al., Reference Choi, Liu, Matthews and Buchowski2011). Sleep periods were determined in ActiLife (Pensocola, FL) using the Cole–Kripke algorithm (Cole et al., Reference Cole, Kripke, Gruen, Mullaney and Gillin1992). If entire nights of sleep were not detected by the Cole–Kripke algorithm but were indicated in participant’s sleep diary, self-reported sleep and wake times were manually entered (n = 23, no age group difference, p < .92). The sleep fragmentation index was computed as the sum of the movement index (total scored awake minutes/total time in bed in hours × 100) and the fragmentation index (total one-minute scored sleep bouts/total number of sleep bouts of any length × 100) (ActiLife 6 User’s Manual, 2012). Total sleep time was the average minutes of sleep per 24 hr.

Intradaily variability, a measure of rest-activity fragmentation, was calculated as a ratio of the variability between successive hours of activity and overall variability of activity in 1 day (Witting et al., Reference Witting, Kwa, Eikelenboom, Mirmiran and Swaab1990). Greater intradaily variability is consistent with greater fragmentation of rest-activity cycles. Intradaily variability was derived from full days of raw activity counts binned in 60-s epochs using the R package “nparACT” (Blume et al., Reference Blume, Santhi and Schabus2016).

Sleep diary

Participants completed the Consensus Sleep Diary (Maich et al., Reference Maich, Lachowski and Carney2018) as soon as possible each morning via REDCap or a paper form.

Psychomotor vigilance

This task was administered using Psychology Experiment Building Language (Mueller & Piper, Reference Mueller and Piper2014). Participants attended to a crosshair centered on the screen and quickly pressed the spacebar when a red dot appeared at randomly varying time intervals. Response time appeared after each trial. The index of performance was a standardized composite score of average reciprocal of response times, average response times for the slowest 10% of trials, and number of trials where response time exceeded 500 milliseconds (Basner & Dinges, Reference Basner and Dinges2011).

Spatial navigation tasks

Spatial navigation tasks were administered in Unity (San Francisco, CA) on 24-inch monitors (see Supplementary Figure 1). Participants first practiced using a joystick in a virtual environment of a long hallway with alternating turns. Next, visuomotor expertise was tested by requiring participants to traverse the environment within 60 seconds. All participants passed this test.

Cognitive mapping

For this task, the indoor environment consisted of interconnected hallways with twelve landmarks (Allison et al., Reference Allison, Fagan, Morris and Head2016). During study, participants freely explored a virtual environment for four minutes and were instructed to discover as many objects as they could and to pay attention to the objects and their locations. For the test phase, participants indicated the locations of the landmarks by making an X on a two-dimensional map. These study-test phases were repeated for four trials. Cognitive mapping performance was quantified as the average number of correctly placed landmarks across trials.

Route learning

During study, participants followed a path marked by arrows in an environment consisting of interconnected hallways and twenty-six landmarks (Allison et al., Reference Allison, Fagan, Morris and Head2016). For the test phase, participants were instructed to accurately traverse the route as quickly as possible without the arrows. These study-test phases were repeated for four trials. Route learning performance was quantified as average time to traverse the route across trials in seconds.

Spatial navigation strategy use

Participants were administered a dual-solution task (Marchette et al., Reference Marchette, Bakker and Shelton2011). First, participants studied pictures of each landmark with its associated name two times for familiarization. Next, participants encoded a route through an outdoor environment that included the twelve landmarks, interconnecting pathways, and four unique surrounding mountains by viewing a video across nine trials. To ensure sustained attention, five matching trials were randomly interspersed with experimental route trials. Matching trials were in a different environment from the experimental route environment with no landmarks, but instead with colored spheres along a route. Participants answered yes/no questions about number, color, or sequence in which spheres were presented.

For the test phase, participants were placed at one landmark in the maze and asked to locate a target landmark indicated at the top of the screen by name. Twenty-four combinations of starting landmarks and target landmarks were administered in random order. For 16 of these trials, participants could either find the target by following the path originally shown or by taking a shortcut route.

We used a protocol to classify trials similar to past work (Furman et al., Reference Furman, Clements-Stephens, Marchette and Shelton2014; Marchette et al., Reference Marchette, Bakker and Shelton2011). Shortcut routes were defined as paths that were shorter than the original path and with more than half of the path deviating from the original path. Original routes were defined as having more than half of the participants’ trajectory on the original path. Inefficient routes were defined as being longer than the original path. Unsuccessful routes were defined as not reaching the target location. The navigation strategy index was defined as the proportion of trials in which a shortcut was taken (0 = no shortcut use; 1 = shortcut used on all trials). Unsuccessful and inefficient routes were not included when calculating the strategy index.

Shortcut use is considered an allocentric strategy because it requires knowledge of object locations and their relative distances irrespective of viewpoint. Conversely, following an original route is considered an egocentric strategy, where the navigator uses knowledge of local landmarks and associated body turns from a first-person perspective. Therefore, higher values on the navigation strategy index, indicating greater use of shortcuts, is interpreted as reflecting greater allocentric strategy use. Greater shortcut use on this task correlates with relative activation of the hippocampus versus caudate (Furman et al., Reference Furman, Clements-Stephens, Marchette and Shelton2014; Marchette et al., Reference Marchette, Bakker and Shelton2011).

Perceived stress

Psychological stress over the prior month was measured with the ten-item version of the Perceived Stress Scale (Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Kamarck, Mermelstein, Cohen, Kessler and Underwood Gordon1994). This scale has demonstrated strong internal consistency, strong test–retest reliability, and moderate criterion validity in both younger and older adults (Lee, Reference Lee2012).

Physical activity

Physical activity was measured via actigraphy. Acquisition parameters and wear-time validation were the same as for sleep measures (Choi et al., Reference Choi, Liu, Matthews and Buchowski2011). ActiLife software was set to indicate wrist-worn watch, which scales the scoring algorithms accordingly via an internal algorithm (ActiLife 6 User’s Manual, 2012). The Freedson algorithm (Freedson et al., Reference Freedson, Melanson and Sirard1998) was used for cut-points to classify intensity levels into sedentary (<100 counts per epoch), light (100–1952), moderate (1952–5723), and vigorous activity (>5724) categories. Physical activity was quantified as average minutes in moderate to vigorous physical activity per valid wear day. Minutes of physical activity only included bouted activity, defined as at least 10 minutes of continuous physical activity allowing for two minutes of interruption (Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee, 2008).

Computer experience

Participants rated computer experience, experience playing computer games, and experience with computer games involving virtual reality on a 0–7 Likert scale (higher number = more experience).

Subjective sleepiness

The Stanford Sleepiness Scale (Hoddes et al., Reference Hoddes, Zarcone, Smythe, Phillips and Dement1973) was completed after the experimental tasks. Participants rated their current state on a 7-point scale (higher number = greater sleepiness).

Analytic approach

Covariates

Gender, computer experience, visuomotor expertise, subjective sleepiness, psychomotor vigilance, and number of nights of actigraphy measurement were considered as potential covariates. Variables significantly associated with age were included as covariates. Computer experience, subjective sleepiness, and number of measurement nights were significantly different between age group (ps < .05; see Table 1).

Outliers

Outliers were defined as data points > 2.5 standard deviations from the mean and with Cook’s D value greater than 4/n (Cook, Reference Cook1977). Results are presented with outliers removed (see Supplemental Tables 25 for results with outliers included).

Aims 1 and 2 analyses: associations amongst age, sleep characteristics, and spatial navigation

For these analyses, we used age group as the predictor, navigation measures as the outcomes, and sleep fragmentation (primary) and total sleep time (secondary) as mediators. All analyses were conducted in Stata 16.0 (StataCorp) using the “sem” command and full information maximum likelihood estimation to address missing data (see Table 1). The first step examined whether the necessary conditions for mediation were met (i.e., the predictor and outcome variable were significantly related to the mediator). Based on current recommendations, a significant association between predictor and outcome was not required to assess for mediation (MacKinnon et al., Reference MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West and Sheets2002; Rucker et al., Reference Rucker, Preacher, Tormala and Petty2011). Mediation analyses implemented a regression-based approach to estimate effects in conjunction with bootstrapping. The total effect represents the association of age group with spatial navigation, including both direct and indirect effects. The direct effects indicate the degree of association between: (a) age group and the mediator; (b) the mediator and spatial navigation controlling for age group; and (c) age group and spatial navigation controlling for the mediator. Indirect effects indicate the degree to which age group influences spatial navigation via the mediator. Indirect effects were examined using 5,000 bootstrapping samples and 95% percentile confidence intervals (CIs).

Aim 3 analyses: stress and physical activity as moderators of sleep associations with spatial navigation

Regression analyses were conducted to examine moderating effects. Analyses were conducted in Stata 16.0 (StataCorp) using the “sem” command and full information maximum likelihood estimation to address missing data (see Table 1). Continuous variables were standardized using a Z-transformation prior to creating interaction terms. In all analyses, covariates, age group, the sleep variable, perceived stress (or physical activity), and the primary 2-way interaction of interest (sleep variable × perceived stress (or physical activity) were entered in the first step. Remaining 2-way interactions with age group (age group × sleep variable; age group × perceived stress (or physical activity) as well as the 3-way interaction of primary interest were entered in the second step. Standardized coefficients and standard errors (SE) are presented.

Aim 4 post-hoc analyses: associations amongst age, intradaily variability, and spatial navigation

Analyses were conducted as described under Aim 1, except intradaily variability was the mediator.

Results

Aims 1 and 2: associations amongst age, sleep characteristics, and spatial navigation

Age-related differences

The age groups did not differ in sleep fragmentation, but older age was associated with reduced total sleep time (sleep fragmentation index: β = .183 [SE = .129], p = .153; total sleep time: β = −.386 [SE = .104], p < .001). There were age group differences in cognitive mapping, route learning, and navigation strategy use. Older adults were less able to form a cognitive map, recreate a learned route, and use shortcuts (β = −.732 [SE = .072], p < .001; β = .620 [SE = .091], p < .001; β = −.373 [SE = .117], p = .001, respectively).

Sleep fragmentation

Sleep fragmentation was not associated with cognitive mapping, route learning, or navigation strategy use (β = −.041 [SE = .068], p = .547; β = .094 [SE = .108], p = .35; β = .131 [SE = .103], p = .203, respectively).

However, we did observe that the association of sleep fragmentation with cognitive mapping differed by age group (see Tables 2 and 3) in the moderation models. The interaction remained significant when examined independently from stress or physical activity (stress model: β = −.265 [SE = .113], p = .019; physical activity model: β = −.298 [SE = .111], p = .008; independent model: β = −.250 [SE = .103], p = .015). Specifically, greater sleep fragmentation was associated with worse cognitive mapping performance among older adults but not younger adults (older: β = −.344 [SE = .156], p = .022; younger: β = .203 [SE = .168], p = .227; see Figure 1). However, sleep fragmentation did not mediate age effects on cognitive mapping within the older adults in a post-hoc analysis (β = −.155; b = −.032; 95% CI: −.088–.009).

Table 2. Stress as a moderator of sleep characteristics

SFI = sleep fragmentation index; TST = total sleep time.

Note. Data represent standardized path coefficients with standard errors in parentheses. Higher scores on the strategy index indicate greater relative shortcut use. Higher scores on cognitive mapping indicate better performance. Higher scores on route learning indicate worse performance.

* p < 0.05.

** p < 0.01.

*** p < 0.001.

Table 3. Physical activity as a moderator of sleep characteristics

MVPA = moderate-to-vigorous physical activity; SFI = sleep fragmentation index; TST = total sleep time.

Note. Data represent standardized path coefficients with standard errors in parentheses. Higher scores on the strategy index indicate greater relative shortcut use. Higher scores on cognitive mapping indicate better performance. Higher scores on route learning indicate worse performance.

* p < 0.05.

** p < 0.01.

*** p < 0.001.

Figure 1. Sleep fragmentation association with cognitive mapping. Scatter plot of the significant age group × sleep fragmentation interaction with separate regression lines for younger adults (gray squares; β = .203 [SE = .168], p = .227) and older adults (black circles; β = –.354 [SE = .154], p = .022). See text for details.

We also examined whether sleep fragmentation was more strongly associated with cognitive mapping relative to route learning within the older adults. While the association between sleep fragmentation and route learning was not significant (β = .123 [SE = .216], p = .570), the magnitude of the association was not significantly lower than the association with cognitive mapping (χ2 = 2.27, p = .132).

While there was also evidence of an age × sleep interaction for the strategy index in the physical activity model, the interaction was not significant when physical activity was removed from the model (β = .624 [SE = .196], p = .001; β = .271 [SE = .152], p = .075, respectively).

Total sleep time

Total sleep time was not associated with cognitive mapping, route learning, or the spatial navigation strategy index (β = .084 [SE =.075], p = .258; β = .119 [SE = .104], p = .252; β = −.199 [SE = .117], p = .087, respectively).

Aim 3: stress and physical activity as moderators of sleep associations with spatial navigation

Sleep fragmentation

There were no significant interactions involving either stress or physical activity with sleep fragmentation predicting navigation outcomes (ps > .113; see Tables 2 and 3).

Total sleep time

There were no significant interactions involving stress and total sleep time for any of the navigation outcomes (ps > .250; see Table 2). Physical activity did not moderate the association of total sleep time with cognitive mapping (ps > .894; see Table 3).

There was a significant total sleep time × physical activity interaction for route learning (β = .270 [SE = .098], p = .006). However, the interaction reflected that longer sleep duration was associated with worse performance at higher physical activity levels but was not associated with performance at lower physical activity levels (high PA: β = .238 [SE = .099], p = .016; low PA: β = .062 [SE = .201], p = .758).

We also observed an age group × physical activity × total sleep time interaction for navigation strategy that was inconsistent with hypotheses (β = .506 [SE = .219], p = .021; see Table 3). For older adults, less sleep was particularly associated with greater shortcut use (i.e., greater allocentric strategy use) at higher physical activity compared to lower physical activity levels (interaction: β = .484 [SE = .190], p = .011; high PA: β = −.239 [SE = .109], p = .028; low PA: β = −.924 [SE = .096], p < .001). The physical activity × total sleep time interaction was not significant for younger adults (β = −.200 [SE = .140], p = .155).

Post-hoc analyses were conducted to better understand these interactions. Considering evidence of an inverted U-shape relationship between sleep duration and cognitive outcomes (Mohlenhoff et al., Reference Mohlenhoff, Insel, Mackin, Neylan, Flenniken, Nosheny, Richards, Maruff and Weiner2018; van Oostrom et al., Reference van Oostrom, Nooyens, van Boxtel and Verschuren2018), we assessed for quadratic (i.e., nonlinear) associations for our navigation outcomes in the full sample by including the quadratic (squared) term for total sleep time in the regression models. There were significant quadratic associations of total sleep time with navigation strategy use and route learning (Supplemental Figure 2) (β = .275 [SE = .099], p = .005); β = .223 [SE = .101], p = .027, respectively). Thus, it is conceivable that observed interactions with physical activity are spurious (Belzak & Bauer, Reference Belzak and Bauer2019; Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Dahmen and DeVore2003).

Aim 4: Associations amongst age, intradaily variability, and spatial navigation in post-hoc analyses

Older adults evidenced lower intradaily variability compared to younger adults indicating reduced rest-activity fragmentation, which is opposite from the expected age-related increase (β = −.287 [SE = .112], p = .015). Greater intradaily variability was associated with lower cognitive mapping but was not related to route learning or strategy use (β = −.235 [SE = .074], p = .001; β = .134 [SE = .089], p = .129; β = −.095 [SE = .112], p = .413, respectively).

Within the older adult sample, there was the expected increase in intradaily variability with advancing age (β = −.449 [SE = .172], p = .009; β = −.389 [SE = .174], p = .026, respectively). In addition, greater intradaily variability was associated with lower cognitive mapping (see Figure 2), but not with route learning (β = −.437 [SE = .175], p = .005; β = .252 [SE = .153], p = .100, respectively) among older adults. However, intradaily variability was not differentially associated with cognitive mapping relative to route learning (χ 2 = .91, p = .341). Lastly, intradaily variability did not mediate age effects on cognitive mapping within the older adults (β = −.170; b = −.035; 95% CI: −.099–.006).

Figure 2. Circadian rest-activity fragmentation association with cognitive mapping. Scatter plot of the significant association of circadian rest-activity fragmentation with cognitive mapping. Younger adult data are depicted with gray squares and older adult data depicted with black circles. The separate regression lines represent the association in the full sample (gray line; β = –.235 [SE = .074], p = .001) and in the older adults (black line; (β = –.437 [SE = .175], p = .005). See text for details.

Discussion

The current study replicated age differences in spatial navigation tasks (Lester et al., Reference Lester, Moffat, Wiener, Barnes and Wolbers2017; Wiener et al., Reference Wiener, Kmecova and de Condappa2012) and extended this literature in several ways. The novel goals of this study were to examine: (a) associations between sleep disturbance and spatial navigation; (b) whether sleep disturbance mediated age differences in spatial navigation; (c) whether perceived stress and physical activity moderated sleep associations with spatial navigation; and (d) associations amongst rest-activity fragmentation, spatial navigation, and age in post-hoc analyses. We discuss the results for the each these aims below.

Associations amongst age, sleep characteristics, rest-activity fragmentation, and spatial navigation

Novel convergent findings were observed regarding the fragmentation measures and cognitive mapping. There was a significant age-related increase in sleep fragmentation within the older adult sample, which is consistent with previous findings (Boulos et al., Reference Boulos, Jairam, Kendzerska, Im, Mekhael and Murray2019; Ohayon et al., Reference Ohayon, Carskadon, Guilleminault and Vitiello2004). Sleep fragmentation has been linked to neurophysiological changes in the hippocampus including reduced neurogenesis (Guzman-Marin et al., Reference Guzman-Marin, Bashir, Suntsova, Szymusiak and McGinty2007), metabolic dysregulation (Nair et al., Reference Nair, Zhang, Ramesh, Hakim, Kaushal, Wang and Gozal2011; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Perland, Eriksson, Carlsson, Erlandsson, Laan, Potter, Frediksson, Benedict and Schiöth2016), and reduced long-term potentiation (Tartar et al., Reference Tartar, Ward, McKenna, Thakkar, Arrigoni, McCarley, Brown and Strecker2006). Consistent with these adverse physiologic effects, greater sleep fragmentation was associated with reduced cognitive map formation in older adults. Circadian rest-activity fragmentation also exhibited an age-related increase within the older adults in post-hoc analyses, similar to past observations (Luik et al., Reference Luik, Zuurbier, Hofman, Van Someren and Tiemeier2013; Musiek et al., Reference Musiek, Bhimasani, Zangrilli, Morris, Holtzman and Ju2018; Oosterman et al., Reference Oosterman, Van Someren, Vogels, Van Harten and Scherder2009; but see Park et al., Reference Park, Lee, Byun, Yi, Lee, Jeon, Hwang, Yoon, Choe, Kim, Shin, Suk and Lee2021). Importantly, greater circadian rest-activity fragmentation was also related to lower cognitive mapping performance. This is consistent with evidence linking circadian rest-activity fragmentation with hippocampal activity and atrophy (Sherman et al., Reference Sherman, Mumford and Schnyer2015; Van Someren et al., Reference Van Someren, Oosterman, Van Harten, Vogels, Gouw, Weinstein, Poggesi, Scheltens and Scherder2019). Neither the sleep nor circadian fragmentation measure significantly mediated age effects on cognitive mapping within the older adults in post-hoc analyses. This may be attributable to the modest effect sizes combined with a smaller sample size.

We also hypothesized specific negative effects of fragmented sleep and circadian rhythms on hippocampal-dependent navigation. However, we did not observe significant differential associations of either sleep fragmentation or circadian rest-activity fragmentation with cognitive mapping relative to route learning. In addition, there were no indications of fragmentation-related shifts from allocentric to egocentric strategies. Thus, it is relevant to consider that sleep fragmentation and circadian rest-activity fragmentation might instead lead to broad rather than selective deficits. Both types of fragmentation have been associated with reduced processing speed, memory, visuospatial abilities, executive functions, and general cognitive status to varying degrees (André et al., Reference André, Tomadesso, de Flores, Branger, Rehel, Mézenge, Landeau, de la Sayette, Eustache, Chetelat and Rauchs2019; Lim et al., Reference Lim, Yu, Costa, Leurgans, Buchman, Bennett and Saper2012; Luik et al., Reference Luik, Zuurbier, Hofman, Van Someren, Ikram and Tiemeier2015; McSorley et al., Reference McSorley, Bin and Lauderdale2019; Oosterman et al., Reference Oosterman, Van Someren, Vogels, Van Harten and Scherder2009; Swanson et al., Reference Swanson, Hood, Hall, Kravitz, Matthews, Joffe, Thurston, Butters, Ruppert and Harlow2021). This literature has generally used neuropsychological tests and no consistent pattern has emerged in terms of domain specificity. Thus, larger-scale studies using more targeted experimental tasks, such as those employed here, may be beneficial for determining the degree to which certain cognitive domains are differentially sensitive to sleep or circadian rest-activity fragmentation.

In terms of total sleep time, duration was not associated with any of the spatial navigation measures, except within the context of interactions with physical activity. However, linear associations were in the opposite directions from those hypothesized for route learning and navigation strategy. One potential contributing factor is the presence of nonlinear associations between total sleep time and both tasks in post-hoc analysis, which can lead to spurious interactions (Belzak & Bauer, Reference Belzak and Bauer2019; Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Dahmen and DeVore2003). Such nonlinear associations are observed in the literature particularly for self-report measures of sleep (Devore et al., Reference Devore, Grodstein and Schernhammer2016). However, findings are also mixed in terms of whether such quadratic associations are observed in younger versus older adults, and in terms of differences across cognitive domains (e.g., Devore et al., Reference Devore, Grodstein, Duffy, Stampfer, Czeisler and Schernhammer2014; Lo et al., Reference Lo, Groeger, Cheng, Dijk and Chee2016; Mohlenhoff et al., Reference Mohlenhoff, Insel, Mackin, Neylan, Flenniken, Nosheny, Richards, Maruff and Weiner2018; van Oostrom et al., Reference van Oostrom, Nooyens, van Boxtel and Verschuren2018; Wilckens et al., Reference Wilckens, Woo, Kirk, Erickson and Wheeler2014). We observed the best route learning performance associated with a sleep duration of approximately seven hours, which is consistent with prior literature implicating short and long sleep duration with poorer cognitive outcomes (Lo et al., Reference Lo, Groeger, Cheng, Dijk and Chee2016). In contrast, short and long sleep durations were associated with more optimal performance for the navigation strategy task (i.e., taking shortcuts reflective of an allocentric strategy). Considering this heterogeneity in findings, large-scale, systematic investigations are necessary to enhance mechanistic understanding of conditions, cognitive domains, and populations in which nonlinear effects are most likely to be observed.

Stress and physical activity as moderators of sleep associations with spatial navigation

Surprisingly, neither perceived stress nor physical activity moderated associations of sleep disruption with spatial navigation tasks (Minkel et al., Reference Minkel, Moreta, Muto, Htaik, Jones, Basner and Dinges2014; Van Reeth et al., Reference Van Reeth, Weibel, Spiegel, Leproult, Dugovic and Maccari2000). In terms of stress, the null effects may be related to its measurement, which was a single retrospective self-report of the past week. Self-reported stress has previously exhibited mixed findings in terms of associations with the stress hormone cortisol (O’Brien et al., Reference O’Brien, Tronick and Moore2013). Whereas modulation of navigation strategy use by experimentally induced acute stress has been more consistently associated with elevated cortisol (Diamond & Rose, Reference Diamond and Rose1994; Schwabe et al., Reference Schwabe, Schächinger, de Kloet and Oitzl2009). Furthermore, stress exposure may affect specific aspects of sleep physiology, such as slow wave activity (Sanford et al., Reference Sanford, Suchecki and Meerlo2015), which would not be captured here. Lastly, studies have indicated that acute and chronic sleep deprivation lead to increased hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis reactivity to experimentally induced stressors (Suchecki et al., Reference Suchecki, Tiba and Tufik2002). Thus, the ecological sleep changes observed here may not be potentiated by stress. Overall, it may be important to systematically incorporate polysomnography to determine sleep architecture, measures of chronic stress exposure, and HPA axis reactivity via cortisol.

Another consideration is that lower physical activity and greater perceived stress may instead be directly associated with greater sleep fragmentation and circadian rest-activity fragmentation (Hooghiemstra et al., Reference Hooghiemstra, Eggermont, Scheltens, van der Flier and Scherder2015; Lambiase et al., Reference Lambiase, Gabriel, Kuller and Matthews2013). Furthermore, there are age-related reductions in physical activity and complex age effects on stress appraisal (Watson et al., Reference Watson, Carlson, Gunn, Galuska, O’Connor, Greenlund and Fulton2016; Surachman & Almeida, Reference Surachman and Almeida2018). Thus, these two lifestyle factors may also instead directly mediate age effects on spatial navigation processes, rather than interact with sleep quality as examined here. Future work in larger samples should test more complex models (e.g., multiple mediator and/or moderated mediation).

Limitations

One potential limitation is the use of measures derived from wrist-worn actigraphy devices. Wrist actigraphy may be less sensitive to sleep fragmentation compared to polysomnography (de Souza et al., Reference de Souza, Benedito-Silva, Pires, Poyares, Tufik and Calil2003), and may underestimate physical activity levels in older adults relative to hip placement (Kwan et al., Reference Kwan, Liu, Lee, Tse and Lee2020). These factors may have influenced the lack of expected age group differences in sleep fragmentation (Boulos et al., Reference Boulos, Jairam, Kendzerska, Im, Mekhael and Murray2019; Ohayon et al., Reference Ohayon, Carskadon, Guilleminault and Vitiello2004), as well as the lack of modulation by physical activity. In addition, the finding of greater circadian rest-activity fragmentation in younger compared to older adults may be due to the recruitment of college students, as this group self-reports irregular sleep-wake patterns (Lund et al., Reference Lund, Reider, Whiting and Prichard2010). Lastly, although we excluded individuals with a known neurological condition, including dementia, we did not screen older adults for global cognitive status.

Conclusions

In conclusion, we provide novel evidence of associations of both sleep fragmentation and circadian rest-activity fragmentation with hippocampal-dependent spatial navigation in a non-clinical population. These results demonstrate that increased sleep fragmentation and circadian rest-activity fragmentation are associated with reduced cognitive mapping performance in older adults. There was no support for moderating influences of physical activity or psychosocial stress. These and other factors should be further examined to determine mediators and moderators of sleep fragmentation and circadian rest-activity fragmentation on spatial navigation.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617722000303

Acknowledgements

We thank Brandon Davis, Alex Nelson, Cheyenne Parson, and Chidera Agwu for assistance with data collection.

Author contributions

DH and HM participated in the design of the study, data analysis and data interpretation. HM participated in data acquisition and wrote the draft of the manuscript. DH participated in manuscript revisions. BJAP participated in providing actigraphy equipment, data interpretation, and manuscript revisions. All the authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience (DH) and National Institutes of Health R01AG057901 (BJAP).

Conflicts of interest

None.

References

ActiLife 6 User’s Manual. (2012). ActiLife 6 User’s Manual. ActiGraph Software Department.Google Scholar
Allison, S. L., Fagan, A. M., Morris, J. C., & Head, D. (2016). Spatial navigation in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Alzheimers Disease, 52, 7790. https://doi.org/10.3233/jad-150855 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
André, C., Tomadesso, C., de Flores, R., Branger, P., Rehel, S., Mézenge, F., Landeau, B., de la Sayette, V., Eustache, F., Chetelat, G., & Rauchs, G. (2019). Brain and cognitive correlates of sleep fragmentation in elderly subjects with and without cognitive deficits. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring, 11, 142150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dadm.2018.12.009 Google ScholarPubMed
Astori, S., Wimmer, R. D., & Lüthi, A. (2013). Manipulating sleep spindles – expanding views on sleep, memory, and disease. Trends in Neurosciences, 36, 738748. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2013.10.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basner, M., & Dinges, D. F. (2011). Maximizing sensitivity of the psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) to sleep loss. Sleep, 34, 581591. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/34.5.581 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belzak, W. C., & Bauer, D. J. (2019). Interaction effects may actually be nonlinear effects in disguise: A review of the problem and potential solutions. Addictive Behaviors, 94, 99108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blackwell, T., Yaffe, K., Ancoli-Israel, S., Redline, S., Ensrud, K. E., Stefanick, M. L., Laffan, A., & Stone, K. L., for the Osteoporotic Fractures in Men (MrOS) Study Group (2011). Association of sleep characteristics and cognition in older community-dwelling men: The MrOS sleep study. Sleep, 34, 13471356. https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.1276 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blume, C., Santhi, N., & Schabus, M. (2016). ‘nparACT’ package for R: A free software tool for the non-parametric analysis of actigraphy data. MethodsX, 3, 430435. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mex.2016.05.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohbot, V. D., Lerch, J., Thorndycraft, B., Iaria, G., & Zijdenbos, A. P. (2007). Gray matter differences correlate with spontaneous strategies in a human virtual navigation task. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 1007810083.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boulos, M. I., Jairam, T., Kendzerska, T., Im, J., Mekhael, A., & Murray, B. J. (2019). Normal polysomnography parameters in healthy adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Respiratory Medicine, 7, 533543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burgess, N. (2008). Spatial cognition and the brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 7797.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, L., Liu, Z., Matthews, C. E., & Buchowski, M. S. (2011). Validation of accelerometer wear and nonwear time classification algorithm. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 43, 357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, S. H., Na, D. L., Lee, B. H., Hahm, D. S., Jeong, J. H., Yoon, S. J., Yoo, K. H., Ha, C. K., Han, I. W., & Dementia Research Group (2001). Estimating the validity of the Korean version of expanded clinical dementia rating (CDR) scale. Journal of the Korean Neurological Association, 19, 585591.Google Scholar
Cohen, A., Dahmen, W., & DeVore, R. (2003). Adaptive wavelet schemes for nonlinear variational problems. SIAM Journal on Numerical Analysis, 41, 17851823.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1994). Perceived stress scale. In Cohen, S., Kessler, R. C. & Underwood Gordon, L. (Eds.), Measuring stress: A guide for health and social scientists (pp. 235283). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cole, R. J., Kripke, D. F., Gruen, W., Mullaney, D. J., & Gillin, J. C. (1992). Automatic sleep/wake identification from wrist activity. Sleep, 15, 461469. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/15.5.461 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, R. D. (1977). Detection of influential observation in linear regression. Technometrics, 19, 1518. https://doi.org/10.2307/1268249 Google Scholar
Daugherty, A. M., Yuan, P., Dahle, C. L., Bender, A. R., Yang, Y., & Raz, N. (2015). Path complexity in virtual water maze navigation: Differential associations with age, sex, and regional brain volume. Cerebral Cortex, 25, 31223131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Souza, L., Benedito-Silva, A. A., Pires, M. L. N., Poyares, D., Tufik, S., & Calil, H. M. (2003). Further validation of actigraphy for sleep studies. Sleep, 26, 8185. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/26.1.81 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devore, E. E., Grodstein, F., Duffy, J. F., Stampfer, M. J., Czeisler, C. A., & Schernhammer, E. S. (2014). Sleep duration in midlife and later life in relation to cognition. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 62, 10731081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devore, E. E., Grodstein, F., & Schernhammer, E. S. (2016). Sleep duration in relation to cognitive function among older adults: A systematic review of observational studies. Neuroepidemiology, 46, 5778.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, D., & Rose, G. M. (1994). Stress impairs LTP and hippocampal-dependent memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 746, 411414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erickson, K. I., Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., Basak, C., Szabo, A., Chaddock, L., Kim, J. S., Heo, S., Alves, H., White, S. M., Wojcicki, T. R., Mailey, E., Vieira, V. J., Martin, S. A., Pence, B. D., Woods, J. A., McAuley, E., & Kramer, A. F. (2011). Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proceeding of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 108, 30173022. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1015950108 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feito, Y., Hornbuckle, L. M., Reid, L. A., & Crouter, S. E. (2017). Effect of ActiGraph’s low frequency extension for estimating steps and physical activity intensity. PLOS ONE, 12, e0188242. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188242 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fogel, S. M., & Smith, C. T. (2011). The function of the sleep spindle: A physiological index of intelligence and a mechanism for sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 11541165. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freedson, P. S., Melanson, E., & Sirard, J. (1998). Calibration of the computer science and applications, Inc. accelerometer. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 30, 777781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furman, A. J., Clements-Stephens, A. M., Marchette, S. A., & Shelton, A. L. (2014). Persistent and stable biases in spatial learning mechanisms predict navigational style. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14, 13751391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gianaros, P. J., Jennings, J. R., Sheu, L. K., Greer, P. J., Kuller, L. H., & Matthews, K. A. (2007). Prospective reports of chronic life stress predict decreased grey matter volume in the hippocampus. Neuroimage, 35, 795803. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.045 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, J. E., Christian, L. M., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (2006). Stress, age, and immune function: Toward a lifespan approach. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 29, 389400. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-006-9057-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guzman-Marin, R., Bashir, T., Suntsova, N., Szymusiak, R., & McGinty, D. (2007). Hippocampal neurogenesis is reduced by sleep fragmentation in the adult rat. Neuroscience, 148, 325333. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.05.030 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagewoud, R., Havekes, R., Tiba, P. A., Novati, A., Hogenelst, K., Weinreder, P., Van der Zee, E. A., & Meerlo, P. (2010). Coping with sleep deprivation: Shifts in regional brain activity and learning strategy. Sleep, 33, 14651473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Head, D., & Isom, M. (2010). Age effects on wayfinding and route learning skills. Behavioural Brain Research, 209, 4958. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.01.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoddes, E., Zarcone, V., Smythe, H., Phillips, R., & Dement, W. C. (1973). Quantification of sleepiness: A new approach. Psychophysiology, 10, 431436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hooghiemstra, A. M., Eggermont, L. H. P., Scheltens, P., van der Flier, W. M., & Scherder, E. J. A. (2015). The rest-activity rhythm and physical activity in early-onset dementia. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders, 29, 4549. https://doi.org/10.1097/wad.0000000000000037 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, Y.-L., Liu, R.-Y., Wang, Q.-S., Van Someren, E. J. W., Xu, H., & Zhou, J.-N. (2002). Age-associated difference in circadian sleep–wake and rest–activity rhythms. Physiology & Behavior, 76, 597603. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00733-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iaria, G., Palermo, L., Committeri, G., & Barton, J. J. (2009). Age differences in the formation and use of cognitive maps. Behavioural Brain Research, 196, 187191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2008.08.040 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iaria, G., Petrides, M., Dagher, A., Pike, B., & Bohbot, V. D. (2003). Cognitive strategies dependent on the hippocampus and caudate nucleus in human navigation: Variability and change with practice. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 59455952.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaneshwaran, K., Olah, M., Tasaki, S., Yu, L., Bradshaw, E. M., Schneider, J. A., Buchman, A. S., Bennett, D. A., de Jager, P. L., & Lim, A. S. (2019). Sleep fragmentation, microglial aging, and cognitive impairment in adults with and without Alzheimer’s dementia. Science Advances, 5, eaax7331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleemeyer, M. M., Kühn, S., Prindle, J., Bodammer, N. C., Brechtel, L., Garthe, A., Kempermann, G., Schaefer, S., & Lindenberger, U. (2016). Changes in fitness are associated with changes in hippocampal microstructure and hippocampal volume among older adults. Neuroimage, 131, 155161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kwan, R. Y. C., Liu, J. Y. W., Lee, D., Tse, C. Y. A., & Lee, P. H. (2020). A validation study of the use of smartphones and wrist-worn ActiGraphs to measure physical activity at different levels of intensity and step rates in older people. Gait & Posture, 82, 306312. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2020.09.022 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lambiase, M. J., Gabriel, K. P., Kuller, L. H., & Matthews, K. A. (2013). Temporal relationships between physical activity and sleep in older women. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 45, 23622368. https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e31829e4cea CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, E.-H. (2012). Review of the psychometric evidence of the perceived stress scale. Asian Nursing Research, 6, 121127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, M. L., Katsuyama, Â. M., Duge, L. S., Sriram, C., Krushelnytskyy, M., Kim, J. J., & de la Iglesia, H. O. (2016). Fragmentation of rapid eye movement and nonrapid eye movement sleep without total sleep loss impairs hippocampus-dependent fear memory consolidation. Sleep, 39, 20212031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lester, A. W., Moffat, S. D., Wiener, J. M., Barnes, C. A., & Wolbers, T. (2017). The aging navigational system. Neuron, 95, 10191035. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.037 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lim, A. S., Yu, L., Costa, M. D., Leurgans, S. E., Buchman, A. S., Bennett, D. A., & Saper, C. B. (2012). Increased fragmentation of rest-activity patterns is associated with a characteristic pattern of cognitive impairment in older individuals. Sleep, 35, 633640.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lo, J. C., Groeger, J. A., Cheng, G. H., Dijk, D.-J., & Chee, M. W. (2016). Self-reported sleep duration and cognitive performance in older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Medicine, 17, 8798.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucassen, P. J., Meerlo, P., Naylor, A. S., van Dam, A. M., Dayer, A. G., Fuchs, E., Oomen, C. A., & Czéh, B. (2010). Regulation of adult neurogenesis by stress, sleep disruption, exercise and inflammation: Implications for depression and antidepressant action. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 20, 117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2009.08.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luik, A. I., Zuurbier, L. A., Hofman, A., Van Someren, E. J., Ikram, M. A., & Tiemeier, H. (2015). Associations of the 24-h activity rhythm and sleep with cognition: A population-based study of middle-aged and elderly persons. Sleep Medicine, 16, 850855. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2015.03.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luik, A. I., Zuurbier, L. A., Hofman, A., Van Someren, E. J. W., & Tiemeier, H. (2013). Stability and fragmentation of the activity rhythm across the sleep-wake cycle: The importance of age, lifestyle, and mental health. Chronobiology International, 30, 12231230. https://doi.org/10.3109/07420528.2013.813528 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lund, H. G., Reider, B. D., Whiting, A. B., & Prichard, J. R. (2010). Sleep patterns and predictors of disturbed sleep in a large population of college students. Journal of Adolescent Health, 46, 124132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machida, M., Takamiya, T., Amagasa, S., Murayama, H., Fujiwara, T., Odagiri, Y., Kikuchi, H., Fukushima, N., Kouno, M., Saito, Y., Yoshimine, F., Inoue, S., & Shobugawa, Y. (2021). Objectively measured intensity-specific physical activity and hippocampal volume among community-dwelling older adults. Journal of Epidemiology.Google ScholarPubMed
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maguire, E. A. (1998). Knowing where and getting there: A human navigation network. Science, 280, 921924. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.280.5365.921 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maich, K. H., Lachowski, A. M., & Carney, C. E. (2018). Psychometric properties of the consensus sleep diary in those with Insomnia disorder. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 16, 117134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchette, S. A., Bakker, A., & Shelton, A. L. (2011). Cognitive mappers to creatures of habit: Differential engagement of place and response learning mechanisms predicts human navigational behavior. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1526415268. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3634-11.2011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McSorley, V. E., Bin, Y. S., & Lauderdale, D. S. (2019). Associations of sleep characteristics with cognitive function and decline among older adults. American Journal of Epidemiology, 188, 10661075.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, D. B., & O’Callaghan, J. P. (2005). Aging, stress and the hippocampus. Ageing Research Reviews, 4, 123140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2005.03.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minkel, J., Moreta, M., Muto, J., Htaik, O., Jones, C., Basner, M., & Dinges, D. (2014). Sleep deprivation potentiates HPA axis stress reactivity in healthy adults. Health Psychology, 33, 14301434. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034219 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohlenhoff, B. S., Insel, P. S., Mackin, R. S., Neylan, T. C., Flenniken, D., Nosheny, R., Richards, A., Maruff, P., & Weiner, M. W. (2018). Total sleep time interacts with age to predict cognitive performance among adults. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 14, 15871594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mueller, S. T., & Piper, B. J. (2014). The psychology experiment building language (PEBL) and PEBL test battery. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 222, 250259. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10.024 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murata, Y., Oka, A., Iseki, A., Mori, M., Ohe, K., Mine, K., & Enjoji, M. (2018). Prolonged sleep deprivation decreases cell proliferation and immature newborn neurons in both dorsal and ventral hippocampus of male rats. Neuroscience Research, 131, 4551. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2017.08.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Musiek, E. S., Bhimasani, M., Zangrilli, M. A., Morris, J. C., Holtzman, D. M., & Ju, Y.-E. S. (2018). Circadian rest-activity pattern changes in aging and preclinical Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurology, 75, 582. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.4719 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nair, D., Zhang, S. X., Ramesh, V., Hakim, F., Kaushal, N., Wang, Y., & Gozal, D. (2011). Sleep fragmentation induces cognitive deficits via nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide phosphate oxidase–dependent pathways in mouse. American Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine, 184, 13051312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niemann, C., Godde, B., & Voelcker-Rehage, C. (2014). Not only cardiovascular, but also coordinative exercise increases hippocampal volume in older adults. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 6, 170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Brien, K. M., Tronick, E. Z., & Moore, C. L. (2013). Relationship between hair cortisol and perceived chronic stress in a diverse sample. Stress and Health, 29, 337344. https://doi.org/10.1002/smi.2475 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ohayon, M. M., Carskadon, M. A., Guilleminault, C., & Vitiello, M. V. (2004). Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: Developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan. Sleep, 27, 12551273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ondze, B., Espa, F., Dauvilliers, Y., Billiard, M., & Besset, A. (2003). Sleep architecture, slow wave activity and sleep spindles in mild sleep disordered breathing. Clinical Neurophysiology, 114, 867874. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00389-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oosterman, J. M., Van Someren, E. J. W., Vogels, R. L. C., Van Harten, B., & Scherder, E. J. A. (2009). Fragmentation of the rest-activity rhythm correlates with age-related cognitive deficits. Journal of Sleep Research, 18, 129135. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00704.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Packard, M. G., & McGaugh, J. L. (1996). Inactivation of hippocampus or caudate nucleus with lidocaine differentially affects expression of place and response learning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 65, 6572. https://doi.org/10.1006/nlme.1996.0007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, J. E., Lee, Y. J., Byun, M. S., Yi, D., Lee, J. H., Jeon, S. Y., Hwang, J. Y., Yoon, H., Choe, Y. M., Kim, Y. K., Shin, S. A., Suk, H. W., Lee, D. Y., & KBASE Research Group. (2021). Differential associations of age and Alzheimer’s disease with sleep and rest-activity rhythms across the adult lifespan. Neurobiology of Aging, 101, 141149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee. (2008). Physical activity guidelines advisory committee report, 2008. US Department of Health and Human Services.Google Scholar
Poldrack, R. A., & Packard, M. G. (2003). Competition among multiple memory systems: Converging evidence from animal and human brain studies. Neuropsychologia, 41(3), 245251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raz, N., Ghisletta, P., Rodrigue, K. M., Kennedy, K. M., & Lindenberger, U. (2010). Trajectories of brain aging in middle-aged and older adults: Regional and individual differences. Neuroimage, 51, 501511. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raz, N., & Rodrigue, K. M. (2006). Differential aging of the brain: Patterns, cognitive correlates and modifiers. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 730748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodgers, M. K., Sindone, J. A., & Moffat, S. D. (2012). Effects of age on navigation strategy. Neurobiology of Aging, 33, 202.e15202.e22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.07.021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rucker, D. D., Preacher, K. J., Tormala, Z. L., & Petty, R. E. (2011). Mediation analysis in social psychology: Current practices and new recommendations. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5, 359371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanford, L. D., Suchecki, D., & Meerlo, P. (2015). Stress, arousal, and sleep. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, 25, 379410. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2014_314 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwabe, L., Schächinger, H., de Kloet, E. R., & Oitzl, M. S. (2009). Corticosteroids operate as a switch between memory systems. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 13621372. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21278 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherman, S. M., Mumford, J. A., & Schnyer, D. M. (2015). Hippocampal activity mediates the relationship between circadian activity rhythms and memory in older adults. Neuropsychologia, 75, 617625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soto-Rodriguez, S., Lopez-Armas, G., Luquin, S., Ramos-Zuñiga, R., Jauregui-Huerta, F., Gonzalez-Perez, O., & Gonzalez-Castañeda, R. E. (2016). Rapid eye movement sleep deprivation produces long-term detrimental effects in spatial memory and modifies the cellular composition of the subgranular zone. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience, 10, 132. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2016.00132 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sportiche, N., Suntsova, N., Methippara, M., Bashir, T., Mitrani, B., Szymusiak, R., & McGinty, D. (2010). Sustained sleep fragmentation results in delayed changes in hippocampal-dependent cognitive function associated with reduced dentate gyrus neurogenesis. Neuroscience, 170, 247258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.06.038 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suchecki, D., Tiba, P. A., & Tufik, S. (2002). Hormonal and behavioural responses of paradoxical sleep-deprived rats to the elevated plus maze. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 14, 549554. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2826.2002.00812.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Surachman, A., & Almeida, D. M. (2018). Stress and coping theory across the adult lifespan. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Swanson, L. M., Hood, M. M., Hall, M. H., Kravitz, H. M., Matthews, K. A., Joffe, H., Thurston, R. C., Butters, M. A., Ruppert, K., & Harlow, S. D. (2021). Associations between sleep and cognitive performance in a racially/ethnically diverse cohort: The study of women’s health across the nation. Sleep, 44, zsaa182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tartar, J. L., McKenna, J. T., Ward, C. P., McCarley, R. W., Strecker, R. E., & Brown, R. E. (2010). Sleep fragmentation reduces hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cell excitability and response to adenosine. Neuroscience Letters, 469, 15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2009.11.032 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tartar, J. L., Ward, C. P., McKenna, J. T., Thakkar, M., Arrigoni, E., McCarley, R. W., Brown, R. E., & Strecker, R. E. (2006). Hippocampal synaptic plasticity and spatial learning are impaired in a rat model of sleep fragmentation. European Journal of Neuroscience, 23(10), 27392748.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Oostrom, S. H., Nooyens, A. C., van Boxtel, M. P., & Verschuren, W. M. (2018). Long sleep duration is associated with lower cognitive function among middle-age adults – the Doetinchem cohort study. Sleep Medicine, 41, 7885.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Reeth, O., Weibel, L., Spiegel, K., Leproult, R., Dugovic, C., & Maccari, S. (2000). Interactions between stress and sleep: From basic research to clinical situations. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 4, 201220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Someren, E. J. W., Oosterman, J. M., Van Harten, B., Vogels, R. L., Gouw, A. A., Weinstein, H. C., Poggesi, A., Scheltens, Ph., & Scherder, E. J. A. (2019). Medial temporal lobe atrophy relates more strongly to sleep-wake rhythm fragmentation than to age or any other known risk. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 160, 132138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2018.05.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walhovd, K. B., Westlye, L. T., Amlien, I., Espeseth, T., Reinvang, I., Raz, N., Agartz, I., Salat, D. H., Greve, D. N., Fischl, B., Dale, A. M., & Fjell, A. M. (2011). Consistent neuroanatomical age-related volume differences across multiple samples. Neurobiology of Aging, 32, 916932.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, K. B., Carlson, S. A., Gunn, J. P., Galuska, D. A., O’Connor, A., Greenlund, K. J., & Fulton, J. E. (2016). Physical inactivity among adults aged 50 years and older—United States, 2014. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 65(36), 954958.10.15585/mmwr.mm6536a3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiener, J. M., de Condappa, O., Harris, M. A., & Wolbers, T. (2013). Maladaptive bias for extrahippocampal navigation strategies in aging humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 60126017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiener, J. M., Kmecova, H., & de Condappa, O. (2012). Route repetition and route retracing: Effects of cognitive aging. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 4, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2012.00007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilckens, K. A., Woo, S. G., Kirk, A. R., Erickson, K. I., & Wheeler, M. E. (2014). Role of sleep continuity and total sleep time in executive function across the adult lifespan. Psychology and Aging, 29, 658665. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037234 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, M. J., Perland, E., Eriksson, M. M., Carlsson, J., Erlandsson, D., Laan, L., Mahebali, T., Potter, E., Frediksson, R., Benedict, C., & Schiöth, H. B. (2016). Recurrent sleep fragmentation induces insulin and neuroprotective mechanisms in middle-aged flies. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 8, 180. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2016.00180 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Witting, W., Kwa, I., Eikelenboom, P., Mirmiran, M., & Swaab, D. F. (1990). Alterations in the circadian rest-activity rhythm in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Biological Psychiatry, 27, 563572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolbers, T., Weiller, C., & Büchel, C. (2004). Neural foundations of emerging route knowledge in complex spatial environments. Cognitive Brain Research, 21, 401411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Summary of key tasks and variables

Figure 1

Table 2. Stress as a moderator of sleep characteristics

Figure 2

Table 3. Physical activity as a moderator of sleep characteristics

Figure 3

Figure 1. Sleep fragmentation association with cognitive mapping. Scatter plot of the significant age group × sleep fragmentation interaction with separate regression lines for younger adults (gray squares; β = .203 [SE = .168], p = .227) and older adults (black circles; β = –.354 [SE = .154], p = .022). See text for details.

Figure 4

Figure 2. Circadian rest-activity fragmentation association with cognitive mapping. Scatter plot of the significant association of circadian rest-activity fragmentation with cognitive mapping. Younger adult data are depicted with gray squares and older adult data depicted with black circles. The separate regression lines represent the association in the full sample (gray line; β = –.235 [SE = .074], p = .001) and in the older adults (black line; (β = –.437 [SE = .175], p = .005). See text for details.

Supplementary material: File

Maybrier et al. supplementary material

Maybrier et al. supplementary material 1

Download Maybrier et al. supplementary material(File)
File 29.7 KB
Supplementary material: File

Maybrier et al. supplementary material

Maybrier et al. supplementary material 2

Download Maybrier et al. supplementary material(File)
File 35.1 MB