Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T06:09:10.799Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Association of Sleep Quality on Memory-Related Executive Functions in Middle Age

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 August 2017

Brinda K. Rana*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California Sam and Rose Stein Institute for Research on Aging, La Jolla, California
Matthew S. Panizzon
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California Center for Behavioral Genetics of Aging, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
Carol E. Franz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California Center for Behavioral Genetics of Aging, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
Kelly M. Spoon
Affiliation:
Center for Behavioral Genetics of Aging, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California Computational Science Research Center, San Diego State University/Claremont Graduate University, San Diego, California
Kristen C. Jacobson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois
Hong Xian
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistcs, St. Louis University School of Public Health, St. Louis, MO St. Louis VA Healthcare System, St. Louis, Missouri
Sonia Ancoli-Israel
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California Department of Medicine, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
Michael Lyons
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts
William S. Kremen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California Center for Behavioral Genetics of Aging, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California Center of Excellence for Stress and Mental Health, VA San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, California
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Brinda K. Rana and William S. Kremen, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, 9500 Gilman Drive, MC-0738, La Jolla, CA 92093. E-mail: [email protected] and [email protected]

Abstract

Objectives: Sleep quality affects memory and executive function in older adults, but little is known about its effects in midlife. If it affects cognition in midlife, it may be a modifiable factor for later-life functioning. Methods: We examined the association between sleep quality and cognition in 1220 middle-aged male twins (age 51–60 years) from the Vietnam Era Twin Study of Aging. We interviewed participants with the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index and tested them for episodic memory as well as executive functions of inhibitory and interference control, updating in working memory, and set shifting. Interference control was assessed during episodic memory, inhibitory control during working memory, and non-memory conditions and set shifting during working memory and non-memory conditions. Results: After adjusting for covariates and correcting for multiple comparisons, sleep quality was positively associated with updating in working memory, set shifting in the context of working memory, and better visual-spatial (but not verbal) episodic memory, and at trend level, with interference control in the context of episodic memory. Conclusions: Sleep quality was associated with visual-spatial recall and possible resistance to proactive/retroactive interference. It was also associated with updating in working memory and with set shifting, but only when working memory demands were relatively high. Thus, effects of sleep quality on midlife cognition appear to be at the intersection of executive function and memory processes. Subtle deficits in these age-susceptible cognitive functions may indicate increased risk for decline in cognitive abilities later in life that might be reduced by improved midlife sleep quality. (JINS, 2018, 24, 67–76)

Type
Special Section: Lifespan Neuropsychology
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Benjamimi, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approah to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society . Series B, Statistical Methodology, 57(1), 289300.Google Scholar
Blackwell, T., Yaffe, K., Ancoli-Israel, S., Redline, S., Ensrud, K.E., Stefanick, M.L., & Stone, K.L. (2011a). Association of sleep characteristics and cognition in older community-dwelling men: The MrOS sleep study. Sleep, 34(10), 13471356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blackwell, T., Yaffe, K., Ancoli-Israel, S., Redline, S., Ensrud, K.E., Stefanick, M.L., & Stone, K.L. (2011b). Associations between sleep architecture and sleep-disordered breathing and cognition in older community-dwelling men: The Osteoporotic Fractures in Men Sleep Study. Journal of the American Geriatric Society, 59(12), 22172225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blackwell, T., Yaffe, K., Ancoli-Israel, S., Redline, S., Ensrud, K.E., & Stefanick, M.L., . . . Osteoporotic Fractures in Men Study Group. (2011). Association of sleep characteristics and cognition in older community-dwelling men: The MrOS sleep study. Sleep, 34(10), 13471356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bratzke, D., Steinborn, M.B., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2012). Effects of sleep loss and circadian rhythm on executive inhibitory control in the Stroop and Simon tasks. Chronobiology International, 29(1), 5561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bugg, J.M., DeLosh, E.L., Davalos, D.B., & Davis, H.P. (2007). Age differences in Stroop interference: Contributions of general slowing and task-specific deficits. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 14(2), 155167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buysse, D.J., Reynolds, C.F. III, Monk, T.H., Berman, S.R., & Kupfer, D.J. (1989). The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Research, 28(2), 193213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cain, S.W., Silva, E.J., Chang, A.M., Ronda, J.M., & Duffy, J.F. (2011). One night of sleep deprivation affects reaction time, but not interference or facilitation in a Stroop task. Brain and Cognition, 76(1), 3742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cajochen, C., Khalsa, S.B., Wyatt, J.K., Czeisler, C.A., & Dijk, D.J. (1999). EEG and ocular correlates of circadian melatonin phase and human performance decrements during sleep loss. The American Journal of Physiology, 277(3 Pt 2), R640R649.Google ScholarPubMed
Deliens, G., Leproult, R., Neu, D., & Peigneux, P. (2013). Rapid eye movement and non-rapid eye movement sleep contributions in memory consolidation and resistance to retroactive interference for verbal material. Sleep, 36(12), 18751883.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delis, D., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. (2001). Delis-Kaplan executive function system technical manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Delis, D., Kramer, J., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. (2000). California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Drake, C.L., Roehrs, T.A., Burduvali, E., Bonahoom, A., Rosekind, M., & Roth, T. (2001). Effects of rapid versus slow accumulation of eight hours of sleep loss. Psychophysiology, 38(6), 979987.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Driscoll, H.C., Serody, L., Patrick, S., Maurer, J., Bensasi, S., Houck, P.R., & Reynolds, C.F. III (2008). Sleeping well, aging well: A descriptive and cross-sectional study of sleep in “successful agers” 75 and older. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 16(1), 7482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellenbogen, J.M., Hulbert, J.C., Stickgold, R., Dinges, D.F., & Thompson-Schill, S.L. (2006). Interfering with theories of sleep and memory: Sleep, declarative memory, and associative interference. Current Biology, 16(13), 12901294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernandez-Mendoza, J., Calhoun, S., Bixler, E.O., Pejovic, S., Karataraki, M., Liao, D., & Vgontzas, A.N. (2010). Insomnia with objective short sleep duration is associated with deficits in neuropsychological performance: A general population study. Sleep, 33(4), 459465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fortier-Brochu, E., & Morin, C.M. (2014). Cognitive impairment in individuals with insomnia: Clinical significance and correlates. Sleep, 37(11), 17871798.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, N.P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 133(1), 101135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golden, C.J., & Freshwater, S.M. (2002). The Stroop Color and Word Test: A manual for clinical and experimental uses. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting.Google Scholar
Griessenberger, H., Heib, D.P., Lechinger, J., Luketina, N., Petzka, M., Moeckel, T., & Schabus, M. (2013). Susceptibility to declarative memory interference is pronounced in primary insomnia. PLoS One, 8(2), e57394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hasher, L., Lustig, C., & Zacks, R.T. (2008). Inhibitory mechanisms and the control of attention. In A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. Kane, A. Miyake & J. Towse (Eds.), Variation in working memory (pp. 227249). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoch, C.C., Dew, M.A., Reynolds, C.F. III, Monk, T.H., Buysse, D.J., Houck, P.R., & Kupfer, D.J. (1994). A longitudinal study of laboratory- and diary-based sleep measures in healthy “old old” and “young old” volunteers. Sleep, 17(6), 489496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaussent, I., Bouyer, J., Ancelin, M.L., Berr, C., Foubert-Samier, A., Ritchie, K., & Dauvilliers, Y. (2012). Excessive sleepiness is predictive of cognitive decline in the elderly. Sleep, 35(9), 12011207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kremen, W.S., Franz, C.E., & Lyons, M.J. (2013). VETSA: The Vietnam Era Twin Study of Aging. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16(1), 399402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kremen, W.S., Panizzon, M.S., Xian, H., Barch, D.M., Franz, C.E., Grant, M.D., & Lyons, M.J. (2011). Genetic architecture of context processing in late middle age: More than one underlying mechanism. Psychology and Aging, 26(4), 852863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kremen, W.S., Thompson-Brenner, H., Leung, Y.M., Grant, M.D., Franz, C.E., Eisen, S.A., & Lyons, M.J. (2006). Genes, environment, and time: The Vietnam Era Twin Study of Aging (VETSA). Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9(6), 10091022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyons, M.J., York, T.P., Franz, C.E., Grant, M.D., Eaves, L.J., Jacobson, K.C., & Kremen, W.S. (2009). Genes determine stability and the environment determines change in cognitive ability during 35 years of adulthood. Psychological Science, 20(9), 11461152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McHorney, C.A., Ware, J.E. Jr., & Raczek, A.E. (1993). The MOS 36-Item Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36): II. Psychometric and clinical tests of validity in measuring physical and mental health constructs. Medical Care, 31(3), 247263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, M.A., Wright, H., Ji, C., & Cappuccio, F.P. (2014). Cross-sectional study of sleep quantity and quality and amnestic and non-amnestic cognitive function in an ageing population: The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA). PLoS One, 9(6), e100991.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N.P., Emerson, M.J., Witzki, A.H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T.D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe“ tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nebes, R.D., Buysse, D.J., Halligan, E.M., Houck, P.R., & Monk, T.H. (2009). Self-reported sleep quality predicts poor cognitive performance in healthy older adults. Journals of Gerontology . Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 64(2), 180187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olaithe, M., & Bucks, R.S. (2013). Executive dysfunction in OSA before and after treatment: A meta-analysis. Sleep, 36(9), 12971305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orme, D., Brehm, W., & Ree, M. (2001). Armed Forces Qualification Test as a measure of premorbid intelligence. Military Psychology, 13, 187197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Potvin, O., Lorrain, D., Forget, H., Dube, M., Grenier, S., Preville, M., & Hudon, C. (2012). Sleep quality and 1-year incident cognitive impairment in community-dwelling older adults. Sleep, 35(4), 491499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prince, T.M., & Abel, T. (2013). The impact of sleep loss on hippocampal function. Learning & Memory, 20(10), 558569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reid, K.J., Martinovich, Z., Finkel, S., Statsinger, J., Golden, R., Harter, K., & Zee, P.C. (2006). Sleep: A marker of physical and mental health in the elderly. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14(10), 860866.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sagaspe, P., Sanchez-Ortuno, M., Charles, A., Taillard, J., Valtat, C., Bioulac, B., & Philip, P. (2006). Effects of sleep deprivation on Color-Word, Emotional, and Specific Stroop interference and on self-reported anxiety. Brain and Cognition, 60(1), 7687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saint Martin, M., Sforza, E., Barthelemy, J.C., Thomas-Anterion, C., & Roche, F. (2012). Does subjective sleep affect cognitive function in healthy elderly subjects? The Proof cohort. Sleep Medicine, 13(9), 11461152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schoenborn, C.A., & Heyman, K.M. (2009). Health characteristics of adults aged 55 years and over: United States, 2004-2007. National Health Statistics Reports, 131.Google ScholarPubMed
Scullin, M.K., & Bliwise, D.L. (2015). Sleep, cognition, and normal aging: Integrating a half-century of multidisciplinary research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(1), 97137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Servan-Schreiber, D., Cohen, J.D., & Steingard, S. (1996). Schizophrenic deficits in the processing of context. A test of a theoretical model. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53(12), 11051112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheth, B.R., Varghese, R., & Truong, T. (2012). Sleep shelters verbal memory from different kinds of interference. Sleep, 35(7), 985996.Google ScholarPubMed
Skaug, H., Fournier, D., Nielsen, A., Magnusson, A., & Bolker, B. (2011). glmmADMB: Generalized linear mixed models using AD Model Builder. R Package Version 0.6, 5, r143.Google Scholar
Spira, A.P., Beaudreau, S.A., Stone, K.L., Kezirian, E.J., Lui, L.Y., Redline, S., & Stewart, A. (2012). Reliability and validity of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index and the Epworth Sleepiness Scale in older men. The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 67(4), 433439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stickgold, R. (2005). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature, 437(7063), 12721278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stickgold, R., & Walker, M.P. (2005). Sleep and memory: The ongoing debate. Sleep, 28(10), 12251227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stroop, J.R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tucker, A.M., Whitney, P., Belenky, G., Hinson, J.M., & Van Dongen, H.P. (2010). Effects of sleep deprivation on dissociated components of executive functioning. Sleep, 33(1), 4757.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uhlaner, J. (1952). Development of the Armed Forces Qualification Test and predecessor army screening tests, 1946-1950. Retrieved from https://www.researchgate.net/publication/235047484_Development_of_Armed_Forces_Qualification_Test_and_predecessor_Army_screening_tests_1946-1950.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Enkhuizen, J., Acheson, D., Risbrough, V., Drummond, S., Geyer, M.A., & Young, J.W. (2014). Sleep deprivation impairs performance in the 5-choice continuous performance test: Similarities between humans and mice. Behavioral Brain Research, 261, 4048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waters, F., & Bucks, R.S. (2011). Neuropsychological effects of sleep loss: Implication for neuropsychologists. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(4), 571586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1997). Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Third Edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Rana supplementary material

Tables S1-S2 and Methods

Download Rana supplementary material(File)
File 47.2 KB