Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T13:48:30.909Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Remember to Buy Milk on the Way Home! A Meta-analytic Review of Prospective Memory in Mild Cognitive Impairment and Dementia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 May 2012

Esther van den Berg*
Affiliation:
Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Instituut, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands Department of Neurology, Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience, University Medical Center, Utrecht, The Netherlands
Neeltje Kant
Affiliation:
Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Instituut, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands Nieuw Unicum, Zandvoort, The Netherlands
Albert Postma
Affiliation:
Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Instituut, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands Department of Neurology, Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience, University Medical Center, Utrecht, The Netherlands
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Esther van den Berg, Experimental Psychology, Utrecht University, Heidelberglaan 2, 3584 CS Utrecht, The Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Prospective memory (PM) is the ability to remember to execute delayed intentions. Previous studies indicate that PM is impaired in persons with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia, but the extent, nature, and cognitive correlates are unclear. A meta-analytic review was, therefore, performed (literature search 1990 to July 2011) on case-control studies on PM in dementia (10 studies, 336 patients, 505 controls) and MCI (7 studies, 225 patients, 253 controls). Differences between event-based and time-based PM and between measures of prospective and retrospective memory were examined, as well as correlations with other cognitive functions. Results showed that patients with dementia or MCI exhibit large deficits in PM (Hedges’ d −1.62 [95% confidence interval −1.98 to −1.27; p < .0001] for dementia; −1.24 [−1.51 to −0.995; p < .0001] for MCI; difference dementia vs. MCI: QM = 1.94, p = .16). Impairments were comparable in size for event-based and time-based PM (p > .05), as well as for prospective and retrospective memory (p > .05). PM showed modest correlations with measures of retrospective memory (median r = 0.27) and executive functioning (median r = 0.30). PM appears a valid construct in neuropsychological assessment in patients with dementia or MCI, but more insight is needed in the optimal characteristics of PM tasks to be used in clinical practice. (JINS, 2012, 18, 1–11)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, J.W., Schmitter-Edgecombe, M. (2010). Mild cognitive impairment and feeling-of-knowing in episodic memory. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32, 505514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnaíz, E., Almkvist, O. (2003). Neuropsychological features of mild cognitive impairment and preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 107, 3441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Backman, L., Jones, S., Berger, A., Laukka, E., Small, B. (2005). Cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer's disease: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychology, 19, 520531.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A., Baddeley, H.A., Bucks, R.S., Wilcock, G.K. (2001). Attentional control in Alzheimer's disease. Brain, 124, 14921508.Google Scholar
Bell-McGinty, S., Lopez, O.L., Cidis Meltzer, C., Scanlon, J., Whyte, E.M., DeKosky, S.T., Becker, J.T. (2005). Archives of Neurology, 62, 13931397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanco-Campal, A., Coen, R.F., Lawlor, B.A., Walsh, J.B., Burke, T.E. (2009). Detection of prospective memory deficits in mild cognitive impairment of suspected Alzheimer's disease etiology using a novel event-based prospective memory task. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 154159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewer, G.A., Marsh, R.L., Clark-Foos, A., Meeks, J.T., Cook, G.I., Hicks, J.L. (2011). A comparison of activity-based to event-based prospective memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 632640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgess, P.W. (2000). Strategy application disorder: The role of the frontal lobes in human multitasking. Psychological Research, 63, 279288.Google Scholar
Burgess, P.W., Quayle, A., Frith, C.D. (2001). Brain regions involved in prospective memory as determined by positron emission tomography. Neuropsychologia, 39, 545555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camp, C.J., Foss, J.W., Stevens, A.B., O'Hanlon, A.M. (1996). Improving prospective memory task performance in persons with Alzheimer's disease. In M. Bradimonte, G.O. Einstein, & M.A. McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. 351367). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cockburn, J., Smith, P.T. (1988). Effects of age and intelligence on everyday memory tasks. In M.M. Gruneberg, P.E. Morris, & R.N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues. Vol. 2: Clinical and educational implications (pp. 132136). Chichester, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences (2nd ed). New Jersey: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Perri, R., Serra, L., Barban, F., Gatto, I., Zabberoni, S., Carlesimo, G.A. (2010). Prospective memory functioning in mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology, 24, 327335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, A., Perri, R., Zabberoni, S., Barban, F., Caltagirone, C., Carlesimo, G.A. (2011). Event-based prospective memory failure in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia, 49, 22092216.Google Scholar
Craik, F.I.M. (1983). On the transfer of information from temporary to permanent memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, Biological Sciences, 302, 341359.Google Scholar
Craik, F.I.M. (1986). A functional account of age differences in memory. In F. Klix & H. Hagendorf (Eds.), Human memory and cognitive capabilities: Mechanisms and performances (pp. 409422). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Duchek, J.M., Balota, D.A., Cortese, M. (2006). Prospective memory and apolipoprotein E in healthy aging and early stage Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology, 20, 633644.Google Scholar
d'Ydewalle, G., Bouckaert, D., Brunfaut, E. (2001). Age-related differences and complexity of ongoing activities in time- and event-based prospective memory. American Journal of Psychology, 114, 411423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
d'Ydewalle, G., Luwel, K., Brunfaut, E. (1999). The importance of on-going concurrent activities as a function of age in time- and event-based prospective memory. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 11, 219237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Einstein, G.O., Holland, L.J., McDaniel, M.A., Guynn, M.J. (1992). Age-related deficits in prospective memory: The influence of task complexity. Psychology and Aging, 7, 471478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Einstein, G.O., McDaniel, M.A. (1990). Normal aging and prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 717726.Google Scholar
Einstein, G.O., McDaniel, M.A. (1996). Retrieval processes in prospective memory: Theoretical approaches and some new empirical findings. In: M. Bradimonte, G.O. Einstein, & M.A. McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. 115141). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Feldman, H.H., Jacova, C. (2005). Mild cognitive impairment. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 13, 5662.Google Scholar
Glisky, E.L. (1996). Prospective memory and the frontal lobes. In M. Bradimonte, G.O. Einstein, M.A. & McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. 249266). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hedges, L.V. (1981). Distribution theory for Glass's estimator of effect sizes and related estimates. Journal of Educational Statistics, 6, 107128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hedges, L.V., Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Henry, J.D., MacLeod, M.S., Philips, L.H., Crawford, J.R. (2004). A meta-analytic review of prospective memory and aging. Psychology and Aging, 19, 2739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodges, J.R. (2000). Memory in the dementias. In E. Tulving & F.I.M. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 441459). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huppert, F.A., Beardsall, L. (1993). Prospective memory impairment as an early indicator of dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 805821.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huppert, F.A., Johnson, T., Nickson, J. (2000). High prevalence of prospective memory impairment in the elderly and in early-stage dementia: Findings from a population-based study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, S63S81.Google Scholar
Jones, S., Livner, A., Bäckman, L. (2006). Patterns of prospective and retrospective memory impairment in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology, 20, 144152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karantzoulis, S., Troyer, A.K., Rich, J.B. (2009). Prospective memory in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 407415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kazui, H., Matsuda, A., Hirono, N., Mori, E., Miyoshi, N., Ogino, A., Takeda, M. (2005). Everyday memory impairment of patients with mild cognitive impairment. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 19, 331337.Google Scholar
Kinsella, G.J., Ong, B., Storey, E., Wallace, J., Hester, R. (2007). Elaborated spaced-retrieval and prospective memory in mild Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 17, 688706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kvavilashivili, L., Ellis, J. (1996). Varieties of intention: Some distinctions and classifications. In M. Bradimonte, G.O. Einstein, & M.A. McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. 2352). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Livner, A., Laukka, E.J., Karlsson, S., Bäckman, L. (2009). Prospective and retrospective memory in Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia: similar patterns of impairment. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 283, 235239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martins, S.P., Damasceno, B.P. (2008). Prospective and retrospective memory in mild Alzheimer's disease. Arquivos de Neuropsiquiatria, 66, 318322.Google Scholar
Masdeu, J.C., Zubieta, J.L., Arbizu, J. (2005). Neuroimaging as a marker of the onset and progression of Alzheimer's disease. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 236, 5564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maylor, E.A. (1995). Prospective memory in normal ageing and dementia. Neurocase, 1, 285289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maylor, E.A., Smith, G., Della Sala, S., Logie, R.H. (2002). Prospective and retrospective memory in normal aging and dementia: an experimental study. Memory & Cognition, 30, 871884.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDaniel, M.A., Einstein, G.O. (2000). Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval: A multiprocess framework. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, S127S144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDaniel, M.A., Einstein, G.O. (2011). The neuropsychology of prospective memory in normal aging: A componential approach. Neuropsychologia, 49, 21472155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDaniel, M.A., Guynn, M.J., Einstein, G.O., Breneiser, J. (2004). Cue-focused and reflexive-associative processes in prospective memory retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 30, 605614.Google ScholarPubMed
Mori, A., Sugimura, K. (2007). Characteristics of assessment of motor and process skills and Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test in elderly women with dementia and community-dwelling women. Nagoya Journal of Medical Sciences, 69, 4553.Google Scholar
Petersen, R.C. (1995). Normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and early Alzheimer's disease. Neurologist, 1, 326344.Google Scholar
Petersen, R.C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. Journal of Internal Medicine, 256, 183194.Google Scholar
Petersen, R.C. (2007). Mild cognitive impairment: Current research and clinical implications. Seminars in Neurology, 27, 2231.Google Scholar
Petersen, R.C., Doody, R., Kurz, A., Mohs, R.C., Morris, J.C., Rabins, P.V., Winblad, B. (2001). Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology, 58, 19851992.Google Scholar
Portet, F., Ousset, P.J., Visser, P.J., Frison, G.B., Nobili, F., Scheltens, Ph., Touchon, J. (2006). Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) in medical practice: A critical review of the concepts and new diagnostic procedure. Report of the MCI Working Group of the European Consortium on Alzheimer's Disease (EADC). Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 77, 714718.Google Scholar
Rose, N.S., Rendell, P.G., McDaniel, M.A., Aberle, I., Kliegel, M. (2010). Age and individual differences in prospective memory during a “Virtual Week”: the roles of working memory, vigilance, task regularity, and cue focality. Psychology and Aging, 25, 595605.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, M.S., Adams, D.C., Gurevitch, J. (2000). MetaWin. Statistical Software for Meta-Analysis. Version 2.0. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T.A., Berish, D.E., Siedlecki, K.L. (2004). Construct validity and age sensitivity of prospective memory. Memory and Cognition, 32, 11331148.Google Scholar
Scheltens, P. (2009). Imaging in Alzheimer's disease. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 11, 191199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitter-Edgecombe, M., Woo, E., Greeley, D.R. (2009). Characterizing multiple memory deficits and their relation to everyday functioning in individuals with mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology, 23, 168177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinnott, J.D. (1989). Prospective/intentional memory and aging: memory as adaptive action. In L.W. Poon, D.C. Rubin, & B.A. Wilson (Eds.), Everyday cognition in adulthood and latelife (pp. 352369). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, G., Della Sala, S., Logie, R.H., Maylor, E.A. (2000). Prospective and retrospective memory in normal ageing and dementia: A questionnaire study. Memory, 8, 311321.Google Scholar
Spinnler, H., Della Sala, S. (1988). The role of clinical neuropsychology in the neurological diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neurology, 235, 258271.Google Scholar
Thompson, C., Henry, J.D., Rendell, P.G., Withall, A., Brodaty, H. (2010). Prospective memory function in mild cognitive impairment and early dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16, 318325.Google Scholar
Troyer, A.K., Murphy, K.J. (2007). Memory for intentions in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: time- and event-based prospective memory. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 365369.Google Scholar
Uttl, B. (2008). Transparent meta-analysis of prospective memory and aging. PLoS One, 3, e1568.Google Scholar
Wang, Y., Cui, J., Chan, R.C.K., Deng, Y., Shi, H., Hong, X., Shum, D. (2009). Meta-analysis of prospective memory in schizophrenia: Nature, extent, and correlates. Schizophrenia Research, 114, 6470.Google Scholar
West, R. (2005). Neural correlates of age-related decline in prospective memory. In R. Cabeza, L. Nyberg, & D. Park (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of aging (pp. 246264). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, B., Cockburn, J., Baddeley, A. (1985). The Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test. Fareham: Thames Valley Test Company.Google Scholar