Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T19:11:30.277Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Prospective Memory and Antiretroviral Medication Non-Adherence in HIV: An Analysis of Ongoing Task Delay Length Using the Memory for Intentions Screening Test

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 October 2012

Amelia J. Poquette
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
David J. Moore*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
Ben Gouaux
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
Erin E. Morgan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
Igor Grant
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
Steven Paul Woods
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: David J. Moore, Department of Psychiatry (8231), University of California, San Diego, 220 Dickinson Street, Suite B, San Diego, CA, USA 92103. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Using multi-process framework by McDaniel and Einstein (2000), the current study examined whether the length of prospective memory (PM) delay intervals as measured by the 2- and 15-min subscales of the Memory for Intentions Screening Test (MIST) have differential predictive value for antiretroviral (ARV) adherence. Participants included 74 HIV-infected individuals whose ARV adherence was tracked with an electronic monitoring system. Participants were classified as “adherent” (n = 49) or “non-adherent” (n = 25) based on recorded pill bottle openings of ≥90% of prescribed doses over 30 days. An adherence group by MIST delay interval interaction was observed, such that non-adherent participants had worse performance on the 15-min, but not 2-min delay PM MIST subscales. The observed MIST 15-min delay effects were significantly more pronounced on time- versus event-cued PM trials. Long-delay time-based PM was predictive of non-adherence independent of demographics, mood state, self-reported adherence, and general cognitive functioning. Findings from this clinical study indicate that ARV non-adherence may be particularly associated with deficits in strategic cue monitoring over longer PM delays, which may inform interventions to improve adherence among persons living with HIV infection. (JINS, 2012, 18, 1–7)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ammassari, A., Trotta, M.P., Murri, R., Castelli, F., Narciso, P., Noto, P., Antinori, A. (2002). Correlates and predictors of adherence to highly active antiretroviral therapy: Overview of published literature. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes, 31(Suppl 3), S123S127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrade, A.S., McGruder, H.F., Wu, A.W., Celano, S.A., Skolasky, R.L. Jr., Selnes, O.A., McArthur, J.C. (2005). A programmable prompting device improves adherence to highly active antiretroviral therapy in HIV-infected subjects with memory impairment. Clinical Infectious Diseases, 41(6), 875882. doi:10.1086/432877CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bangsberg, D.R. (2008). Preventing HIV antiretroviral resistance through better monitoring of treatment adherence. Journal of Infectious Diseases, 197(Suppl 3), S272S278. doi:10.1086/533415CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedict, R.H.B., Schretlen, D., Groninger, L., Brandt, J. (1998). Hopkins Verbal Learning Test- Revised: Normative data and analysis of inter-form and test-retest reliability. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12, 4355. doi:10.1076/clin.12.1.43.1726CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedict, R.H.B., Schretlen, D., Groninger, L., Dobraski, M. (1996). Revision of the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test: Studies of normal performance, reliability, and validity. Psychological Assessment, 8, 145153. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.8.2.145CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benton, A.L., Hamsher, K., Sivan, A.B. (1983). Multilingual Aphasia Examination (3rd ed.). Iowa City, IA: AJA Associates.Google Scholar
Carey, C.L., Woods, S.P., Gonzalez, R., Conover, E., Marcotte, T.D., Grant, I., … The HNRC Group. (2004). Predictive validity of global deficit scores in detecting neuropsychological impairment in HIV infection. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26(3), 307319. doi:10.1080/13803390490510031CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diehr, M.C., Cherner, M., Wolfson, T.J., Miller, S.W., Grant, I., Heaton, R.K., & HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center Group. (2003). The 50 and 100-item short forms of Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT): Demographically corrected norms and comparisons with the full PASAT in normal and clinical samples. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(4), 571585. doi:10.1076/jcen.25.4.571.13876CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gladsjo, J.A., Schuman, C.C., Evans, J.D., Peavy, G.M., Miller, S.W., Heaton, R.K. (1999). Norms for letter and category fluency: Demographic corrections for age, education, and ethnicity. Assessment, 6, 147178. doi:10.1177/107319119900600204CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golden, C.J. (1978). Stroop Color and Word Test. Chicago: Stoelting.Google Scholar
Heaton, R.K., Grant, I., Matthews, C.G. (1991). Comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead- Reitan Battery: Demographic corrections, research findings, and clinical applications. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.Google Scholar
Heaton, R.K., Taylor, M.J., Manly, J.J. (2002). Demographic effects and use of demographically corrected norms with the WAIS-III and WMS-III. In D.S. Tulsky, R.K. Heaton, G. Chelune, R. Ivnik, R.A. Bornstein, A. Prifitera, & M. Ledbetter (Eds.), Clinical interpretation of the WAIS-III and WMS- III. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hinkin, C.H., Castellon, S.A., Durvasula, R.S., Hardy, D.J., Lam, M.N., Mason, K.I., Stefaniak, M. (2002). Medication adherence among HIV+ adults: Effects of cognitive dysfunction and regimen complexity. Neurology, 59(12), 19441950.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kløve, H. (1963). Clinical neuropsychology. In F.M. Forster (Ed.), The medical clinics of North America. New York: Saunders.Google Scholar
Kongs, S.K., Thompson, L.L., Iverson, G.L., Heaton, R.K. (2000). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test – 64 card- computerized version. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Martin, B.A., Brown, N.L., Hicks, J.L. (2011). Ongoing task delays affect prospective memory more powerfully than filler task delays. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(1), 4856. doi:10.1037/a0022872CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDaniel, M.A., Einstein, G.O. (2000). Strategic and automatic processes in prospective remembering: A multiprocess framework. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, S127S144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, D.J., Posada, C., Parikh, M., Arce, M., Vaida, F., Riggs, P.K., … The HIV Neurobehavioral Research Program (HNRP) (2011). HIV-infected individuals with co-occurring bipolar disorder evidence poor antiretroviral and psychiatric medication adherence. AIDS and Behavior. doi:10.1007/s10461-011-0072-2 [Epub ahead of print].Google Scholar
Morgan, E.E., Weber, E., Rooney, A.S., Grant, I., Woods, S.P., & The HIV Neurobehavioral Research Program (HNRP) Group. (2012). Longer ongoing task delay intervals exacerbate prospective memory deficits in HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND). Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 34(4), 416427. doi:10.1080/13803395.2012.654764CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nieuwkerk, P.T., Sprangers, M.A., Burger, D.M., Hoetelmans, R.M., Hugen, P.W., Danner, S.A., … & The ATHENA Project. (2001). Limited patient adherence to highly active antiretroviral therapy for HIV-1 infection in an observational cohort study. Archives of Internal Medicine, 161(16), 19621968. doi:10-1001/pubs.Arch Intern Med.-ISSN-0003-9926-161-16-ioi00746CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raskin, S.A., Woods, S.P., Poquette, A.J., McTaggart, A.B., Sethna, J., Williams, R.C., Tröster, A.I. (2011). A differential deficit in time- versus event-based prospective memory in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychology, 25(2), 201209. doi:10.1037/a0020999CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reitan, R. (1979). Manual for administration of neuropsychological test batteries for adults and children. Tucson: Neuropsychology Laboratory.Google Scholar
The Psychological Corporation. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Third Edition (WAIS-III). San Antonio, TX: Author.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Memory Scale– Third Edition: Administration and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Weinborn, M., Woods, S.P., Nulsen, C., Park, K. (2011). Prospective memory deficits in Ecstasy users: Effects of longer ongoing task delay interval. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(10), 11191128. doi:10.1080/13803395.2011.614595CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woods, S.P., Dawson, M.S., Weber, E., Gibson, S., Grant, I., Atkinson, J.H. (2009). Timing is everything: Antiretroviral nonadherence is associated with impairment in time-based prospective memory. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(1), 4252. doi:10.1017/S1355617708090012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woods, S.P., Moran, L.M., Carey, C.L., Dawson, M.S., Iudicello, J.E., Gibson, S., … & HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center Group. (2008). Prospective memory in HIV infection: Is “remembering to remember” a unique predictor of self-reported medication management? Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23(3), 257270. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2007.12.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woods, S.P., Moran, L.M., Dawson, M.S., Carey, C.L., Grant, I. (2008). Psychometric characteristics of the memory for intentions screening test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 22(5), 864878. doi:10.1080/13854040701595999CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woods, S.P., Scott, J.C., Sires, D.A., Grant, I., Heaton, R.K., Troster, A.I. (2005). Action (verb) fluency: Test-retest reliability, normative standards, and construct validity. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11(4), 408415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Word Health Organization. Composite International Diagnostic Interview. (1998 (CIDI, version 2.1)). Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Young, R.C., Biggs, J.T., Ziegler, V.E., Meyer, D.A. (1978). A rating scale for mania: Reliability, validity and sensitivity. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 133, 429435. doi:10.1192/bjp.133.5.429CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zogg, J.B., Woods, S.P., Sauceda, J.A., Wiebe, J.S., Simoni, J.M. (2012). The role of prospective memory in medication adherence: A review of an emerging literature. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 35(1), 4762. doi:10.1007/s10865-011-9341-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed