Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-29T12:26:19.806Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Performing Lateralized Approach and Avoidance Behaviors: Effects on Perceptual, Affective and Confidence Judgments

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 January 2011

Vincent Dru*
Affiliation:
UFR-STAPS, Unité de Formation et de Recherche en Sciences et Techniques des Activités Physiques et Sportives, Université Paris Ouest, Nanterre, La Défense, France
Joël Cretenet
Affiliation:
UFR-STAPS, Unité de Formation et de Recherche en Sciences et Techniques des Activités Physiques et Sportives, Université Paris Ouest, Nanterre, La Défense, France
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Vincent Dru, University of Paris Ouest, UFR STAPS, 200 av de la république, F-92001 Nanterre Cedex, France. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

In recent work, we showed that the judgment of affective stimuli is influenced by the degree of congruence between apparently innate hemispheric dispositions (left hemisphere positive and approach, right hemisphere negative and avoidance), and the type of movement produced by the contralateral arm (flexion-approach; extension-avoidance). Incongruent movements (e.g., right arm extension) were associated with attenuation of affective valuations. In the present study, we replicated these results. We also assessed confidence in judgments as a function of stimulus valence and congruence and determined that confidence is maximal with congruent movements and highly positive or negative stimuli, suggesting that congruence effects on affective valuation could be mediated by confidence effects. However, in a second experiment, involving judgments regarding segmented lines, congruence effects were observed only for bisected lines, for which confidence was lowest. Thus, confidence does not provide a unifying explanation for congruence effects in the performance of these two tasks. (JINS, 2011, 17, 289–294)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Briñol, P., Petty, R.E. (2003). Overt head movements and persuasion: A self-validation analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 11231139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, J.T., Priester, J.R., Berntson, G.G. (1993). Rudimentary determinants of attitudes: Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, K.M., Ku, B.D., Jeong, Y., Lee, B.H., Ahn, H.-J., Kang, S.J. (2005). The influence of illusory motion on line bisection performance in normal subjects. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11, 881888.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cretenet, J., Dru, V. (2009). Influence of peripheral and motivational cues on rigid-flexible thinking: Perceptual, behavioral and cognitive aspects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138, 201217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daini, R., Arduino, L.S., Di Menza, D., Vallar, G., Silveri, M.C. (2008). Line bisection and cerebellar damage. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 21, 214220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, R.J. (1984). Affect cognition and hemispheric specialization. In C.E. Izard, J. Kagan & R. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotion, cognition and behavior (320365). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Davidson, R.J. (1992). Emotion and affective style hemispheric substrates. Psychological Science, 3, 3943.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, R.J., Ekman, P., Saron, C.D., Senulis, J.A., Friesen, W.V. (1990). Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: Emotional expression and brain physiology I. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 330341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dru, V., Cretenet, J. (2008). Influence of unilateral motor behaviors on the judgment of valenced stimuli. Cortex, 44, 717727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frassinetti, F., Rossi, M., Làdavas, E. (2001). Passive limb movements improve visual neglect. Neuropsychologia, 39, 725733.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ito, T.A., Cacioppo, J.T., Lang, P.J. (1998). Eliciting affect using the International Affective Picture System: Trajectories through evaluative space. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 855879.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lang, P.J., Bradley, M.M., Cuthbert, B.N. (2005). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Digitized photographs, instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical Report A-6. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.Google Scholar
Lin, K.-C., Cermak, S.A., Kinsbourne, M., Trombly, C.A. (1996). Effects of left-sided movements on line bisection in unilateral neglect. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 404411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacLeod, M.S., Turnbull, O.H. (1999). Motor and perceptual factor in pseudoneglect. Neuropsychologia, 37, 707713.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, P.J. (2009). Relating psychology and neuroscience. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 113125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porac, C., Cohen, S. (1981). Lateral preferences and human behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotteveel, M., Phaf, R.H. (2004). Automatic affective evaluation does not automatically predispose for arm flexion and extension. Emotion, 4, 156172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiff, B.B., Lamon, M. (1989). Inducing emotion by unilateral contraction of facial muscles: A new look at hemispheric specialization and the experience of emotion. Neuropsychologia, 21, 923935.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiff, B.B., Lamon, M. (1994). Inducing emotion by unilateral contraction of hand muscles. Cortex, 30, 247254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed