Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T10:12:58.708Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

It Is the Outcome that Counts! Damage to the Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Disrupts the Integration of Outcome and Belief Information for Moral Judgment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 August 2012

Elisa Ciaramelli*
Affiliation:
Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università di Bologna, Italy Centro studi e ricerche in Neuroscienze Cognitive, Università di Bologna, Italy
Davide Braghittoni
Affiliation:
Centro studi e ricerche in Neuroscienze Cognitive, Università di Bologna, Italy
Giuseppe di Pellegrino
Affiliation:
Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università di Bologna, Italy Centro studi e ricerche in Neuroscienze Cognitive, Università di Bologna, Italy
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Elisa Ciaramelli, Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università di Bologna, Viale Berti Pichat 5, 40126 Bologna, Italy. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Moral judgment involves considering not only the outcome of an action but also the intention with which it was pursued. Previous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) research has shown that integrating outcome and belief information for moral judgment relies on a brain network including temporo-parietal, precuneus, and medial prefrontal regions. Here, we investigated whether the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) plays a crucial role in this process. Patients with lesions in vmPFC (vmPFC patients), and brain-damaged and healthy controls considered scenarios in which the protagonist caused intentional harm (negative-outcome, negative-belief), accidental harm (negative-outcome, neutral-belief), attempted harm (neutral-outcome, negative-belief), or no harm (neutral-outcome, neutral-belief), and rated the moral permissibility of the protagonists’ behavior. All groups responded similarly to scenarios involving intentional harm and no harm. vmPFC patients, however, judged attempted harm as more permissible, and accidental harm as less permissible, than the control groups. For vmPFC patients, outcome information, rather than belief information, shaped moral judgment. The results indicate that vmPFC is necessary for integrating outcome and belief information during moral reasoning. During moral judgment vmPFC may mediate intentions’ understanding, and overriding of prepotent responses to salient outcomes. (JINS, 2012, 18, 1–10)

Type
Symposia
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, S.W., Barrash, J., Bechara, A., Tranel, D. (2006). Impairments of emotion and real-world complex behavior following childhood- or adult-onset damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 224235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrash, J., Tranel, D., Anderson, S.W. (2000). Acquired personality disturbances associated with bilateral damage to the ventromedial prefrontal region. Developmental Neuropsychology, 18, 355381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, R.J.R., Cipolotti, L. (2000). Impaired social response reversal: A case of ‘acquired sociopathy’. Brain, 123, 11221141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckner, R.L., Carroll, D.C. (2007). Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 4957.Google ScholarPubMed
Ciaramelli, E., di Pellegrino, G. (2011). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex and the future of morality. Emotion Review, 3, 308309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ciaramelli, E., Muccioli, M., Làdavas, E., di Pellegrino, G. (2007). Selective deficit in personal moral judgment following damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Social Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience, 2, 8492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
di Pellegrino, G., Ciaramelli, E., Làdavas, E. (2007). The regulation of cognitive control following rostral anterior cingulate cortex lesion in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 275286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egner, T., Etkin, A., Gale, S., Hirsch, J. (2008). Dissociable neural systems resolve conflict from emotional versus nonemotional distracters. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 14751484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eslinger, P.J. (1998). Neurological and neuropsychological bases of empathy. European Neurology, 39, 193199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eslinger, P.J., Damasio, A.R. (1985). Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation. Patient EVR. Neurology, 35, 17311741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fellows, L.K., Farah, M.J. (2003). Ventromedial frontal cortex mediates affective shifting in humans: Evidence from a reversal learning paradigm. Brain, 126, 18301837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, P.C., Happe, F., Frith, U., Baker, S.C., Dolan, R.J., Frackowiak, R.S.J., Frith, C.D. (1995). Other minds in the brain: A functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension. Cognition, 57, 109128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folstein, M.F., Robins, L.N., Helzer, J.E. (1983). The Mini-Mental State Examination. Archives of General Psychiatry, 40, 812.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallagher, H.L., Frith, C.D. (2003). Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 7783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallagher, H.L., Happe, F., Brunswick, N., Fletcher, P.C., Frith, U., Frith, C.D. (2000). Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: An fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gobbini, M.I., Koralek, A.C., Bryan, R.E., Montgomery, K.J., Haxby, J.V. (2007). Two takes on the social brain: A comparison of theory of mind tasks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 18031814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greene, J.D., Sommerville, R.B., Nystrom, L.E., Darley, J.M., Cohen, J.D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293, 21052108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grossman, M., Eslinger, P.J., Troiani, V., Anderson, C., Avants, B., Gee, J.C., Antani, S. (2010). The role of ventral medial prefrontal cortex in social decisions: Converging evidence from fMRI and frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neuropsychologia, 48, 35053512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jenkins, A.C., Mitchell, J.P. (2009). Mentalizing under uncertainty: Dissociated neural responses to ambiguous and unambiguous mental state inferences. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 404410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knobe, J. (2005). Theory of mind and moral cognition: Exploring the connections. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 357359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenigs, M., Young, L., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., Damasio, A. (2007). Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements. Nature, 446, 908911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, J.P., Macrae, C.N., Banaji, M.R. (2006). Dissociable medial prefrontal contributions to judgments of similar and dissimilar others. Neuron, 50, 655663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Eslinger, P.J., Bramati, I.E., Mourão-Miranda, J., Andreiuolo, P.A., Pessoa, L. (2002). The neural correlates of moral sensitivity: A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of basic and moral emotions. Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 27302736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moretto, G., Làdavas, E., Mattioli, F., di Pellegrino, G. (2010). A psychophysiological investigation of moral judgment after ventromedial prefrontal damage. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 18881899.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rorden, C., Brett, M. (2000). Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. Behavioural Neurology, 12, 191200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruby, P., Decety, J. (2003). What you believe versus what you think they believe: A neuroimaging study of conceptual perspective-taking. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 24752480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saver, J.L., Damasio, A.R. (1991). Preserved access and processing of social knowledge in a patient with acquired sociopathy due to ventromedial frontal damage. Neuropsychologia, 29, 12411249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaich Borg, J., Hynes, C., Van Horn, J., Grafton, S., Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (2006). Consequences, action, and intention as factors in moral judgments: An FMRI investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 803817.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shamay-Tsoory, S.G., Aharon-Peretz, J. (2007). Dissociable prefrontal networks for cognitive and affective theory of mind: A lesion study. Neuropsychologia, 45, 30543067.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shamay-Tsoory, S.G., Aharon-Peretz, J., Perry, D. (2009). Two systems for empathy: A double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain, 132, 617627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shamay-Tsoory, S.G., Tomer, R., Berger, B.D., Aharon-Peretz, J. (2003). Characterization of empathy deficits following prefrontal brain damage: The role of the right ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 324337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spinnler, H., Tognoni, G. (1987). Standardizzazione e taratura italiana di Test Neuropsicologici. Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 6(Suppl. 8), 120.Google Scholar
Stuss, D.T., Levine, B. (2002). Adult clinical neuropsychology: Lessons from studies of the frontal lobes. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 401433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saxe, R., Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people. The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”. Neuroimage, 19, 18351842.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, L., Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., Hauser, M., Damasio, A. (2010). Damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex impairs judgment of harmful intent. Neuron, 65, 845851.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, L., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., Saxe, R. (2007). The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 82358240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, L., Saxe, R. (2008). The neural basis of belief encoding and integration in moral judgment. Neuroimage, 40, 19121920.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, L., Saxe, R. (2009). Innocent intentions: A correlation between forgiveness for accidental harm and neural activity. Neuropsychologia, 47, 20652072.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed