Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T09:05:37.930Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Interference effects in chronic alcoholism

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 February 2009

Matthew J. Blusewicz
Affiliation:
Department of Veterans Affairs Northern California Health Care System, Pleasant Hill, CA 94523University of California, Davis, School of Medicine, Depts. of Neurology and Psychiatry, Sacramento, CA 95817
Joel H. Kramer
Affiliation:
University of California, San Francisco, School of Medicine, Dept. of Psychiatry, San Francisco, CA 94143
Richard L. Delmonico
Affiliation:
Santa Clara Valley Medical Center, Dept. of Psychology, San Jose, CA 95128

Abstract

This study investigated underlying mechanisms of the verbal memory disorder associated with chronic alcoholism. Previous investigations have suggested that alcoholics are more vulnerable to interference effects on verbal learning and memory tasks, both with respect to retroactive interference (RI) and proactive interference (PI); this was the hypothesis of the current study. Measures of RI and build-up and release from PI were administered to 31 abstinent male chronic alcoholics and 24 healthy male nonalcoholic control subjects. Alcoholics demonstrated more sensitivity to RI than controls. Additionally, alcoholics displayed a more rapid build-up of PI, although they showed normal release. An increased interference effect was found to be a component of chronic alcoholics’ verbal memory impairment and may differentiate chronic alcoholism from other disorders affecting verbal learning and memory. (JINS, 1996, 2, 141–145.)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Becker, J.T., Butters, N., Herman, A., & D'Angelo, N. (1983). A comparison of the effects of long-term alcohol abuse and aging on the performance of verbal and nonverbal divided attention tasks. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 7, 213219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brandt, J. & Butters, N. (1986). The alcoholic Wernicke-Korsa-koff syndrome and its relationship to long-term alcohol abuse. In Grant, I. & Adams, K.M. (Eds.), Neuropsychological assessment of neuropsychiatric disorders (pp. 441477). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Butters, N., Granholm, E., Salmon, D.P., Grant, I., & Wolfe, J. (1987). Episodic and semantic memory: A comparison of amnesic and demented patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 9, 479497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carroll, J.B., Davies, P., & Richman, B. (1971). Word frequency book. New York: American Heritage.Google Scholar
Cermak, L.S., Butters, N., & Moreines, J. (1974). Some analyses of the encoding deficit of alcoholic Korsakoff patients. Brain and Language, I, 141150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delis, D.C., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B.A. (1987). The California Verbal Learning Test. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Goldstein, G. (1987). Etiological considerations regarding the neuropsychological consequences of alcoholism. In Parsons, O.A., Butters, N., & Nathan, P.E. (Eds.), Neuropsychology of alcoholism: Implications for diagnosis and treatment (pp. 227246). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Grant, I. (1987). Alcohol and the brain: Neuropsychological correlates. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 310324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hightower, M.G. & Anderson, R.P. (1986). Memory evaluation of alcoholics with Russell's revised Wechsler Memory Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42, 10001005.3.0.CO;2-J>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kramer, J.H., Blusewicz, M.J., & Preston, K.A. (1989). The premature aging hypothesis: Old before its time? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 257262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lurla, A.R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Massman, P.J., Delis, D.C., Butters, N., Levin, B.E., & Salmon, D.P. (1990). Arc all Subcortical dementias alike?: Verbal learning and memory in Parkinson's and Huntington's disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, 729744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nixon, S.J., Kujawski, A., Parsons, O.A., & Yohman, J.R. (1987). Semantic (verbal) and figural memory impairment in alcoholics. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychohgy, 9, 311322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oscar-Berman, M. (1987). Neuropsychological consequences of alcohol abuse: Questions, hypotheses, nnd models. In Parsons, O.A., Butters, N., & Nathan, P.E. (Eds.), Neuropsychology of alcoholism: Implications for diagnosis and treatment (pp. 256272). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Peterson, L.R. & Peterson, M.J. (1959). Short-term retention of individual verbal items. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 193198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Postman, L. (1971). Transfer, interference, and forgetting. In Kling, J.W. & Riggs, L.A. (Eds.), Experimental Psychology (pp. 10191132). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Preston, K.A., Kramer, J.H., & Blusewicz, M.J. (1989). Prose memory in chronic alcoholics: Performance on the California Discourse Memory Test (abstract). Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, 6162.Google Scholar
Raymond, R.M., Stern, R.A., Authelet, A.M., & Penny, D. (1987). A comparison of CVLT performances among patients with MS, right-hemisphere vascular lesions, and normal controls (abstract). Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 9, 49.Google Scholar
Reed, R.J., Grant, I., & Rourke, S.B. (1992). Long-term abstinent alcoholics have normal memory. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 16, 677683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riege, W.H., Tomaszewski, R., Lanto, A., & Metter, E.J. (1984). Age and alcoholism: Independent memory decrements. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 8, 4247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, C. & Butters, N. (1980a). Further evidence for a continuum of impairment encompassing male alcoholic Korsakoff patients and chronic alcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 4, 190198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, C. & Butters, N. (1980b). Learning and memory impairments in young and old alcoholics: Evidence for the premature aging hypothesis. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 4, 288293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sagar, H.J., Sullivan, E.V., Cooper, J.A., & Jordan, N. (1991). Normal release from proactive interference in untreated patients with Parkinson's disease, Neuropsychologia, 29, 10331044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaeffer, K.W. & Parsons, O.A. (1987). Learning impairment in alcoholics using an ecologically relevant test. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 213218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squire, L.R., Shimamura, A.P., & Graf, P. (1987). Strength and duration of priming effects in normal subjects and amnestic patients. Neuropsychologia, 25, 195210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tweedy, J.R., Langer, K.G., & McDowell, F.H. (1982). The effect of semantic relations on the memory deficit associated with Parkinson's disease. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 4, 235247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. New York: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wickens, D.D., Born, D.G., & Allen, C.K. (1963). Proactive inhibition and item similarity in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 440445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winocur, G., Kinsbourne, M., & Moscovitch, M. (1981). The effect of cuing on release from proactive interference in Korsakoff amnesic patients. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 7, 5665.Google ScholarPubMed