Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T09:04:58.146Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Religion or Race? Using Intersectionality to Examine the Role of Muslim Identity and Evaluations on Belonging in the United States

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 June 2023

Amanda Sahar d’Urso*
Affiliation:
Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA
Tabitha Bonilla
Affiliation:
Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA
*
Corresponding author: Amanda Sahar d’Urso; Email: Amanda.S.d’[email protected]

Abstract

How do White Americans evaluate the politics of belonging in the United States across different ethnoreligious identity categories? This paper examines this question through two competing frameworks. On the one hand, given the salience of anti-Muslim attitudes in the United States, we consider whether White Americans penalize Muslim immigrants to the United States regardless of their ethnoracial background. On the other hand, Muslim identity is often conflated by the general public with Middle Eastern and North African (MENA) ethnoracial identity. We argue MENA-Muslim identity should be understood through the lens of intersectionality. In this case, White Americans may penalize MENA-Muslims immigrants to the United States more than Muslims from other ethnoracial groups. We test these two frameworks through a conjoint experimental design wherein respondents are asked to evaluate immigrants and indicate to whom the United States should give a green card—signaling legal belonging—and how likely the immigrant is to assimilate into America—signaling cultural belonging. Although White Americans believe White Muslims may assimilate better to the United States relative to MENA-Muslims, race does not moderate how White Americans evaluate who should be allowed to belong in the United States.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Race, Ethnicity, and Politics Section of the American Political Science Association

How do White Americans evaluate the politics of belonging in the United States across different ethnoreligious identity categories? Research suggests Americans are more accepting of immigrants with higher levels of education, White-collar jobs, and English proficiency (Adida et al. Reference Adida, Lo and Platas2019; Hainmueller et al. Reference Hainmueller, Hopkins and Yamamoto2014; Hainmueller, and Hopkins Reference Hainmueller and Hopkins2015). The literature describes these characteristics as signaling how much immigrants might contribute to the economy and how easily they can assimilate into American culture, each a constitutive part of belonging. Some of this work has also found that the country of origin of the immigrant influence these decisions, as well. For instance, certain groups, such as Asian and Latin Americans, are evaluated as foreigners even if they have lived in the country for generations (Chouhoud, Reference Chouhoud and Stockemer2022; Huynh et al. Reference Huynh, Devos and Smalarz2011; C. J. Kim Reference Kim1999; S. Y. Kim et al. Reference Kim, Wang, Deng, Alvarez and Li2011). In this case, country of origin is a salient identity characteristic that precludes the group from full inclusion. This exemplifies how certain racial groups may not be evaluated as belonging in the United States, even if they embody qualities such as English proficiency or high education merely due to their racial background. While country of origin is usually a marker of insider or outsider status as well as one’s race/ethnicity, recent research suggests that religion plays a role in how society perceives and assigns racial categories to others (d’Urso Reference d’Urso2022).Footnote 1 This means religion may also play a role in how people decide who belongs in the United States. Given that religion and race can be conflated, how do Americans navigate between these two identity characteristics when expressing who belongs in the United States?

In the case of Muslims and Middle Easterners and North Africans (MENA) individuals, religion and race are often conflated (Beydoun Reference Beydoun2013; Husain Reference Husain2019; Lajevardi Reference Lajevardi2020; Peek Reference Peek2005). This complexifies our understanding of the formation of attitudes toward migrants (Allport Reference Allport1954; Arora Reference Arora2020; Hellwig, and Sinno Reference Hellwig and Sinno2017; Reny, and Barreto Reference Reny and Barreto2022), the racialization of different migrant groups (Romero Reference Romero2008; Sáenz, and Manges Douglas Reference Sáenz and Manges Douglas2015; Tesler Reference Tesler2018; Valentino et al. Reference Valentino, Brader and Jardina2013), and perceptions of belonging in the United States (Chouhoud Reference Chouhoud and Stockemer2022; Esaiasson et al. Reference Esaiasson, Lajevardi and Sohlberg2022; Hobbs, and Lajevardi Reference Hobbs and Lajevardi2019; Lajevardi Reference Lajevardi2020). For instance, White Americans are less likely to accept certain Muslims (Adida et al. Reference Adida, Lo and Platas2019; Chouhoud Reference Chouhoud and Stockemer2022) and certain Middle Easterners (Hainmueller, and Hopkins Reference Hainmueller and Hopkins2015) into the United States. Given that MENA and Muslim identities are often described interchangeably, it is not clear what drives these existing findings: religion (i.e., presumed Muslimness of a given MENA immigrant) or race/ethniciy (i.e., presumed MENA identity of a given Muslim immigrant).

We compare two hypotheses to understand the relationship between religion and race (proxied through country of origin) on belonging. Our first hypothesis is that religion, specifically Muslim identity, has been racialized to the point where actual race/ethnicity will be an irrelevant identity characteristic for those who belong in the United States. That is, regardless of an individual’s race, a Muslim identity will be the most salient identity factor White Americans will use to evaluate who belongs. We contrast this approach to the idea that MENA-Muslim identity is understood intersectionally: while MENA and Muslim identities can overlap, one can be MENA and not Muslim or Muslim and not MENA. In practice, however, because individuals often use the terms MENA and Muslim interchangeably, the two identities may not be understood as distinct identity facets but instead conflated into one trait that transcends either category of Muslim or MENA. Thus, White Americans may discriminate more against an individual who fits the prototype of a MENA-Muslim immigrant.

We examine these questions while using a conjoint survey experiment that allows us to create more holistic profiles of migrants and specify religion and race (proxied via country of origin). The experimental design fielded through Bovitz, Inc., isolates the role of both country of origin and religion on who White Americans indicate belongs in the United States—both via green card and assimilation. With this measurement strategy, we investigate how the public conceptualizes Muslim migrants of different origins, in contrast to MENA individuals with other religions. We find White Americans singularly consider religion when determining who should be given a green card. However, they do consider religion and race when determining who will better assimilate into the United States.

The questions at the heart of this paper, then, examine the nature of MENA as an ethnoracial categorization: a demographic marker that incorporates elements of both race and ethnicity. MENA individuals come from a specific geographic area, can be Muslim, but are not exclusively, and may be of various races. While MENA and Muslim identities may overlap and often be conflated with each other among outsiders, one key effort in this paper is to recognize that research must understand the extent to which the public understands these overlapping identities. Thus, a major contribution of this effort is to consider how ethnoracial identities are understood by outsiders.

The results also have important policy implications. First, the results present further evidence for reconsidering a MENA categorization on the census, as MENA individuals are indeed viewed differently than White (the current legal classification in the United States), which is consistent with other research (d’Urso Reference d’Urso2022). While these definitions are important for how migrants are viewed, they are also important in determining how to present questions on the U.S. Census, and if the traditional question choices are the most appropriate (Jones Reference Jones2017). These findings join that of prior work (Beydoun Reference Beydoun2015a, Reference Beydoun2015b; Jonny Reference Jonny2020; Kayyali Reference Kayyali2013; Mathews et al. Reference Mathews, Phelan, Jones, Konya, Marks, Pratt, Coombs and Bentley2017; Strmic-Pawl et al. Reference Strmic-Pawl, Jackson and Garner2018) as evidence that including MENA as a separate category most accurately describes the experience of MENA individuals in the United States—regardless or religious background.

With practical application to immigration policy, this research gives insight into how White Americans may react to policies that support including immigrants—regardless of their education, English fluency, or race—simply because they are Muslim. But in a broader sense, it elucidates the immense burdens Muslim Americans may face when trying to belong in and assimilate into the United States. This can have downstream consequences for how the functioning of a pluralistic democracy when groups who otherwise legally belong are not treated as such. We add nuance to the literature on immigration and belonging by showing that race or religion alone is not enough for understanding societal perceptions of who belongs. In fact, removing religion from consideration prevents us from understanding the marginalization of those who may be racially White but excluded from joining America due to Muslim identity. At the same time, focusing on Muslim identity, alone, precludes us from understanding how anti-Islamic attitudes toward those already in the United States are not uniformly distributed across Muslim racial identity groups.

How religion and race contribute to belonging

Social belonging revolves around who society at large accepts and believes belongs in a given country.Footnote 2 Deeply tied to an understanding of what belonging means are beliefs related to if and how well migrant communities will assimilate into a new country (Bonilla, and Mo Reference Bonilla and Mo2018). These attitudes tend to rest on assessments of cultural or religious assimilation, such as whether migrants will “threaten” national traditions (Brader et al. Reference Brader, Valentino and Suhay2008; Fetzer Reference Fetzer2000; Kinder, and Kam Reference Kinder and Kam2010; Knoll et al. Reference Knoll, Redlawsk and Sanborn2011).Footnote 3 For Muslim and MENA individuals, understanding belonging in terms of cultural assimilation is of particular importance because a key element of belonging incorporates religion as a feature potentially preventing assimilation. This paper examines assessments of belonging but does so using two competing theories for the interplay between race and religion on social belonging in the United States. First, we examine literature suggesting Muslim identity, alone, is the most salient identity characteristic when considering who belongs in the United States. We compare this with the literature on intersectionality. MENA and Muslim identities are understood so interchangeably that it has transcended either category and is an identity in and of themselves. Thus, White Americans may only preclude MENA-Muslim immigrants from social belonging.

The salience of Muslim identity

Anti-Muslim attitudes and policies did not exclusively emerge out of 9/11, though much work has discussed changes in anti-Muslim attitudes post-9/11 (Bonilla et al. Reference Bonilla, Filindra and Lajevardi2022; Lajevardi Reference Lajevardi2020; Naber Reference Naber2000). For instance, Kalkan et al. (Reference Kalkan, Layman and Uslaner2009) find Americans view Muslims negatively because they are seen as a cultural outgroup. Although tolerance toward other religious and racial outgroups is positively correlated with positivity toward Muslims, perceptions of Muslims as cultural outsiders are negatively correlated with attitudes toward Muslims. Moreover, when comparing levels of xenophobia (i.e., fear, hatred, or prejudice against those from another country) versus Islamophobia (i.e., fear, hatred, or prejudice against Islam and Muslims), researchers find respondents have a stronger negative effect on Muslim foreigners than toward (non-Muslim) foreigners (Spruyt, and Elchardus Reference Spruyt and Elchardus2012).

Being perceived as cultural outsiders stems from the long-standing tropes of Orientalism (Said Reference Said1979). As Said argues, the Occident could only understand itself through the creation of the Orient. The Orient and by extension Muslims have been stereotyped as violent, misogynistic, intolerant, and fundamentalists (Esposito, and Kalin Reference Esposito and Kalin2011; Hobbs, and Lajevardi Reference Hobbs and Lajevardi2019; Khan, and Ecklund Reference Khan and Ecklund2012; Said Reference Said1979). Research suggests Orientalist tropes—present and prevalent long before 9/11—are key drivers in how Americans evaluate Muslims (Oskooii et al. Reference Oskooii, Dana and Barreto2019). The aftermath of 9/11 merely made these tropes more salient and solidified in the mind of Americans (Dana et al. Reference Dana, Lajevardi, Oskooii and Walker2018). Although it may seem Orientalism is more suited as a framework for former colonial countries to understand their role in colonialism—thereby adjacent to the U.S. context—research has shown that Americans hold and employ Orientalist stereotypes when thinking about Muslims (Oskooii et al. Reference Oskooii, Dana and Barreto2019). This means that not only was the influence of Orientalism far-reaching, but it has been long-lasting, as well. As a result, the American public has been shown to view Muslims as “culturally inferior, uncivilized, and out of touch with modern social and democratic norms” (Oskooii et al. Reference Oskooii, Dana and Barreto2019, p. 3). These stereotypes are linked with the racialization of Muslims.

In addition to being perceived as cultural outsiders, another reason to suspect Muslim identity may be more salient for social belonging relative to race is because of the racialization of Islam. That is, Muslim identity, although one of religion, is often thought of as a racial category. Scholars have focused on the racialization of many identity groups, including the racialization of Muslims (Al-Saji Reference Al-Saji2014; Aziz Reference Aziz2022; Bayoumi Reference Bayoumi2006; Beydoun Reference Beydoun2013; Considine Reference Considine2017; Fourlas Reference Fourlas2015; Galonnier Reference Galonnier2015; Garner, and Selod Reference Garner and Selod2015; Jamal, and Sinno Reference Jamal and Sinno2009; Meer Reference Meer2013). The racialization of religion occurs when “religious beliefs and practices of the adherents are associated with cultural traits, which in turn are surrogates for biological traits” (Aziz Reference Aziz2022, p. 20). This means that Islam is no longer seen as a religious practice, protected under the First Amendment (Aziz Reference Aziz2022; Garner, and Selod Reference Garner and Selod2015; Gotanda Reference Gotanda2011, Reference Gotanda2017). Rather, religious identity is seen as an immutable trait.

Because of the racialization of Islam, Islamophobia has little to do with religious beliefs but with perceptions of those who are Muslims: “In a religious conflict, it is not who you are but what you believe that is important. Under a racist regime, there is no escape from who you are (or are perceived to be by the power elite)” (Bayoumi Reference Bayoumi2006, p. 275). Therefore, Muslim individuals’ racial background matters little when they are being evaluated; all that matters is that the individual is Muslim. Indeed, research on White American converts to Islam has found this to be true. White American Muslims are often assumed to be immigrants and often met with tropes associated with Muslims (Husain Reference Husain2019). This means that although there are many Muslims from all ethnoracial backgrounds, the process of racialization has made it difficult to disentangle religious beliefs, something that is (largelyFootnote 4 ) a choice and mutable, from an ethnoracial identity, which is immutable. Thus, we test whether Muslim identity is prioritized over other identity characteristics when determining who belongs.

H1: Muslim immigrants will be considered less likely to belong in the United States regardless of their race, holding all other features constant.

Are MENA and Muslim identities understood together?

In contrast to Muslim identity, being the most salient identity characteristic for belonging is the argument that MENA-Muslim identity could be understood intersectionally. Therefore, only those who fit into the MENA-Muslim category will be excluded, while other Muslim racial identities could viewed as belonging. As mentioned, Muslim identity is not a racial identity, yet it is regularly discussed in relation to MENA racial identity (Khan, and Ecklund, Reference Khan and Ecklund2012; Lajevardi Reference Lajevardi2020; Nielsen, and Allen Reference Nielsen and Allen2002). For instance, when discussing “Muslims” it is often apparent the scope is specifically MENA-Muslims. This is argued to occur because individuals tend to view Muslims as monolithic (Khan, and Ecklund, Reference Khan and Ecklund2012; McCarus Reference McCarus1994; Nyang Reference Nyang1999). It is not, therefore, surprising that negative sentiments are present toward both Arabs/MENA individuals and Muslims (Kteily et al. Reference Kteily, Bruneau, Waytz and Cotterill2015). And this is by no means a new trend. For instance, polls conducted in 1991 and 1993 showed that Americans viewed Arabs as “religious fanatics” (Cainkar Reference Cainkar2009), indicating that Americans blur the lines between the racial category and religious affiliation. The blurred line between the racial and religious category is best understood through an intersectional framework.

The theory of intersectionality emphasizes the understanding of identity as being formed of multiple features, all of which occur simultaneously (Collins Reference Collins1991; Crenshaw Reference Crenshaw1989, Reference Crenshaw1990; Hancock Reference Hancock2016). That is, intersectionality theory is “interested in how the differential situatedness of different social agents affects the ways they affect and are affected by different social, economic and political projects” (Yuval-Davis Reference Yuval-Davis2006, p. 4). This means that people with particular combinations of features (e.g., gender and race or race and religion) may be subjected to unique lived experiences that cannot be captured by one identity or even the addition of two identities (Hancock Reference Hancock2007). Considering this framework—that White Americans commonly group MENA and Muslim as overlapping categories—means that Muslims from different racial backgrounds may be viewed differently. Thus, theories of intersectionality help explain the dynamics of power and discrimination along racial and religious lines; when evaluating who belongs are these identities evaluated together or as distinct considerations?

A Muslim religious identity then intersects with other features that are historically marginalized, “especially with respect to gender and religion (e.g., hijabi women), or race and religion (e.g., African American Muslims)” (Lajevardi (Reference Lajevardi2020, p. 11). Often “Muslim” is used to refer to anyone from the Middle Eastern or North Africa, regardless of their religion. At the same time, Muslim identity is often not thought of as, including East Asian-, Black-, or Latin Americans. As Hussain (Reference Husain2019) shows, Black and African American Muslims are often not thought of as being Muslim at all. One of her interviewees remarks: “No matter how I felt about my identity, Muslim or not, I’ve been treated like a black dude” (Husain Reference Husain2019, p. 594). This suggests that in the mind of Americans, Muslim identity feels inherently tied to the MENA race. Black-Muslims and MENA-Muslims do not have the same positionality or experiences merely because they are Muslim. Rather, the intersections of Muslim identity with MENA identity produce different life experiences for these different groups.

Recent empirical findings also lend credence to the idea that perhaps Americans understand Muslim identity incompletely, conflating it with a racial or geographic identity. Although MENA individuals are legally classified as White in the United States, White Americans do not firmly place MENA individuals into the category of Whiteness. D’Urso (Reference d’Urso2022) shows that White Americans use both country of origin and religious cues when operationalizing who is White. While these two traits additively constitute assignment as White, those who were MENA-Muslims were perceived to have darker skin pigmentation relative to those who are either Muslim and White, MENA and Christian, or both Christian and White. This suggests Muslim MENA identity could be understood best from a perspective of intersectionality, because the two identities together are perceived as a part of the cultural stereotype of darker-skinned individuals, than any one trait on its own.

One drawback of this study is, however, that there is no assessment of the consequence of this identity. That is, although White Americans may not use religion alone as a proxy to assign racial categories to others, do they still evaluate Muslims differentially based on ethnoracial background? Perhaps negative sentiment toward Muslims is directed toward the prototype of a MENA-Muslim, rather than any Muslim. Race and religion, together, may play a role in determining who belongings, in addition to religion; thus, only MENA-Muslims would be precluded from belonging.

H2: Muslim immigrants from the Middle East or North Africa will be thought to belong in the United States less than Muslim immigrants from other parts of the world, holding all other features constant.

Design and method

Experiments using conjoint designs have become more prevalent within political science (Hainmueller et al. Reference Hainmueller, Hopkins and Yamamoto2014, Reference Hainmueller, Hangartner and Yamamoto2015; Hainmueller, and Hopkins Reference Hainmueller and Hopkins2015; Horiuchi et al. Reference Horiuchi, Markovich and Yamamoto2022). Similar to a full factorial design, conjoint experiments allow scholars to understand multidimensional preferences people have when making choices based on hypothetical profiles. A conjoint design allows for more attributes to be compared without having traditional issues of power in full factorial designs.Footnote 5 In this case, a conjoint design is an appropriate design, because we are interested in understanding the multidimensional preferences of immigrant belonging. The study and our hypotheses were preregistered.Footnote 6 With our design, we had sufficient power to detect effect sizes as small as 0.05% changes with 86% power and 95% confidence intervals (Lukac, and Stefanelli Reference Lukac and Stefanelli2020). Appendix 3 provides our power calculation.

In this study, White respondents were given two immigrant profiles with several descriptive attributes. Each profile contained five attributes: education, gender, English proficiency, religion, and country of origin.Footnote 7 Table 1 lists the attributes on which each immigrant profile varies. Education, gender, and English proficiency are all attributes shown to affect attitudes toward migrantsFootnote 8 and are included to both ground the experiment in what might be considered relevant considerations and work as a check of internal validity, as well. Education (Hainmueller et al. Reference Hainmueller, Hopkins and Yamamoto2014; Hainmueller, and Hiscox Reference Hainmueller and Hiscox2010; Scheve, and Slaughter Reference Scheve and Slaughter2001) as well as “American identity”—of which English proficiency is a key feature—are both relevant characteristics with well-known responses (Hainmueller et al. Reference Hainmueller, Hopkins and Yamamoto2014; Schildkraut Reference Schildkraut2010; Wong Reference Wong2010; Wright, and Citrin Reference Wright and Citrin2011). Thus, our expectation, consistent with prior literature, is that education attributes and English proficiency with the expectation that higher levels of education and increased English fluency will be viewed more positively.

Table 1. Attributes and levels

*Implied attribute based on country of origin, not explicitly asked.

The attributes that test our hypotheses are the religion and country of origin attributes. Elements for the religion category include the three Abrahamic religions of Judaism, Christianity, and Islam. With the country-of-origin attribute, we create a proxy for race by signaling the region a migrant is from. We selected multiple countries of origin per region, as specific countries of origin have been shown to influence the acceptance of immigrants (Brader et al. Reference Brader, Valentino and Suhay2008; Citrin et al. Reference Citrin, Green, Muste and Wong1997). Bosnia and Russia are Eastern Europe, Lebanon, Iran, and Libya are the Middle East and North Africa, Sudan is Black, and India and Pakistan are South Asia.Footnote 9 The four regions also work to proxy race/ethnicity: Bosnian and Russians are Eastern European and White; Lebanese, Iranians, and Libyans are MENA; Sudanese are Black; and Indians and Pakistanis are South Asian. The treatment bundles race and country of origin as a proxy, but we argue this is the strongest approach for our study. First, White Americans use country of origin to assess race/ethnicity (d’Urso Reference d’Urso2022). Second, signaling race with an image, for example, presents additional challenges with the study of an ethnoracial categorization such as MENA and Muslim because cultural or religious garments signal more than simply stating the religion or race/ethnicity alone. For instance, the choice to wear a head covering by women is both regional and religious, and the design of the head covering differs by both region and culture (see Monkebayeva et al. Reference Monkebayeva, Baitenova and Mustafayevа2012). Third, White Americans appear to readily connect a country of origin with a racial background. Two supplementary studies presented in Appendix 1 demonstrate that White Americans assign racial categories via country of origin that is consistent with a country or origin proxy for race. That is, respondents grouped the eight countries of origin into the four abovementioned racial groups: White, MENA, Black, and South Asian. Finally, our country selection process balanced additional country information as much as possible.

While the country-of-origin proxy may signal more information because international relations between the United States and each country are not the same, we also held constant the envisioned country-of-origin for each respondent by describing a country of origin. We selected countries based on the known populations of Christian, Jewish, and Muslim immigrants of which most respondents should be aware. For each region, we also chose one country with a positive or neutral relationship with the United States, and another with a negative or contentious relationship with the United States. For example, although Bosnians and Russians are both considered racially White, the United States has a tense relationship with Russia, relative to Bosnia. The same is for Pakistan relative to India. As a result, we controlled for country of origin while also recognizing potential variation in evaluations that may be due to how well-liked or disliked individuals from a given country may be. After fielding, we also did not find evidence of within-region differences for country of origin as demonstrated in Appendix 5.

Table 2 provides an example of what respondents saw when given the immigrant profiles. Respondents were assigned a pair of immigrant profiles, with a full factorial design. After looking at both profiles, respondents were asked two questions which became our dependent variables. First, respondents were asked to select to which immigrant they thought the United States should give a green card. Although there are many types of visas, they are all temporary to varying degrees. Green cards, on the other hand, are more permanent and allow for a pathway to naturalization, and we believe this better captures our aim of understanding which characteristics White Americans value for belonging. Second, respondents were asked how likely the immigrant is to assimilate into U.S. culture. While the first DV is a forced choice, the second is a Likert scale asked for each immigrant profile shown. Respondents repeated this task four times so that they saw five-paired profiles with subsequent questions.

Table 2. Example of immigrant profiles given during conjoint task

The respondent i chooses among immigrant profiles k in task j. This function is modeled as a vector containing the attributes of the immigrant profiles presented to the respondent for each task. Because each respondent provides us with j × k observations, we use respondent cluster robust standard errors. With two profiles over five iterations, each respondent provides 10 rows of observations. Each row represents one profile as well as the attributes that the respondent was exposed to for that profile.Footnote 10

Analyses of conjoint designs allow us to consider multidimensional preference. We are interested in the marginal means of attributes of immigrants on belonging. We report marginal means instead of the Average Marginal Component Effect (AMCE) because the marginal means are the “level of favorability toward profiles that have a particular feature level, ignoring all other features” (Leeper et al. Reference Leeper, Hobolt and Tilley2020, p. 209). For instance, in a force choice design, the marginal mean corresponds directly to the probability a given attribute level is selected. We have also included the AMCE figures of our main findings in Appendix 4 and tables of the AMCEs and robustness check in Appendix 8.

Data and findings

This study was conducted on Qualtrics and fielded to 600 White, non-Latinx respondents by a quota-based sample from Bovitz, Inc Forthright panel from August 12 2019 to August 24 2019.Footnote 11 Demographic characteristics of the sample are available in Appendix 2. Our findings support prior literature on evaluations of immigrants. More educated immigrants, with more English fluency, and are women, are more likely to be selected to belong in the United States. Moreover, we find support for hypothesis 1 and partial elucidation of how the intersectionality of race and religion influences belonging. That is, regardless of race (proxied by country of origin), White Americans are more likely to exclude immigrants from the United States who are Muslim. Respondents also tend to exclude based on religion beyond race: if an immigrant is both White and Muslim, White respondents are more likely to exclude them. However, we find that although White Americans believe all Muslims are less likely to assimilate into American culture, on average, Muslim MENA immigrants are rated the lowest.

As described above, we present the results using country of origin as a proxy for race. We feel comfortable using this proxy for two reasons. First, two manipulation checks that show respondents tend to infer the proxied races from these countries (discussed further in Appendix 1, Studies 1 and 2). Second, geopolitical information does not seem to affect the results. While our intent with the treatment was to balance U.S. relations within region, we also find no statistical difference between the two countries in each region. Analysis of the results by country can be found in Appendix 5.

Figure 1 displays the main effects of the experiment, with the features—grouped by attribute—displayed on the y-axis and the x-axis displaying the marginal means (tables of the marginal means are provided in Appendix 6). This figure shows the attributes associated with respondents selecting or avoiding a given immigrant profile for a green card. In this image, the features are centered around 0.5 because the question was a forced choice: selecting either Immigrant 1 or Immigrant 2 to receive a green card. Attributes that do not overlap with 0.5 indicates that the attribute was selected or avoided at a rate that statistically significantly differs from random. We can also compare within attributes to see which characteristics, or levels, were preferred.

Figure 1. Who Should be given a Green Card?

Note: Figure 1 contains the marginal means for each group. The bars indicate 95% confidence interval. Immigrants with higher education and English fluency who are female, Jewish, Christian, White, or South Asian are more likely to be selected. Less educated, men, with less fluency, and immigrants who are Muslim, Middle Eastern, or Black are less likely to be selected.

First, profiles with higher levels of English proficiency, higher levels of education, and women cause a more positive response among respondents. Further, each subsequent degree is preferred relative to the degree below it at statistically significant levels. These results are consistent with prior literature and gives us confidence in the external validity of the treatment. Relative to elementary education alone, those with master’s degrees were 38.1 percentage points more likely to be selected (p<0.01). Those with college ( $\bar x$ = 0.57, p<0.01) and high school ( $\bar x$ = 0.42, p<0.01) were also more likely to be selected relative to those with only elementary education ( $\bar x$ = 0.31. Relative to male immigrants, female immigrants were 6.5 percentage points (p<0.01) likely to be selected. Moreover, those who had fluent ( $\bar x$ = 0.53, p<0.01) or advanced ( $\bar x$ = 0.53, p<0.01) English fluency were more likely to be selected relative to those with only intermediate fluency ( $\bar x$ = 0.43). Consistency with prior experiments, these findings point to the strong internal validity of this study.

Race and religion, the focal point of this study, are featured at the bottom of Figure 1. Relative to Christians, Muslims were selected 13.1 percentage points less often (p<0.01). Respondents are 54.95% likely to select profiles if the hypothetical immigrant is Christian; however, they are only 41.73% likely to select the profile of a hypothetical immigrant who is Muslim. The effect was the third largest after immigrants with master’s degrees and college degrees. There was no statistical difference between Jewish immigrants relative to Christian immigrants. The results from race show that relative to White immigrants, MENA immigrants were selected 4.0 percentage points less often (p<0.05). The effect size for MENA immigrants was the smallest. Black and South Asian immigrants were not selected at rates statistically distinguishable from White immigrants.

In Figure 2, we present the effect of immigrants’ attributes on the perceived likelihood they are thought to assimilate into American culture on a four-point scale. Again, female identity, higher levels of education, and increased English fluency all lead to higher assessments of cultural assimilation. Immigrants with more education are rated as more likely to assimilate into American culture relative to elementary school education. And each subsequent degree is preferred relative to the degree below it at statistically significant levels. Relative to elementary education alone, those with master’s degrees were rated as 7.0 percentage points more likely to assimilate (p<0.01). Those with college ( $\bar x$ = 0.72, p<0.01) and high school ( $\bar x$ = 0.70, p<0.01) were also rated as more likely to assimilate relative to those with only elementary education ( $\bar x$ = 0.66). Relative to male immigrants, female immigrants were rated 3.0 percentage points (p<0.01) more likely to assimilate. Moreover, those who had advanced (b = 0.025, p<0.01) English proficiency were rated as more likely to assimilate relative to those with intermediate English proficiency. Collectively, these results are consistent with prior results on the preferences of immigrants’ profiles.

Figure 2. Who will Assimilate into American Culture?

Note: Figure 2 contains the marginal means for each group. The bars indicate 95% confidence interval. Immigrants with more education who are Christian or Jewish are more likely be evaluated as assimilating into American culture. High school educated or Muslim immigrants are less likely to be evaluated as assimilating into American culture.

Both Muslim ( $\bar x$ = 0.64, p<0.01) and MENA ( $\bar x$ = 0.69, p<0.05) are statistically significantly less likely to be rated as assimilating to American culture relative to their respective baselines: Christian ( $\bar x$ = 0.73) and White ( $\bar x$ = 0.72). Notably, whereas the largest substantive effect on green cards given was education, in this case, Muslim identity is the largest substantive effect on assimilation. This suggests that White Americans may view Muslims as cultural outsiders, consistent with Kalkan et al. (Reference Kalkan, Layman and Uslaner2009). Even with smaller effect sizes, however, MENA immigrants are also not seen as cultural insiders, regardless of their religion.

Thus, both MENA and Muslim identity matter; however, the effect size of Muslim identity is one of the largest, while the effect size for MENA immigrants was the smallest across our two dependent variables. To get a fuller understanding of our hypotheses, we interact religion and race. To find support for hypothesis 2, MENA-Muslims should be penalized relative to Muslims from any other background.

In hypothesis 2, we state that migrant profiles who fit a prototype of being a Muslim Middle Easterner or Muslim North African would be more likely to be excluded than those with other combinations of race and religion. To reject our null, we would need to find heterogeneity wherein only the MENA-Muslim would be penalized, but not the White, Black, or South Asian Muslim. Figure 3 and Figure 4, below, present these findings conditional on Christians. That is, each figure includes the Jewish and Muslim profiles relative to the Christian profile. We have also included these figures conditional upon race (relative to White) in Appendix 7. However, for ease of interpretation, we plot the means conditional upon religion, below.

Figure 3. Who Should be given a Green Card Conditional on Religion?

Note: Figure 3 contains the marginal means for each group conditional on religion. The bars indicate 95% confidence interval. Across every attribute, Muslims are less likely to be selected for green cards relative to Christians.

Figure 4. Who will Assimilate into American Culture Conditional on Religion?

Note: Figure 4 contains the marginal means for each group conditional on religion. The bars indicate 95% confidence interval. Across every attribute, Muslims are less likely to be evaluated as. Assimilating into American culture relative to Christians.

As seen in Figure 3, below, there is no heterogeneity based on the race of the Muslim immigrant on the likelihood of a green card given. If there was heterogeneity, we would expect to see differences between those who were Muslim by ethnoracial background; however, we see Muslims of all ethnoracial backgrounds are excluded at similar rates relative to the Christian immigrant baseline. Although Muslim immigrants are excluded relative to Christians across attributes, generally, Jewish individuals are not excluded at rates that differ relative to Christians. There are a few attributes that do differ, however. Female, college-educated, Jewish immigrants are excluded relative to Christian immigrants at a statistically significant level of p < 0.1. Moreover, Middle Eastern Jewish immigrants are also excluded relative to Middle Eastern Christians. This difference is also statistically significant at a p < 0.1 level. Although these significance levels are a bit above the convention of p < 0.05, it suggests there could be intersectional considerations between religion and racial categories for Jewish individuals.

Returning to hypothesis 1, however, Muslim immigrants were excluded at rates that were statistically significantly different relative to Christians across all attributes and levels. This provides support for hypothesis 1 over hypothesis 2; Muslim immigrants are excluded from green cards irrespective of race relative to Christians regardless of their race.

Next, we test whether we see whether the intersection of MENA-Muslim identity influences assimilation. In Figure 4, below, there is an effect of MENA-Muslim identity on assimilation. Black Muslims ( $\bar x$ = −0.111, p<0.05), South Asian Muslims ( $\bar x$ = −0.106, p<0.05), and MENA-Muslims ( $\bar x$ = −0.120, p<0.01) are rated as less likely to assimilate to U.S. culture relative to White Muslims ( $\bar x$ = −0.056). After Elementary education only ( $\bar x$ = −0.122), the largest relative substantive effect on the likelihood for respondents to say the immigrant in the profile shown would not be likely to assimilate into American culture are immigrants who are MENA and Muslim. Thus, there is some heterogeneity where White Muslims are rated as more likely to assimilate into American culture relative to Muslims from other racial backgrounds (this effect can also be seen in Appendix 4, Figure 10).

Together, the findings in Figures 3 and 4 demonstrate consistent findings. The results from Figure 4 indicate that while there are no racial differences based on religious identifiers, respondents do differentiate between White Muslims and Black, MENA, and South Asian Muslims as less likely to assimilate into American culture. Thus, Muslims are evaluated as not being as likely to assimilate relative to Christians at levels that are highly statistically significant (p < 0.01). However, a closer examination shows an additional pattern within racial groups for the Muslim immigrants. White Muslims are evaluated as being more likely to assimilate relative to Middle Eastern Muslims. This suggests that within Muslim racial identities, respondents differentiate the extent of belonging in American society. Overall, however, the preference is for immigrants from another religion, regardless of race. Despite this distinction between White and MENA Muslims, the Muslim identifier alone is sufficient for individuals to be more likely for respondents to exclude them from American society.

Conclusion

Research on Islamophobia and Muslim identity in the United States and around the world has increasingly become an area of interest in research on race and ethnic politics in the United States (Adida et al. Reference Adida, Lo and Platas2019; Beydoun Reference Beydoun2013; Lajevardi Reference Lajevardi2017, Reference Lajevardi2020; Lajevardi, and Oskooii Reference Lajevardi and Oskooii2018; Oskooii et al. Reference Oskooii, Dana and Barreto2019). However, the American public and even the media often conflate Middle Easterners and North Africans with Muslims, and vice versa. We ask, given that Islam is a religious, rather than an ethnoracial category, does the White American public understand the nuances between these two categories?

We explore the relationship between Muslim identity on belonging. Although there are multiple dimensions from which we can study belonging, we focus on societal belonging. That is, who does society at large believe belongs in the United States? Here, we evaluate the role of religion and present two competing frameworks, which we empirically test using a conjoint experiment. We find that Muslim identity is more salient relative to racial identity considerations when White Americans evaluate who belongs in the United States. Muslims are evaluated as a monolithic group and the race of any given Muslim individual is irrelevant.

Although Muslim and MENA identities are often conflated with each other—that is, Muslim MENA identity is not merely an addition of racial and religious characteristics, but an identity with different experiences than Muslims who may also be Black, White, or Asian. But we find that White Americans do not discriminate against Muslim MENA individuals differently than White, Black, or South Asian Muslims.

These findings have a number of practical implications. First, research has shown that religion and country of origin together can alter perceptions of others’ ethnoracial identity (d’Urso Reference d’Urso2022). Although religion may alter perceptions along with country of origin, religion plays a singular role in how individuals are evaluated by society. Muslim Americans are a diverse ethnoracial group that experiences monolithic discrimination. And recent research shows that is how Muslims experience personal belonging in the United States. While Muslims feel “at home” in America, they are not welcomed by society at large (Chouhoud Reference Chouhoud and Stockemer2022). Thus, researchers on Muslims and Islamophobia should be careful to think through the implications of any findings if they are treating Muslims as a proxy for MENA individuals alone. Here, we find that discrimination toward Muslim inclusion reaches beyond the prototype of Muslim MENA individuals, meaning Islamophobia may be a framework that extends to more individuals, such as converts or “model minorities” who are Muslim than otherwise thought.

Second, the evidence here provides additional rationale to other research demonstrating that MENA should be categorized separately from White in the U.S. Census. Although there are certainly exclusionary attitudes toward Muslims across all races, there are also unique attitudes toward MENA individuals in our experimental treatments. Although other studies provide evidence that the category should be separated because of different social conditions that may go unnoticed (e.g., Jonny Reference Jonny2020; Strmic-Pawl et al. Reference Strmic-Pawl, Jackson and Garner2018), this follows others (e.g., Beydoun Reference Beydoun2015a; Kayyali Reference Kayyali2013) in demonstrating that the experience of MENA individuals in the United States—regardless or religious background— is distinct from that of others considered to be White Americans.

There are a number of ways this work can and should be expanded in the future. For example, there is work showing the disconnect between personal belonging of Muslims in the United States and how accepted they are by American society (Chouhoud Reference Chouhoud and Stockemer2022). Future work can include whether this experience is moderated by race. This study indicates White Americans believe White Muslims will be better at assimilating into American culture. Is this how White Muslims feel? Moreover, White Americans believing White Muslims are more likely to assimilate does not mean they will be more accepting of White Muslims relative to Muslims from other races.

Another area for expansion is to have respondents from different ethnoracial backgrounds—not merely White Americans. We selected White Americans because MENA individuals are legally classified in the United States and our questions about inclusion considered the racial hierarchy.Footnote 12 We see in the case of green cards; race did not matter when also presented with religious information. However, we see MENA-Muslims were the least likely to be rated as able to assimilate and White Muslims the most. And, since norms of racial equality differ by audience race (Bonilla et al. Reference Bonilla, Filindra and Lajevardi2022), future research should investigate if race is a more salient factor for non-White Americans.

Last, we focus specifically on the case of MENA-Muslim identity in the United States. The relationship between the racialization of Muslims and MENA individuals in the United States is not necessarily applicable to the experiences of Muslims and MENA individuals in different country contexts. For example, in the UK, the conflation may be between South Asians and Muslims (e.g., Abbas Reference Abbas2004); in France, it may be for Afro-Muslims (e.g., Adida et al. Reference Adida, Laitin and Marie-Anne2016). Future research might investigate the roles of religion and race and the intersectionality of the two in other country contexts.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/rep.2023.7

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank James Druckman, Alvin B. Tillery Jr., J. Seawright, S. R. Gubitz, Kumar Ramanathan, Matthew D. Nelsen, Margaret Brower, and Youssef Chouhoud for comments on early drafts. We also thank S. R. Gubitz for insights on coding the survey. Finally, we thank Georgia Caras and Michelle Sheinker for their excellent research assistance.

Funding Declaration

The data collection effort was funded through the Center for the Study of Diversity and Democracy at Northwestern University.

Ethical Statement

The study was approved by the NU Institutional Review Board (#) and was deemed exempt.

Footnotes

1 Country of origin is often used as a heuristic to determine a person’s race—although the relationship can work in the opposite direction, as well. For example, someone from Japan may typically be assigned Asian, whereas someone from Ghana would typically be assigned Black.

2 Social belonging is the meso-level of three dimensions of belonging. The macro-level—institutional belonging— includes policies such as barriers for entry or inclusion criteria, citizenship, naturalization, and other forms of documentation related to who can be in and contribute to a given country (Masuoka, and Junn, Reference Masuoka and Junn2013). At the microlevel—individual belonging—includes how one feels they fit into their country or country of residence. In this case, “belonging is about an emotional…attachment, about feeling ‘at home’” (Yuval-Davis Reference Yuval-Davis2006, p. 10).

3 Research focuses also on economic assimilation—if migrants will build up the local economy (e.g., Malhotra et al. Reference Malhotra, Margalit and Mo2013; Mayda Reference Mayda2006)—but our focus here is more on the cultural and religious aspect of assimilation because we are examining identity rather than economic contributions in this line of inquiry.

4 For those in collectivistic religious communities, religion may feel like less of a choice (Jakelić Reference Jakelić2010). Nonetheless, we argue that religious is still different than a racial categorization.

5 An added benefit of conjoints is that it mitigates social desirability bias on the part of respondents (Horiuchi et al., Reference Horiuchi, Markovich and Yamamoto2022).

6 This study was preregistered at AsPredicted. Please see the anonymized submission description in Appendix 9.

7 While it is sometimes the case that researchers signal racial identity with an image, in this case, there are many complications with doing so. An image may reflect additional considerations in the way that religious preferences might be interpreted in different countries (head coverings for instance tend to vary by region and by sect), as well as how respondents may interpret lack of (or addition of) a religious covering within assimilation itself. Due to the potential additional variations, we might have encountered, and we decided to forego images altogether.

8 Previous research has found country of origin, education, language fluency, skin tone, and job skills each matter in determining immigrant preferences (see Adida et al., 2010; Brader et al., Reference Brader, Valentino and Suhay2008; Hainmueller et al., Reference Hainmueller, Hopkins and Yamamoto2014; Hainmueller & Hiscox, Reference Hainmueller and Hiscox2010; Hainmueller & Hopkins, Reference Hainmueller and Hopkins2015; Harell et al., Reference Harell, Soroka, Iyengar and Valentino2012; Hopkins, Reference Hopkins2014; Ostfeld, Reference Ostfeld2017; Schildkraut, Reference Schildkraut2011; Sniderman et al., 2004, Reference Sniderman, Hagendoorn and Prior2004; Valentino & Iyengar, Reference Valentino and Iyengar2011; Wright & Citrin, Reference Wright and Citrin2011).

9 There is no formal definition of which countries belong in the MENA. Although some consider Sudan as a part of North African, most do not. Using two additional samples, we determine Sudanese are not evaluated as being MENA, rather they are evaluated as Black. However, Libyans, Iranians, and Lebanese are evaluated as being MENA. These analyses are available in Appendix 1.

10 For table of frequency of attributes shown, see Appendix 3.

11 Due to several studies demonstrating that online survey panels tend to replicate that of more expensive data collection firms (e.g., Berinsky et al., Reference Berinsky, Huber and Lenz2012; Coppock, Reference Coppock2019) and increasing use of the Bovitz, Inc Forthright panel (e.g., Druckman et al., Reference Druckman, Klar, Krupnikov, Levendusky and Ryan2022; Landry et al., Reference Landry, Schooler, Willer and Seli2023; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Lelkes, Hawkins and Theodoridis2022), we are reasonably confident in the reliability of this panel for research.

12 The U.S. Federal Government via the Office of Budget and Management classifies those of MENA descent as racially White and has done so since 1978. That means that under the legal categorization of race in the United States, MENA individuals are White.

References

Abbas, T (2004) After 9/11 British South Asian Muslims, Islamophobia, multiculturalism, and the state. American Journal of Islamic Social Sciences 24, 2638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adida, CL, Lo, A and Platas, MR (2019) Americans preferred Syrian Refugees who are female, English-Speaking, and Christian on the Eve of Donald Trump’s election. PloS One 14, e0222504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adida, CL, Laitin, DD and Marie-Anne, V (2016) Why Muslim Integration Fails in Christian-Heritage Societies. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Al-Saji, A (2014) A phenomenology of hesitation: Interrupting racializing habits of seeing. In Emily Lee (Ed.), Living Alterities: Phenomenology, Embodiment, and Race. State University of New York Press. pp. 133–172.Google Scholar
Allport, GW (1954) The Nature of Prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Arora, M (2020) The Racial Muslim: When Racism Quashes Religious Freedom. Oakland, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Aziz, S (2022) The Racial Muslim (1st ed.). University of California Press; JSTOR. https://doi.org/10.2307/j.ctv21r3hzg Google Scholar
Bayoumi, M (2006) Racing religion. CR: The New Centennial Review 6, 267293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berinsky, AJ, Huber, GA and Lenz, GS (2012) Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon. com’s mechanical turk. Political Analysis 20(3), 351–368.10.1093/pan/mpr057CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beydoun, KA (2013) Between Muslim and White: The legal construction of Arab American identity. Annual Survey of American Law 69, 29.Google Scholar
Beydoun, KA (2015a) A demographic threat: Proposed reclassification of Arab Americans on the 2020 census. Michigan Law Review 114, 1.Google Scholar
Beydoun, KA (2015b) Boxed in: Reclassification of Arab Americans on the US census as progress or peril. Loyola University Chicago Law Journal 47, 693.Google Scholar
Bonilla, T, Filindra, A and Lajevardi, N (2022) How source cues shape evaluations of group-based derogatory political messages. The Journal of Politics, 84(4), 1979–1996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonilla, T and Mo, CH (2018) Bridging the partisan divide on immigration policy attitudes through a bipartisan issue area: The case of human trafficking. Journal of Experimental Political Science 5, 107120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brader, T, Valentino, N and Suhay, E (2008) Is it immigration or the immigrants? The emotional influence of groups on public opinion and political action. American Journal of Political Science 52, 959978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cainkar, LA (2009) Homeland Insecurity: The Arab American and Muslim American Experience After 9/11. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Chouhoud, Y (2022) American Muslim Immigrants: Identity and Belonging in the Shadow of 9/11. In Stockemer, D. (Ed.), Muslims in the Western World: Sense of Belonging and Political Identity. Springer International Publishing, pp. 2946. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-99487-7_3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Citrin, J, Green, DP, Muste, C and Wong, C (1997) Public opinion toward immigration reform: The role of economic motivations. The Journal of Politics 59, 858881.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, PH (1991) Learning from the outsider within: the sociological significance of black feminist thought. Social Problems 33(6), s14–s32.Google Scholar
Considine, C (2017) The racialization of Islam in the United States: Islamophobia, hate crimes, and “Flying While Brown.” Religions 8, 165.10.3390/rel8090165CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coppock, A (2019) Generalizing from survey experiments conducted on Mechanical Turk: A replication approach. Political Science Research and Methods 7, 613628.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crenshaw, K (1989) Demarginalizing the intersection of race and sex: A black feminist critique of antidiscrimination doctrine, feminist theory and antiracist politics. University of Chicago Legal Forum, 139.Google Scholar
Crenshaw, K (1990) Mapping the margins: Intersectionality, identity politics, and violence against women of color. Stanford Law Review 43, 12411300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
d’Urso, AS (2022) A boundary of white inclusion: The role of religion in Ethnoracial assignment. Perspectives on Politics, 118. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1537592722003309 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dana, K, Lajevardi, N, Oskooii, KAR and Walker, HL (2018) Veiled politics: Experiences with discrimination among Muslim Americans. Politics and Religion, 149. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1755048318000287 Google Scholar
Druckman, JN, Klar, S, Krupnikov, Y, Levendusky, M and Ryan, JB (2022) (Mis) estimating affective polarization. The Journal of Politics 84, 11061117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Esaiasson, P, Lajevardi, N and Sohlberg, J (2022) Reject, Limbo, and accept: The effect of migration decisions on Asylum seekers. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 48(15), 3469–3483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Esposito, JL and Kalin, I (2011) Islamophobia: The Challenge of Pluralism in the 21st Century. New York, NY: OUP USA.Google Scholar
Fetzer, JS (2000) Public Attitudes toward Immigration in the United States, France, and Germany. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fourlas, GN (2015) Being a target: On the racialization of Middle Eastern Americans. Critical Philosophy of Race 3, 101123.10.5325/critphilrace.3.1.0101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galonnier, J (2015) The racialization of Muslims in France and the United States: Some insights from White Converts to Islam. Social Compass 62, 570583.10.1177/0037768615601966CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garner, S and Selod, S (2015) The racialization of Muslims: Empirical studies of Islamophobia. Critical Sociology 41, 919. https://doi.org/10.1177/0896920514531606 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gotanda, N (2011) The racialization of Islam in American Law. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 637, 184195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gotanda, N (2017) Reflecting on race, law and White Supremacy: Asian American and Muslim American experiences. Western State Law Review 45, 147.Google Scholar
Hainmueller, J, Hangartner, D and Yamamoto, T (2015) Validating vignette and conjoint survey experiments against real-world behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 112, 23952400. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1416587112 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hainmueller, J and Hiscox, MJ (2010) Attitudes toward highly skilled and low-skilled immigration: Evidence from a survey experiment. American Political Science Review 104, 6184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hainmueller, J and Hopkins, DJ (2015) The hidden American immigration consensus: A conjoint analysis of attitudes toward immigrants: THE HIDDEN AMERICAN IMMIGRATION CONSENSUS. American Journal of Political Science 59, 529548. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12138 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hainmueller, J, Hopkins, DJ and Yamamoto, T (2014) Causal inference in conjoint analysis: Understanding multidimensional choices via stated preference experiments. Political Analysis 22, 130. https://doi.org/10.1093/pan/mpt024 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hancock, A-M (2007) When multiplication doesn’t equal quick addition: Examining intersectionality as a research paradigm. Perspectives on Politics 5, 6379.10.1017/S1537592707070065CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hancock, A-M (2016) Intersectionality: An Intellectual History. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harell, A, Soroka, S, Iyengar, S and Valentino, N (2012) The impact of economic and cultural cues on support for immigration in Canada and the United States. Canadian Journal of Political Science/Revue Canadienne de Science Politique 45, 499530.10.1017/S0008423912000698CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hellwig, T and Sinno, A (2017) Different groups, different threats: Public attitudes towards immigrants. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 43, 339358. https://doi.org/10.1080/1369183X.2016.1202749 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobbs, W and Lajevardi, N (2019) Effects of divisive political campaigns on the day-to-day segregation of Arab and Muslim Americans. American Political Science Review 113, 270276. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055418000801 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, DJ (2014) One language, two meanings: Partisanship and responses to Spanish. Political Communication 31, 421445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horiuchi, Y, Markovich, ZD and Yamamoto, T (2022) Does conjoint analysis mitigate social desirability bias? Political Analysis 30(4), 535–549.10.1017/pan.2021.30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Husain, A (2019) Moving beyond (and Back to) the Black–White Binary: A study of Black and White Muslims’ racial positioning in the United States. Ethnic and Racial Studies 42, 589606.10.1080/01419870.2017.1410199CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huynh, Q-L, Devos, T and Smalarz, L (2011) Perpetual Foreigner in one’s own land: Potential implications for identity and psychological adjustment. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 30, 133162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jakelić, S (2010) Collectivistic Religions: Religion, Choice, and Identity in Late Modernity (1st ed.). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315572710.Google Scholar
Jamal, A and Sinno, AH (2009) The racialization of Muslim Americans. In Muslims in Western Politics, 200215. Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Jones, N. A. (2017). Update on the US Census Bureau’s race and ethnic research for the 2020 Census. Survey News, 3(5).Google Scholar
Jonny, S (2020) The erasure of Middle Eastern and North African immigrants from the American narrative: A case for adding a Mena category to the United States. Georgetown Immigration Law Journal 35, 1009.Google Scholar
Kalkan, KO, Layman, GC and Uslaner, EM (2009) “Bands of others”? Attitudes toward Muslims in contemporary American society. The Journal of Politics 71, 847862.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kayyali, R. (2013). US Census classifications and Arab Americans: Contestations and definitions of identity markers. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies, 39(8), 12991318.10.1080/1369183X.2013.778150CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khan, M and Ecklund, K (2012) Attitudes toward Muslim Americans Post-9/11. Journal of Muslim Mental Health 7(1), 1–16.Google Scholar
Kim, CJ (1999) The racial triangulation of Asian Americans. Politics & Society 27, 105138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, SY, Wang, Y, Deng, S, Alvarez, R and Li, J (2011) Accent, perpetual foreigner stereotype, and perceived discrimination as indirect links between English proficiency and depressive symptoms in Chinese American adolescents. Developmental Psychology 47, 289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kinder, DR and Kam, CD (2010) Us Against Them: Ethnocentric Foundations of American Opinion. University of Chicago Press. https://doi.org/10.5860/choice.47-7142 Google Scholar
Knoll, BR, Redlawsk, DP and Sanborn, H (2011) Framing labels and immigration policy attitudes in the Iowa Caucuses: “Trying to Out-Tancredo Tancredo.” Political Behavior 33, 433454. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-010-9141-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kteily, N, Bruneau, E, Waytz, A and Cotterill, S (2015) The ascent of man: Theoretical and empirical evidence for Blatant Dehumanization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 109, 901.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lajevardi, N (2017) A Comprehensive Study of Muslim American Discrimination by Legislators, the Media, and the Masses. UC San Diego.Google Scholar
Lajevardi, N (2020) Outsiders at Home: The Politics of American Islamophobia. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/9781108782814CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lajevardi, N and Oskooii, KAR (2018) Old-fashioned racism, contemporary Islamophobia, and the Isolation of Muslim Americans in the age of Trump. The Journal of Race, Ethnicity, and Politics 3, 112152. https://doi.org/10.1017/rep.2017.37 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landry, AP, Schooler, JW, Willer, R and Seli, P (2023). Reducing explicit blatant dehumanization by correcting exaggerated meta-perceptions. Social Psychological and Personality Science 14(4), 407–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, AH-Y, Lelkes, Y, Hawkins, CB and Theodoridis, AG (2022) Negative partisanship is not more prevalent than positive partisanship. Nature Human Behaviour 6, 951963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leeper, TJ, Hobolt, SB and Tilley, J (2020) Measuring subgroup preferences in conjoint experiments. Political Analysis 28, 207221.10.1017/pan.2019.30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lukac, M and Stefanelli, A (2020) Conjoint Experiments: Power Analysis Tool (Shiny App) [R].Google Scholar
Malhotra, N, Margalit, Y and Mo, CH (2013) Economic explanations for opposition to immigration: Distinguishing between prevalence and conditional impact. American Journal of Political Science 57, 391410. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masuoka, N and Junn, J (2013) The Politics of Belonging: Race, Public Opinion, and Immigration. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mathews, K, Phelan, J, Jones, NA, Konya, S, Marks, R, Pratt, BM, Coombs, J and Bentley, M (2017) National Content Test: Race and Ethnicity Analysis Report. 380.Google Scholar
Mayda, AM (2006) Who is against immigration? A cross-country investigation of individual attitudes toward immigrants. The Review of Economics and Statistics 88, 510530.10.1162/rest.88.3.510CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarus, EN (1994) The Development of Arab-American Identity. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meer, N (2013) Racialization and religion: Race, culture and difference in the study of Antisemitism and Islamophobia. Ethnic and Racial Studies 36, 385398.10.1080/01419870.2013.734392CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monkebayeva, KZ, Baitenova, NZ and Mustafayevа, AA (2012) History of appearance and distribution of hijab and its types. International Journal of Humanities and Social Sciences 6, 31193122.Google Scholar
Naber, N (2000) Ambiguous insiders: An investigation of Arab American invisibility. Ethnic and Racial Studies 23, 3761. https://doi.org/10.1080/014198700329123 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nielsen, J and Allen, C (2002) Summary Report on Islamophobia in the EU after 11 September 2001.Google Scholar
Nyang, SS (1999) Islam in the United States of America. Chicago, IL: ABC International Group, Inc.Google Scholar
Oskooii, KAR, Dana, K and Barreto, MA (2019) Beyond generalized ethnocentrism: IslamSpecific beliefs and prejudice toward Muslim Americans. Politics, Groups, and Identities, 128. https://doi.org/10.1080/21565503.2019.1623053 Google Scholar
Ostfeld, M (2017) The backyard politics of attitudes toward immigration: Backyard politics of attitudes toward immigration. Political Psychology 38, 2137. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12314 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peek, L (2005) Becoming Muslim: The development of a religious identity. Sociology of Religion 66, 215242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reny, TT and Barreto, MA (2022) Xenophobia in the time of pandemic: Othering, Anti-Asian Attitudes, and Covid-19. Politics, Groups, and Identities 10(2), 209–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romero, M (2008) Crossing the immigration and race border: A critical race theory approach to immigration studies. Contemporary Justice Review 11, 2337.10.1080/10282580701850371CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sáenz, R and Manges Douglas, K (2015) A call for the racialization of immigration studies: On the transition of ethnic immigrants to racialized immigrants. Sociology of Race and Ethnicity 1, 166180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Said, EW (1979) Orientalism. New York, NY: Vintage.Google Scholar
Scheve, KF and Slaughter, MJ (2001) Labor market competition and individual preferences over immigration policy. Review of Economics and Statistics 83, 133145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schildkraut, DJ (2010) Americanism in the Twenty-First Century: Public Opinion in the Age of Immigration. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schildkraut, DJ (2011) National identity in the United States. In Handbook of identity theory and research (pp. 845865). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sniderman, PM, Hagendoorn, L and Prior, M (2004) Predisposing factors and situational triggers: Exclusionary reactions to immigrant minorities. American Political Science Review 98, 3549. https://doi.org/10.1017/s000305540400098x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spruyt, B and Elchardus, M (2012) Are anti-Muslim feelings more widespread than anti-foreigner feelings? Evidence from two split-sample experiments. Ethnicities 12(6), 800820.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strmic-Pawl, HV, Jackson, BA and Garner, S (2018) Race counts: Racial and ethnic data on the US Census and the implications for tracking inequality. Sociology of Race and Ethnicity 4, 113.10.1177/2332649217742869CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tesler, M (2018) Elite domination of public doubts about climate change (not evolution). Political Communication 35, 306326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valentino, NA, Brader, T and Jardina, AE (2013) Immigration opposition among us Whites: General ethnocentrism or media priming of attitudes about Latinos? Political Psychology 34, 149166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valentino, NA and Iyengar, S (2011) Skin vs. skill: Exploring economic vs. racial threats as drivers of immigration opinion in the comparative context. Meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association. Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
Wong, CJ (2010) Boundaries of Obligation in American Politics: Geographic, National, and Racial Communities. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, M and Citrin, J (2011) Saved by the stars and stripes? Images of protest, salience of threat, and immigration attitudes. American Politics Research 39, 323343.10.1177/1532673X10388140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yuval-Davis, N (2006) Belonging and the politics of belonging. Patterns of Prejudice 40, 197214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Attributes and levels

Figure 1

Table 2. Example of immigrant profiles given during conjoint task

Figure 2

Figure 1. Who Should be given a Green Card?Note: Figure 1 contains the marginal means for each group. The bars indicate 95% confidence interval. Immigrants with higher education and English fluency who are female, Jewish, Christian, White, or South Asian are more likely to be selected. Less educated, men, with less fluency, and immigrants who are Muslim, Middle Eastern, or Black are less likely to be selected.

Figure 3

Figure 2. Who will Assimilate into American Culture?Note: Figure 2 contains the marginal means for each group. The bars indicate 95% confidence interval. Immigrants with more education who are Christian or Jewish are more likely be evaluated as assimilating into American culture. High school educated or Muslim immigrants are less likely to be evaluated as assimilating into American culture.

Figure 4

Figure 3. Who Should be given a Green Card Conditional on Religion?Note: Figure 3 contains the marginal means for each group conditional on religion. The bars indicate 95% confidence interval. Across every attribute, Muslims are less likely to be selected for green cards relative to Christians.

Figure 5

Figure 4. Who will Assimilate into American Culture Conditional on Religion?Note: Figure 4 contains the marginal means for each group conditional on religion. The bars indicate 95% confidence interval. Across every attribute, Muslims are less likely to be evaluated as. Assimilating into American culture relative to Christians.

Supplementary material: PDF

d’Urso and Bonilla supplementary material

d’Urso and Bonilla supplementary material

Download d’Urso and Bonilla supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 2.1 MB