Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T08:31:15.845Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Monosodium glutamate delivered in a protein-rich soup improves subsequent energy compensation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 August 2014

Una Masic*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9QH, UK
Martin R. Yeomans
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9QH, UK
*
*Corresponding author: Dr Una Masic, email [email protected]

Abstract

Previous research suggests that monosodium glutamate (MSG) may have a biphasic effect on appetite, increasing appetite within a meal with its flavour-enhancing effect, but enhancing subsequent satiety due to its proposed role as a predictor of protein content. The present study explored this by assessing the impact of a 450 g soup preload differing in MSG concentration (1 % MSG added (MSG+) or no MSG (MSG–)) and nutrient content (low-energy control or high-energy carbohydrate or high-energy protein) on rated appetite and ad libitum intake of a test meal in thirty-five low-restraint male volunteers using a within-participant design. Protein-rich preloads significantly reduced intake at the test meal and resulted in more accurate energy compensation than did carbohydrate-rich preloads. This energy compensation was stronger in the MSG+ protein conditions when compared with MSG+ carbohydrate conditions. No clear differences in rated appetite were seen in MSG or the macronutrient conditions alone during preload ingestion or 45 min after intake. Overall, these findings indicate that MSG may act to further improve energy compensation when provided in a protein-rich context.

Type
Behaviour, Appetite and Obesity
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
The online version of this article is published within an Open Access environment subject to the conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution license .
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2014

Monosodium glutamate (MSG) is a flavour enhancer that improves the savoury experience of foods and is the prototypical chemical associated with the ‘umami’ taste( Reference Ikeda 1 Reference Ninomiya 3 ). Because of its role as a flavour enhancer, it was initially believed that MSG increased appetite and intake( Reference Yeomans 4 Reference Bellisle, Tournier and Louis-Sylvestre 11 ). However, initial enhanced intake tends to decrease over time( Reference Schiffman, Sattely-Miller and Zimmerman 8 , Reference Essed, van Staveren and Kok 12– Reference Bellisle 14 ) and recent research suggests that MSG may also delay hunger recovery in the short term too, particularly when in combination with a food rich in protein( Reference Masic and Yeomans 15 ).

So why might any appetite-enhancing effects of MSG be short-lived? One explanation is that appetite is stimulated by MSG during the course of the meal (reducing satiation), but then suppressed during the post-ingestive stage to delay later intake (enhancing satiety). This suggestion is based on the idea that our ability to detect MSG evolved as a means of detecting the presence of protein in foods( Reference Ikeda 1 ) and regulating protein consumption( Reference Smriga and Torii 16 , Reference Laska and Hernandez Salazar 17 ). There is clear evidence that protein enhances satiety more effectively than does either carbohydrate( Reference Poppitt, McCormack and Buffenstein 18 Reference Fischer, Colombani and Wenk 20 ) or fat( Reference Weigle, Breen and Matthys 21 , Reference Simpson and Raubenheimer 22 ) when delivered acutely as a preload( Reference Bertenshaw, Lluch and Yeomans 23 , Reference Latner and Schwartz 24 ) and longer term in the diet( Reference Lejeune, Kovacs and Westerterp-Plantenga 25 Reference Simpson and Raubenheimer 27 ). Recent data suggest that the satiating effects of protein are in part due to the sensory characteristics of protein-rich foods acting to enhance post-ingestive satiety( Reference Bertenshaw, Lluch and Yeomans 28 ), and umami could be one of the critical cues generating these sensory–nutrient interactions. Indeed, the umami taste has been linked to the satiating effects of protein in infants( Reference Mennella and Beauchamp 29 , Reference Ventura, Beauchamp and Mennella 30 ) and may explain why intake of MSG meals over time remains stable despite increases in palatability. A previous study assessing the time course of changes in rated appetite over 120 min after consumption of a fixed volume of soup (high-energy carbohydrate-rich or protein-rich or low-energy control) with and without added MSG also supports these findings. Hunger decreased less after MSG soup intake (consistent with the stimulation of appetite through flavour enhancement), but hunger recovery was slower in the MSG protein-rich condition compared with the no-MSG protein-rich condition which was not seen for MSG in a low-energy or carbohydrate-rich context( Reference Masic and Yeomans 15 ). However, another study has found no effect of MSG and protein on satiety when measured at a test meal( Reference Luscombe-Marsh, Smeets and Westerterp-Plantenga 31 ). This discrepancy might be due to the previous study( Reference Masic and Yeomans 15 ) relying on measures of rated appetite at predefined time points whilst other research has assessed intake at ad libitum test meal sessions( Reference Luscombe-Marsh, Smeets and Westerterp-Plantenga 31 ).

Given this ambiguity in the literature about the role of MSG in appetite control, the present research examined the effects of a soup preload differing in specific macronutrient ratios (high-energy soups further enhanced with additional protein and carbohydrate contrasted with a low-energy control) either with MSG added (MSG+) or no MSG (MSG–) on appetite and intake at a subsequent ad libitum meal. A preload meal interval of 45 min was used as this was found to be the optimum time for differences in hunger and fullness ratings between MSG+ and MSG– conditions in our recent study( Reference Masic and Yeomans 15 ). Although MSG is not a nutritive substance, its potential influence on satiation and satiety may modify the response to nutrients, particularly when in combination with high ratios of a satiating nutrient such as protein. It was hypothesised that the flavour-enhancing effects of MSG would mean less of a decrease in hunger when consuming a fixed portion of the MSG+ versions regardless of nutrient content, but that hunger would recover more slowly, and consequently that test-meal intake would be less in the protein-rich than carbohydrate-rich preload conditions relative to the control, with MSG+ enhancing the satiating effects of protein.

Methods

Design

The study used a within-participant design to examine the effects of consumption of a fixed soup preload differing in specific nutrient ratios/energy (high-energy carbohydrate-rich or protein-rich or low-energy control) and MSG content (1 % (w/w) MSG (MSG+) or no MSG (MSG–)). Preload condition order was balanced using a Williams square design( Reference Williams 32 ).

Participants

A total of thirty-six low-restraint males initially participated in the research but one participant failed to complete all sessions. The thirty-five remaining participants (mean age: 21 (sd 0·4) years, range 18–28 years; mean BMI: 22 (sd 0·5) kg/m2, range 18–33 kg/m2) were students at the University of Sussex. Sample size was determined from our earlier study( Reference Masic and Yeomans 15 ) with effect size based on the maximal difference in rated hunger between the protein preload with and without added MSG, indicating that thirty-six participants would be required. Exclusion criteria included smoking, being on medications, a history of diabetes, diagnosed eating disorders, allergies or dietary intolerances to the foods used. Also, those with high restraint scores (ratings above 7 on the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire( Reference Stunkard and Messick 33 )) were excluded, as restrained individuals may not be representative of general eating behaviour( Reference Fedoroff, Polivy and Herman 34 , Reference Fedoroff, Polivy and Herman 35 ). Prospective participants were emailed with details of the study disguised as ‘assessing the effects of food on motor skills’ to minimise demand effects of the experimental manipulation. Written informed consent was given before participation and participants were paid £60 on completion. The study was conducted in accordance with the standards expressed in the Helsinki Declaration and was approved by the University of Sussex ethics committee.

Test food

Control breakfast

Breakfast on all test days consisted of 80 g cereal (Crunchy Nut Cornflakes; Kellogg's), 200 g semi-skimmed milk (Sainsbury's plc) and 200 g orange juice (Sainsbury's plc) (total 2107 kJ (503·6 kcal)). These quantities were established based on UK Food Standard Agency guidelines for male breakfast consumption( 36 ).

Soup preloads

All flavour and energy manipulations used the same low-energy density (ED) control soup, which was a carrot and spice soup containing carrots (Frozen Baby Carrots; Sainsbury's plc), onions, celery, olive oil (Medium Flavour Olive Oil; Sainsbury's plc), spice mixture (Garam Masala, Schwartz) and water (see Masic &Yeomans( Reference Masic and Yeomans 15 )). Portion size was fixed at 450 g, as this has been established as an adequate portion for males( Reference Yeomans and Gray 5 ) and was successful in our previous study( Reference Masic and Yeomans 15 ). Energy content was enhanced by the addition of 52 g/450 g portion maltodextrin (dextrose equivalent: 15·3; Cargill) in the carbohydrate soup and 17·86 g/450 g maltodextrin (Cargill) combined with 36 g/450 g whey protein isolate (MyProtein UK) to the protein soup. 1 % (w/w) MSG (Ajinomoto) was added to all MSG+ soup conditions. Base soup formulation followed extensive pilot testing to formulate a novel soup low in MSG, and which was rated as moderately pleasant to allow for enhancement by MSG. Pilot testing was also carried out on the energy and macronutrient soup combinations used (see Masic &Yeomans( Reference Masic and Yeomans 15 )).

The high-ED conditions contained approximately 750 kJ (180 kcal) more per portion than the low-ED condition (carbohydrate: 743·7 kJ (177·5 kcal), protein: 771·1 kJ (184·3 kcal)). The small energy difference between the high-ED carbohydrate and protein conditions was due to efforts to minimise the impact of maltodextrin on sweetness in the carbohydrate condition. All nutritional information can be found in Table 1.

Table 1. Nutritional composition of soup preloads (per 100 g)

Ad libitum meal

The two-course ad libitum lunch comprised of pasta in tomato sauce followed by ice cream. Due to an unexpected change in formulation of the pasta sauce by the manufacturer part way through the study, two versions of the meal had to be used but each participant was only tested with one version. Both versions used 250 g cooked pasta (Conchiglie Pasta; Sainsbury's plc). This was combined with 250 g of pasta sauce (Tomato and Basil Sauce; Sainsbury's plc) for version 1, giving a 500 g served portion (total 2273·6 kJ (543·4 kcal)), but since the reformulated sauce had a higher energy density, 200 g of sauce was used for version 2 to ensure a similar energy density across test meals providing a 450 g portion (total 2093·7 kJ (500·4 kcal)). In all, fifteen participants were tested with version 1 and twenty with version 2. Analyses of sensory ratings and intake of the test meal depending on the served version found a significant difference in rated saltiness between the two versions (F(5,165) = 2·78; P = 0·02) but no other ratings differed significantly, including pleasantness (F(5,165) = 0·73; NS). Intake did not differ significantly between those consuming version 1 and version 2 (F(5,165) = 0·85; NS) and when test meal version was included as a covariate, no differences in pasta meal intake was apparent (MSG × test meal version: F(1,33) = 2·57, NS; MSG × condition × test meal version: F(2,66) = 1·34, NS). The served dessert was 150 g of vanilla ice cream (Carte D'Or; Unilever; total 1318 kJ (315 kcal)).

Computerised data collection

Sensory, hedonic and appetite ratings were tracked using the Sussex Ingestion Pattern Monitor (SIPM version 2·0·13; University of Sussex), which is comprised of a digital balance linked to a computer. Participants were asked to complete appetite, sensory, hedonic and mood ratings using digital visual analogue scales by the SIPM. All ratings were presented as sentences (‘How <word> do you feel?’) with a left-hand anchor reading ‘Not at all <word > ’ (coded as 0) and a right-hand anchor reading ‘As <word> as I have ever felt/experienced’ (coded as 100). Instructions on how to use the scale were presented to participants before each evaluation to ensure compliance. Participants registered their selection by pressing ‘Rating Complete’. Presentation order for each group of visual analogue scale ratings was randomised.

When participants were presented with the fixed preload or relevant course they completed a taste test of the food (familiar, pleasant, salty, savoury, strong and sweet for the preload, the same excluding sweetness for the pasta and excluding salty and savoury for the dessert). Following this, appetite ratings of hunger, fullness and thirst after tasting the relevant course and at the end of the course were completed. At the ad libitum lunch participants were given a portion of the food with instructions to ‘Please eat as much (pasta/ice cream) as you like until you feel comfortably full’. Additional (refill) portions of the course being consumed were prompted by the SIPM with an on-screen message and alert sound which instructed participants to call the experimenter.

Procedure

The research took place over six non-consecutive sessions at the Ingestive Behaviour Unit (IBU) at the University of Sussex. Participants were asked to consume nothing but water from 23·00 hours the night before each testing session and were provided with the control breakfast at pre-arranged times (from 09·00 hours to 10·30 hours) across testing days. Participants were free to leave the IBU after breakfast with instructions to consume nothing but water and returned after 3 h for the soup preload.

For all consumption trials (preload and test meal) participants were first asked to complete a bogus motor skills task (the star motor task, which consisted of tracing the outline of a star with their non-dominant hand) in compliance with the cover story. Both the preload (which ran from 12·00 hours to 13·30 hours) and main course sessions started with mood and appetite ratings followed by serving of the relevant course. Taste test and appetite ratings were completed after tasting each course (preload, main course and dessert) with further appetite ratings when each course was completed. The test meal was provided 45 min after the soup as this was the time when differences in hunger were maximal between the protein-rich preloads with and without added MSG( Reference Masic and Yeomans 15 ). For the pasta course a portion of the pasta in sauce was served with refills provided in 500 g (or 450 g) portions after approximately 450 g (or 400 g) consumption whilst a portion of ice cream was served with additional 100 g portions provided after 100 g ice cream consumption. This ensured that food was always present on the plate to prevent normative external cues such as an empty plate from influencing meal intake( Reference Herman and Polivy 37 , Reference Wansink, Painter and North 38 ). The session ended after the final set of mood and appetite ratings. A graphical representation is present in Fig. 1. At the end of sessions 1–5, participants were free to leave but their height and weight was recorded and they were debriefed before payment on the final test day.

Fig. 1. Graphical representation of the timings of the preload and ad libitum meals and the appetite ratings made on each test day. ↓, Appetite rating made; ■, fixed meal; □, ad libitum meal.

Data analysis

Test meal intake (energy consumed) was contrasted using two-way 3 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA with soup type (control, protein or carbohydrate) and MSG condition (MSG+ or MSG–) as variables. Intake was also analysed across the whole test meal and by individual courses (savoury course and dessert) to determine whether preload conditions made an impact on intake overall or differed by course. Additional analyses were conducted to determine energy compensation (COMPX) at the test meal after high-energy (protein-rich or carbohydrate-rich) preloads across MSG conditions (MSG+ or MSG–). COMPX values were calculated by subtracting test meal intake in the relevant control (low-energy) MSG+ or MSG– condition from the corresponding protein or carbohydrate (high-energy) MSG+ or MSG– condition and expressing this value as a percentage of the actual difference in soup preload energy between low- and high-energy preloads (protein: 770 kJ (184 kcal); carbohydrate: 741 kJ (177 kcal)). The resulting COMPX scores were contrasted using two-way 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA. To control for repeat testing, test order was included as a factor in all analyses.

Satiation analyses (pre-meal, post-taste and post-course) were conducted for each eating episode over time using 3 × 2 × 3 repeated-measures ANOVA assessing appetite (hunger and fullness) for the preload and separate 3 × 2 × 5 repeated-measures ANOVA assessing appetite for the test meal due to the hypothesised effects of MSG increasing hunger during the preload but maintaining satiety over the test meal duration. A separate 3 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA analysing the preload effects over 45 min post-ingestion was also carried out to assess the hypothesis of a slower return of hunger in protein-rich MSG+ conditions, as found previously( Reference Masic and Yeomans 15 ). Hunger and fullness variables during preload satiation were transformed to track the more subtle changes from baseline over the eating episode. Sensory evaluations of the soup and test meal were analysed using repeated-measures two-way 3 × 2 ANOVA. In cases of violated sphericity, Greenhouse Geisser values (ε ≤ 0·75) were adopted. In cases of violated Greenhouse Geisser assumptions (ε ≥ 0·75), Huynh–Feldt values were reported. Effect sizes are reported for specific effects using Pearson's correlation coefficient. Data are shown for all thirty-five participants.

Results

Test meal intake

As expected, energy intake at the test lunch varied between the three soup preloads (F(1·75,59·55) = 4·58; P = 0·02) with significantly less energy consumed after the protein-rich soup relative to control (F(1,34) = 7·47, P = 0·01, r 0·42; Table 2). However, although there was a tendency for lower energy consumption in MSG+ relative to MSG– in both the protein-rich and carbohydrate-rich, but not control, conditions (Table 2), these effects were not significant, nor did any effects of added MSG emerge from analysis of the two courses of the meal separately. An order × condition interaction effect was found for intake of the savoury (F(10,58) = 2·41; P = 0·02) and sweet (F(10,58) = 2·05; P = 0·04) course, with inspection of the data across test sessions suggesting that intake was highest in the first session after which consumption was adjusted to the nutrients ingested in the preload.

Table 2. Energy intake of an ad libitum meal after a soup preload (low-energy control, high-energy carbohydrate or high-energy protein) with (MSG+) or without (MSG–) added monosodium glutamate

(Mean values with their standard errors)

Compensation for preload energy

Overall energy compensation at the test meal was significantly better in the protein than carbohydrate condition (F(1,34) = 4·19, P = 0·05, r 0·33; Fig. 2). Notably, the small effects of added MSG on lunch intake translated into significantly better compensation for added energy in the protein-rich soup, with 62 % compensation in the protein MSG+ condition compared with only 24 % compensation in the carbohydrate MSG+ condition (F(1,34) = 5·45; P = 0·03; r 0·37). This effect was largely driven by differences in savoury course intake (F(1,34) = 5·63; P = 0·02; r 0·38) with 51 % compensation in pasta intake in the MSG+ protein condition but only 16 % in the carbohydrate MSG+ condition. No significant differences in compensation were found when comparing carbohydrate and protein MSG– conditions overall (F(1,34) = 1·21; NS; r 0·04).

Fig. 2. Energy compensation at an ad libitum test meal (pasta main course (□) and ice cream dessert (■)) after fixed consumption of high-energy carbohydrate and high-energy protein soup preloads with and without added monosodium glutamate (MSG). Values are means, with standard errors represented by vertical bars. a,b Mean values with unlike letters were significantly different (P ≤ 0·05; within-subjects Bonferroni-corrected contrasts).

Rated hunger and fullness during the preload and test meal

Preload satiation and satiety

There were no significant spurious differences in rated hunger (F(2,68) = 0·06; NS) or fullness (F(2,68) = 0·68; NS) before the soup was tasted. Thus, change from baseline hunger and fullness were analysed to assess the influence of MSG manipulations when the soup was first tasted (assessing the appetiser effect( Reference Yeomans 4 )) and immediately after consuming the soup preload (assessing effects on satiation). A significant condition × MSG interaction (F(2,68) = 4·10; P = 0·02) in hunger ratings immediately after tasting the soup indicated that hunger increased after tasting the MSG+ control and carbohydrate soups but surprisingly decreased after tasting in the MSG+ protein condition whilst in the MSG– conditions, hunger decreased after control and carbohydrate conditions but increased after tasting the protein-rich soup (F(1,34) = 5·70, P = 0·02, r 0·38; Table 3). No significant effects of soup preload on hunger (F(2,68) = 0·31; NS) or fullness (F(2,68) ≤ 0·001; NS) were found immediately after soup intake across conditions, indicating that differences in nutrient content did not affect satiation. Satiety analyses post-preload to pre-lunch revealed an increase in hunger (F(1,34) = 38·30; P ≤ 0·001) and decrease in fullness (F(1,34) = 59·55; P ≤ 0·001) over the 45 min period as expected. However, there was no effect of MSG and no MSG × condition interaction on hunger (MSG: F(1,34) = 0·87, NS; condition × MSG interaction: F(1·7,57·2) = 0·21, NS) or fullness (MSG: F(1,34) ≤0·001, NS; condition × MSG interaction: F(1·8,57·3) = 0·33, NS) in contrast to what was found previously( Reference Masic and Yeomans 15 ).

Table 3. Change from baseline visual analogue scale appetite ratings for three versions of soups (low-energy control, high-energy carbohydrate, and high-energy protein) with (MSG+) and without (MSG–) added monosodium glutamate

(Mean values with their standard errors)

Test meal satiation

Changes in fullness and hunger during the test meal were examined separately to see whether the nutrient and MSG preload manipulations modified the rate of satiation for the test meal. Over the course of the test meal protein maintained increased satiety (hunger: F(2,68) = 15·18, P ≤ 0·001; fullness: F(2,68) = 4·01, P = 0·02) with a significant condition × time interaction (F(2,68) = 10·31, P ≤ 0·001), suggesting that appetite was most suppressed post-meal after the protein-rich preloads than all other conditions (Fig. 3). Overall, hunger was more suppressed after MSG– soup preloads compared with MSG+ (main effect of MSG: F(1,34) = 4·52; P = 0·04; r 0·34) but this may be driven by the effect of MSG on the carbohydrate-rich condition compared with the protein-rich condition, as a significant condition × MSG × time interaction (F(2,68) = 5·39; P = 0·007) revealed that the addition of MSG to the carbohydrate-rich soup suppressed hunger less over the course of the ad libitum meal when compared with control (F(1,34) = 4·15; P = 0·05; r 0·33) but acted to reduce hunger more in the protein condition when compared with control (F(1,34) = 9·77, P = 0·004, r 0·47; Fig. 3). No significant effects of added MSG were found (F(1,34) = 0·43; NS) and no MSG × condition interaction was evident (F(2,68) = 1·08; NS).

Fig. 3. Change in hunger (A, B and C) and fullness (D, E and F) ratings over the duration of an ad libitum test meal (energy consumed) following consumption of three versions of soup (low-energy control (A and D); high-energy carbohydrate (B and E); high-energy protein (C and F)) with (- - -) and without (–––) added monosodium glutamate. Values are means, with standard errors represented by vertical bars. See text for statistical analysis. To convert kcal to kJ, multiply by 4·184.

Sensory ratings of the preload and test meal

There were no significant differences noted for familiarity (F(2,68) = 0·08; NS), pleasantness (F(2,68) = 2·93; NS) or sweetness (F(2,68) = 2·31; NS) across soup conditions; all soups were deemed familiar, pleasant and marginally sweet (Table 4). MSG+ soups were rated as more salty (F(1,34) = 26·26; P ≤ 0·001) and stronger tasting (F(1,34) = 12·57; P = 0·001) than MSG– soups and there was a significant soup × MSG interaction found for savoury assessments (F(2,68) = 4·37; P = 0·02), with MSG+ control and protein-rich soups deemed more savoury than MSG– but MSG+ carbohydrate soups rated as less savoury than MSG– conditions (Table 4). This may be due to the ambiguous nature of the ‘savoury’ label affecting sensory judgements as trained sensory panels were not tested. The test lunch items were deemed equally familiar across conditions. There was a significant difference in savoury course pleasantness (F(2,68) = 5·93; P = 0·004), and strength of flavour (F(2,68) = 3·16; P = 0·05) ratings across conditions, with higher ratings made after protein preload consumption, followed by control and carbohydrate conditions. There were no significant differences in sensory ratings across conditions for the dessert course.

Table 4. Visual analogue scale ratings of the sensory characteristics of three versions of soup (low-energy control, high-energy carbohydrate and high-energy protein) with (MSG+) and without (MSG–) added monosodium glutamate

(Mean values with their standard errors)

Discussion

The main findings of the study indicate that nutrients received largely as protein in a soup preload allow for an adequate adjustment of energy consumed at a later ad libitum meal. Increasing the overall energy content of the test soup mainly with added protein resulted in a greater reduction in lunch intake, and consequently more accurate compensation for this added energy, than was seen when energy was increased by addition of carbohydrate, in line with several recent studies( Reference Bertenshaw, Lluch and Yeomans 23 , Reference Bertenshaw, Lluch and Yeomans 39, Reference Yeomans and Chambers 40 ). Although the predicted enhancement of protein-induced satiety by the addition of MSG was not significant based on intake data, the addition of MSG further improved compensation for added protein energy. However, there were no differences between MSG or specific macronutrient conditions in rated satiety over the course of testing after preload intake.

As predicted, test meal intake after consumption of the protein-enriched preload was lowest followed by the carbohydrate and control conditions consistent with the broader literature, suggesting that protein is more satiating than is carbohydrate( Reference Poppitt, McCormack and Buffenstein 18 Reference Fischer, Colombani and Wenk 20 ). Importantly, compensation became even more accurate when MSG was added to the protein-rich compared with the carbohydrate-rich preload, with this effect strongest in the savoury course of the test meal. This compensation effect was evident despite the relatively small energy difference between low- and high-energy conditions. Indeed, in previous studies compensation effects have only been apparent with larger energy differences between preloads( Reference Bertenshaw, Lluch and Yeomans 23 , Reference Yeomans and Chambers 40, Reference Yeomans, Lee and Gray 41 ). This suggests that moderate increases in the energy content of a food through the addition of protein and MSG, for example as a savoury snack, may reduce the likelihood of subsequent overconsumption.

We suggest two possible explanations for how MSG may enhance compensation for energy added as protein. First, the sensory quality generated by the addition of MSG may have made the experience of protein more salient, and so enhanced the satiating effects of protein at the test meal. This idea is supported by recent data showing no significant differences in compensation at a test meal following high-protein and high-carbohydrate preloads when these were matched for thickness and creaminess( Reference Bertenshaw, Lluch and Yeomans 28 ). Indeed, it may be that the characteristics of MSG are more strongly associated with protein than are the sensory characteristics of thickness and creaminess previously identified and thus act as a more reliable cue for protein-based satiety. Alternatively, this improved compensation of protein in the MSG+ condition could be related to post-ingestive stimulation of gut glutamate sensors( Reference Masic and Yeomans 15 , Reference Kitamura, Tsurugizawa and Torii 42 ) which have been related to enhanced satiety in animals( Reference Kondoh and Torii 43 Reference Niijima 45 ).

An appetising effect( Reference Yeomans 4 , Reference Yeomans and Gray 46 ) of MSG was seen in both the control and carbohydrate-rich soups with added MSG but was not found in the equivalent protein-rich soup. This may be due to the immediate sensory experience of protein and MSG eliciting lower feelings of hunger in the protein-rich MSG+ condition; however, as ad libitum intake of the soup course was not assessed, this remains as speculation. Despite the immediate stimulation of appetite by added MSG in some conditions, no significant differences in hunger were seen immediately after ingesting the soup, in contrast to our recent study( Reference Masic and Yeomans 15 ). This may be related to volume and hedonic assessments, with participants expecting each soup to be equally satiating due to the equivalent volumes consumed( Reference Allirot, Saulaisa and Disseb 47 ). These predetermined portion sizes may then have influenced hedonic hunger( Reference Yeomans 48 ). No difference in appetite was also evident immediately before the ad libitum meal 45 min after preload ingestion and may be due to participants responding in anticipation of the meal to be received( Reference Weingarten 49 ). Hunger also remained stronger at the ad libitum meal in added MSG control and carbohydrate-rich conditions but was suppressed in added MSG protein-rich conditions. Such an effect may be indicative of delayed rebound hunger( Reference Yeomans and Chambers 40 ), as participants consuming added MSG preloads without the accompanying protein may have been anticipating stronger satiety due to the MSG protein cue. However, as a large dose of protein was not delivered, hunger was not as satisfied at the end of the test meal as when no protein cue was present (in no-MSG conditions). But when added MSG was presented in combination with protein, the additional protein cue from MSG acted to reduce feelings of hunger more strongly, suggesting that the added MSG may have increased the salience of this cue as has been found previously( Reference Masic and Yeomans 15 ) and is evident in infants experiencing umami taste in mother's milk( Reference Mennella and Beauchamp 29 , Reference Ventura, Beauchamp and Mennella 30 ).

The present study also noted that most of the effects of the preload manipulations on lunch intake were evident for the first (savoury) course whereas intake at the dessert course was not affected by preload type. This may be due to the high palatability of this course overriding sensory and energy effects as has been found previously( Reference Yeomans, Lee and Gray 41 ), as sweet appetite relies less on the experience of hunger( Reference de Graaf, Schreurs and Blauw 50 ) and more on the hedonic effects of palatability( Reference Rogers and Blundell 51 , Reference Blundell and Rogers 52 ). Thus consumption of a sweet course may be less suppressed by a previously consumed savoury course( Reference Appleton, Rogers and Blundell 53 ), with the critical impact on behaviour being an earlier switch from savoury to sweet courses. There were also a number of limitations in the present design that constrained the conclusions drawn. Due to reformulations of the ad libitum main course item, the different versions of the main course may have influenced test meal intake. However, further analyses of intake taking this into account indicated that this was most likely not the case. Similarly, effects of order on ad libitum intake indicated that consumption was greater after the first test day but thereafter consumption was in line with the nutrients ingested, indicating that these order effects should not have influenced the sensory–nutrient interactions reported. Initial power analyses indicated that the sample tested would yield adequate power; however, although rated appetite and intake were in the direction predicted they was not found to be significant. This may suggest that a larger sample would be required to assess the more subtle effects of MSG on appetite. It must also be noted that some research has claimed that there can be adverse effects for some consumers when they ingest MSG( Reference Kwok 54 ) although double-blind studies suggest this may be more due to expectation than actual effects of MSG( Reference Geha, Beiser and Ren 55 , Reference Freeman 56 ). Further research is warranted to truly understand such findings in human subjects.

Overall, the addition of protein to a soup preload reduced subsequent intake and allowed for more accurate energy compensation at a test meal and this was enhanced by the addition of MSG. However, subjective satiety ratings were not influenced by MSG, the nutrients tested or energy 45 min after preload intake. Further research is required to understand the influence of MSG and protein on sensory and gut responding as well as measures of appetite hormones during and after intake of the preload conditions assessed to gain a more detailed understanding of how sensory–nutrient interactions influence rated appetite and subsequent intake.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Gabriella Margetts-Smith for carrying out some of the preload soup testing.

This research was funded by Ajinomoto Co., Inc., which had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article.

U. M. and M. R. Y. formulated the research question and designed the study. U. M. carried out the testing, analysed the data and wrote the article with revisions read by M. R. Y. and corrections made accordingly.

There are no conflicts of interest to declare.

References

1. Ikeda, K (1908) On a new seasoning. J Tokyo Chem Soc 30, 820836.Google Scholar
2. Jinap, S & Hajeb, P (2010) Glutamate. Its applications in food and contribution to health. Appetite 55, 110.Google Scholar
3. Ninomiya, K (1998) Natural occurrence. Food Rev Int 14, 177211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
4. Yeomans, MR (1996) Palatability and the micro-structure of feeding in humans: the appetizer effect. Appetite 27, 119133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5. Yeomans, MR & Gray, RW (2002) Opioid peptides and the control of human ingestive behaviour. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26, 713728.Google Scholar
6. Yeomans, MR, Gray, RW & Conyers, THB (1998) Maltodextrin preloads reduce food intake without altering the appetiser effect. Physiol Behav 64, 501506.Google Scholar
7. Mathey, M-FAM, Siebelink, E, de Graaf, C, et al. (2001) Flavor enhancement of food improves dietary intake and nutritional status of elderly nursing home residents. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 56, M200M205.Google Scholar
8. Schiffman, SS, Sattely-Miller, EA, Zimmerman, IA, et al. (1994) Taste perception of monosodium glutamate (MSG) in foods in young and elderly subjects. Physiol Behav 56, 265275.Google Scholar
9. Essed, N, Oerlemans, P, Hoek, M, et al. (2009) Optimal preferred MSG concentration in potatoes, spinach and beef and their effect on intake in institutionalized elderly people. J Nutr Health Aging 13, 769775.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10. Bellisle, F, Monneuse, MO, Chabert, M, et al. (1991) Monosodium glutamate as a palatability enhancer in the European diet. Physiol Behav 49, 869873.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11. Bellisle, F, Tournier, A & Louis-Sylvestre, J (1989) Monosodium glutamate and the acquisition of food preferences in a European context. Food Qual Prefer 1, 103108.Google Scholar
12. Essed, NH, van Staveren, WA, Kok, FJ, et al. (2007) No effect of 16 weeks flavor enhancement on dietary intake and nutritional status of nursing home elderly. Appetite 48, 2936.Google Scholar
13. Bellisle, F, Dalix, AM, Chappuis, AS, et al. (1996) Monosodium glutamate affects mealtime food selection in diabetic patients. Appetite 26, 267276.Google Scholar
14. Bellisle, F (1999) Glutamate and the UMAMI taste: sensory, metabolic, nutritional and behavioural considerations. A review of the literature published in the last 10 years. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 23, 423438.Google Scholar
15. Masic, U & Yeomans, MR (2013) Does monosodium glutamate interact with macronutrient composition to influence subsequent appetite? Physiol Behav 116–117, 2329.Google Scholar
16. Smriga, M & Torii, K (2000) Release of hypothalamic norepinephrine during MSG intake in rats fed normal and nonprotein diet. Physiol Behav 70, 413415.Google Scholar
17. Laska, M & Hernandez Salazar, LT (2004) Gustatory responsiveness to monosodium glutamate and sodium chloride in four species of nonhuman primates. J Exp Zool A Comp Exp Biol 301A, 898905.Google Scholar
18. Poppitt, SD, McCormack, D & Buffenstein, R (1998) Short-term effects of macronutrient preloads on appetite and energy intake in lean women. Physiol Behav 64, 279285.Google Scholar
19. Marmonier, C, Chapelot, D & Louis-Sylvestre, J (2000) Effects of macronutrient content and energy density of snacks consumed in a satiety state on the onset of the next meal. Appetite 34, 161168.Google Scholar
20. Fischer, K, Colombani, PC & Wenk, C (2004) Metabolic and cognitive coefficients in the development of hunger sensations after pure macronutrient ingestion in the morning. Appetite 42, 4961.Google Scholar
21. Weigle, DS, Breen, PA, Matthys, CC, et al. (2005) A high-protein diet induces sustained reductions in appetite, ad libitum caloric intake, and body weight despite compensatory changes in diurnal plasma leptin and ghrelin concentrations. Am J Clin Nutr 82, 4148.Google Scholar
22. Simpson, SJ & Raubenheimer, D (editors) (2000) Geometric Models of Macronutrient Selection. Boca Raton: CRC Press.Google Scholar
23. Bertenshaw, EJ, Lluch, A & Yeomans, MR (2008) Satiating effects of protein but not carbohydrate consumed in a between-meal beverage context. Physiol Behav 93, 427436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24. Latner, JD & Schwartz, M (1999) The effects of a high-carbohydrate, high-protein or balanced lunch upon later food intake and hunger ratings. Appetite 33, 119128.Google Scholar
25. Lejeune, MPGM, Kovacs, EMR & Westerterp-Plantenga, MS (2005) Additional protein intake limits weight regain after weight loss in humans. Br J Nutr 93, 281289.Google Scholar
26. Leidy, HJ, Tang, M, Armstrong, CLH, et al. (2011) The effects of consuming frequent, higher protein meals on appetite and satiety during weight loss in overweight/obese men. Obesity 19, 818824.Google Scholar
27. Simpson, SJ & Raubenheimer, D (2005) Obesity: the protein leverage hypothesis. Obes Rev 6, 133142.Google Scholar
28. Bertenshaw, EJ, Lluch, A & Yeomans, MR (2013) Perceived thickness and creaminess modulates the short-term satiating effects of high-protein drinks. Br J Nutr 110, 578586.Google Scholar
29. Mennella, JA & Beauchamp, GK (1996) Developmental changes in the acceptance of protein hydrolysate formula. J Dev Behav Pediatr 17, 386391.Google Scholar
30. Ventura, AK, Beauchamp, GK & Mennella, JA (2012) Infant regulation of intake: the effect of free glutamate content in infant formulas. Am J Clin Nutr 95, 875881.Google Scholar
31. Luscombe-Marsh, ND, Smeets, AJPG & Westerterp-Plantenga, MS (2009) The addition of monosodium glutamate and inosine monophosphate-5 to high-protein meals: effects on satiety, and energy and macronutrient intakes. Br J Nutr 102, 929937.Google Scholar
32. Williams, EJ (1949) Experimental designs balanced for the estimation of residual effects of treatment. Aust J Sci Res A 2, 149168.Google Scholar
33. Stunkard, AJ & Messick, S (1985) The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. J Psychosom Res 29, 7183.Google Scholar
34. Fedoroff, IDC, Polivy, J & Herman, CP (1997) The effect of pre-exposure to food cues on the eating behavior of restrained and unrestrained eaters. Appetite 28, 3347.Google Scholar
35. Fedoroff, I, Polivy, J & Herman, CP (2003) The specificity of restrained versus unrestrained eaters' responses to food cues: general desire to eat, or craving for the cued food? Appetite 41, 713.Google Scholar
36. Food Standards Agency (2006) FSA Nutrient and Food Based Guidelines for UK Institutions. London: Food Standards Agency.Google Scholar
37. Herman, CP & Polivy, J (2008) External cues in the control of food intake in humans: the sensory–normative distinction. Physiol Behav 94, 722728.Google Scholar
38. Wansink, B, Painter, JE & North, J (2005) Bottomless bowls: why visual cues of portion size may influence intake. Obes Res 13, 93100.Google Scholar
39. Bertenshaw, EJ, Lluch, A & Yeomans, MR (2009) Dose-dependent effects of beverage protein content upon short-term intake. Appetite 52, 580587.Google Scholar
40. Yeomans, MR & Chambers, L (2011) Satiety-relevant sensory qualities enhance the satiating effects of mixed carbohydrate–protein preloads. Am J Clin Nutr 94, 14101417.Google Scholar
41. Yeomans, MR, Lee, MD, Gray, RW, et al. (2001) Effects of test-meal palatability on compensatory eating following disguised fat and carbohydrate preloads. Int J Obes 25, 12151224.Google Scholar
42. Kitamura, A, Tsurugizawa, T & Torii, K (2011) Biological significance of glutamate signaling during digestion of food through the gut–brain axis. Digestion 83, Suppl. 1, 3743.Google Scholar
43. Kondoh, T & Torii, K (2008) MSG intake suppresses weight gain, fat deposition, and plasma leptin levels in male Sprague–Dawley rats. Physiol Behav 95, 135144.Google Scholar
44. San Gabriel, AM, Maekawa, T, Uneyama, H, et al. (2007) mGluR1 in the fundic glands of rat stomach. FEBS Lett 581, 11191123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45. Niijima, A (2000) Reflex effects of oral, gastrointestinal and hepatoportal glutamate sensors on vagal nerve activity. J Nutr 130, 971S973S.Google Scholar
46. Yeomans, MR & Gray, RW (1997) Effects of naltrexone on food intake and changes in subjective appetite during eating: evidence for opioid involvement in the appetizer effect. Physiol Behav 62, 1521.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
47. Allirot, X, Saulaisa, L, Disseb, E, et al. (2014) Integrating behavioral measurements in physiological approaches of satiety. Food Qual Pref 31, 181189.Google Scholar
48. Yeomans, MR (2010) Understanding individual differences in acquired flavour liking in humans. Chemosens Percept 3, 3441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
49. Weingarten, HP (1984) Meal initiation controlled by learned cues: basic behavioral properties. Appetite 5, 147158.Google Scholar
50. de Graaf, C, Schreurs, A & Blauw, YH (1993) Short-term effects of different amounts of sweet and nonsweet carbohydrates on satiety and energy intake. Physiol Behav 54, 833843.Google Scholar
51. Rogers, PJ & Blundell, JE (1989) Separating the actions of sweetness and calories: effects of saccharin and carbohydrates on hunger and food intake in human subjects. Physiol Behav 45, 10931099.Google Scholar
52. Blundell, JE & Rogers, PJ (editors) (1994) Sweet Carbohydrate Substitutes (Intense Sweeteners) and the Control of Appetite: Scientific Issues. London: CRC Press.Google Scholar
53. Appleton, KM, Rogers, PJ & Blundell, JE (2004) Effects of a sweet and a nonsweet lunch on short-term appetite: differences in female high and low consumers of sweet/low-energy beverages. J Hum Nutr Diet 17, 425434.Google Scholar
54. Kwok, RHM (1968) Chinese restaurant syndrome. New Engl J Med 18, 796.Google Scholar
55. Geha, RS, Beiser, A, Ren, C, et al. (2000) Review of alleged reaction to monosodium glutamate and outcome of a multicenter double-blind placebo-controlled study. J Nutr 130, 1058S1062S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
56. Freeman, M (2006) Reconsidering the effects of monosodium glutamate: a literature review. J Am Acad Nurse Pract 18, 482486.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Nutritional composition of soup preloads (per 100 g)

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Graphical representation of the timings of the preload and ad libitum meals and the appetite ratings made on each test day. ↓, Appetite rating made; ■, fixed meal; □, ad libitum meal.

Figure 2

Table 2. Energy intake of an ad libitum meal after a soup preload (low-energy control, high-energy carbohydrate or high-energy protein) with (MSG+) or without (MSG–) added monosodium glutamate(Mean values with their standard errors)

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Energy compensation at an ad libitum test meal (pasta main course (□) and ice cream dessert (■)) after fixed consumption of high-energy carbohydrate and high-energy protein soup preloads with and without added monosodium glutamate (MSG). Values are means, with standard errors represented by vertical bars. a,b Mean values with unlike letters were significantly different (P ≤ 0·05; within-subjects Bonferroni-corrected contrasts).

Figure 4

Table 3. Change from baseline visual analogue scale appetite ratings for three versions of soups (low-energy control, high-energy carbohydrate, and high-energy protein) with (MSG+) and without (MSG–) added monosodium glutamate(Mean values with their standard errors)

Figure 5

Fig. 3. Change in hunger (A, B and C) and fullness (D, E and F) ratings over the duration of an ad libitum test meal (energy consumed) following consumption of three versions of soup (low-energy control (A and D); high-energy carbohydrate (B and E); high-energy protein (C and F)) with (- - -) and without (–––) added monosodium glutamate. Values are means, with standard errors represented by vertical bars. See text for statistical analysis. To convert kcal to kJ, multiply by 4·184.

Figure 6

Table 4. Visual analogue scale ratings of the sensory characteristics of three versions of soup (low-energy control, high-energy carbohydrate and high-energy protein) with (MSG+) and without (MSG–) added monosodium glutamate(Mean values with their standard errors)