Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T19:15:12.929Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Association of household food insecurity with developmental delay in preschool children: 2018 Ecuadorian Nutrition and Health National Survey

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 August 2023

M. Margaret Weigel*
Affiliation:
Department of Environmental & Occupational Health, Indiana University Bloomington, Bloomington, IN, USA Global Environmental Health Research Laboratory, Indiana University-Bloomington School of Public Health, Bloomington, IN, USA Center for Latin American and Caribbean Studies, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA Indiana University Center for Global Health Equity, Indianapolis, IN, USA
Rodrigo X. Armijos
Affiliation:
Department of Environmental & Occupational Health, Indiana University Bloomington, Bloomington, IN, USA Global Environmental Health Research Laboratory, Indiana University-Bloomington School of Public Health, Bloomington, IN, USA Center for Latin American and Caribbean Studies, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA Indiana University Center for Global Health Equity, Indianapolis, IN, USA
*
*Corresponding author: M. Margaret Weigel, Department of Environmental & Occupational Health, Innovation Center 130-E, Indiana University School of Public Health, Bloomington, IN, USA, email [email protected]

Abstract

We investigated the association of household food insecurity (HFI) with developmental delays in 36–59-month-old preschool children (n 7005) using cross-sectional data from the 2018 Ecuadorian National Health and Nutrition Survey. HFI was assessed with the Food Insecurity Experience Scale and developmental delays with the Early Childhood Development Index. Log-binomial regression models estimated the association of HFI with global (overall) developmental delay (GDD) and delays in four individual developmental domains, adjusting for covariates. Nearly half of the children lived in households with marginal (24⋅5 %) or moderate-severe HFI (21⋅7 %). Eighteen percent were identified with GDD. Delays in the individual domains of literacy-numeracy, social-emotional, physical and cognitive development were identified for 64, 21⋅5, 3⋅3 and 3⋅1 %, respectively. GDD was more likely among preschool children from households with marginal (aPR = 1⋅29; 95 % C.I. = 1⋅10, 1⋅49) and moderate-severe HFI (aPR = 1⋅30; 95 % C.I. = 1⋅11, 1⋅51). Social-emotional development delays were also more likely among those from households with marginal (aPR = 1⋅36; 95 % C.I. = 1⋅19, 1⋅56) and moderate-severe HFI (aPR = 1⋅33; 95 % C.I. = 1⋅15, 1⋅54) different from the other three domains. Several other potentially modifiable risk (violent discipline, maternal depressive symptoms) and protective factors (adequate child stimulation, higher maternal education, handwashing with soap/detergent) were also independently associated with GDD and/or literacy-numeracy and cognitive delays. Our findings suggest that HFI is an independent risk factor for GDD and social-emotional developmental delays in Ecuadorian preschoolers. They underscore the importance of strengthening and expanding poverty reduction, food security and early childhood development policies and interventions to improve the opportunities for children to achieve their full developmental potential.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Introduction

Early childhood developmental delay (ECDD) is a major global public health concern including in low- and middle-income countries (LMICs) where an estimated 250 million children under the age of five are at-risk for not achieving their full developmental potential(Reference Black, Walker and Fernald1). ECDD occurs when a child does not achieve basic developmental milestones in cognitive, literacy-numeracy, physical and social-emotional domains at an expected age(Reference Gil, Ewerling and Ferreira2). These delays can be individual or involve two or more domains (global developmental delay). The importance of ECDD is underscored by its inclusion as one of the UN 2030 Sustainable Development Goals (Goal 4⋅2) and its implicit role in several others(3).

Early childhood development is affected by multiple biological, social and environmental determinants and their interactions across multiple levels of influence. Household-level exposures linked in the literature to ECDD include material poverty, environmental toxins, maternal mental illness, suboptimal parenting practices, violence, lack of adequate stimulation and learning opportunities, poor quality diets and malnutrition(Reference Sania, Sudfeld and Danaei4Reference Ward, Lee and Grogan-Kaylor10). Another is exposure to household food insecurity (HFI) or ‘the limited or uncertain availability of households to obtain nutritionally adequate and safe foods or the limited or uncertain ability to acquire acceptable foods in socially acceptable ways’(Reference Andersen11).

Both nutritional and non-nutritional pathways have been proposed to explain how HFI may cause developmental delays in young children(Reference Schwarzenberg and Georgieff12Reference Jackson, Testa and Semenza15). One of the nutritional pathways is through the adverse impact of HFI on dietary quality/quantity, child nutrition and health (e.g., micronutrient deficiencies, stunting, infections) and ultimately on neurological and physical development. Another potential nutritional pathway is through the negative effects of HFI on the dietary energy and micronutrient intakes of children as well as their parental caregivers. A consequence of this is that both children and their parents may be more likely to experience fatigue, lethargy and/or depressive symptoms. This can lead to decreased quality and quantity of parental nurturing, parent–child interactions and child interactions with other adults and children in the child's social sphere, which are all essential for normal child development. In addition, it has been hypothesised that feelings of stress, anxiety and depression experienced by parental caregivers over their household food security situation can negatively affect their child's nurturing behaviour and other interactions and ultimately, their child's development. Another hypothesised pathway is that the poverty-related financial constraints experienced by food-insecure households may make them less able to invest in early childhood education and physical resources (e.g., educational toys, books) that stimulate child development.

Research investigating the association of HFI with ECDD in preschool children is relatively limited compared to other child age groups. Most studies focused on the first 1000 d of life (age <3 years) because it is a period of rapid brain growth and development(Reference Johnson and Markowitz16). However, the second 1000 d of life (preschool period) is also important. This developmental period is characterised by the maturation of the anatomical and physiological substrates of the brain that is accompanied by increases in working memory, attention control, self-regulation and cognitive and social-emotional growth changes that set the stage for future academic, social and life success(Reference de Oliveira, de Almeida and Gubert17,Reference Gallegos, Eivers and Sondergeld18) . In preschool children, the failure to meet developmental milestones is strongly linked to reduced school readiness, decreased educational achievement, lower future economic productivity and income and the intergenerational transmission of poverty(Reference Black, Walker and Fernald1,Reference Shankar, Chung and Frank19,Reference Finch, Yousafzai and Rasheed20) .

Most prior studies examining the relationship between HFI and ECDD in preschoolers were conducted in the U.S. and other high-income countries (HIC), using different study designs, analytic methods and instruments. Their findings generally suggest that those living in food-insecure households were more likely to show evidence of delayed development, especially in the social-emotional domain, compared to those from fully food secure households(Reference Schwarzenberg and Georgieff12Reference Alderman and Fernald14). Findings from the few studies published on this age group in LMICS have reported mixed results with respect to the association of HFI with social-emotional, cognitive or other specific developmental domains(Reference Jackson, Testa and Semenza15,Reference Aurino, Wolf and Tsinigo21) . Moreover, none were conducted in LMICS in Latin America, a region where 30 % or more of all households suffer from moderate-severe HFI(22,23) and global developmental delay is estimated to affect 25 % of all preschool children(Reference Gil, Ewerling and Ferreira2). The investigation of this relationship in Latin American LMICs is important since not only does the prevalence of HFI and ECDD differ from HIC in the larger Americas region but they also frequently vary on household-level risk and protective factors as well as population access to early childhood development programs, social welfare, health care and child food and nutrition policies and programs. Understanding the relationship between exposure to HFI and developmental delays in preschool children is important for informing policy and interventions can reduce both and improve their future chances for better well-being, educational prospects and future productivity.

Ecuador is a middle-income South American country with high rates of income poverty (25 %) and extreme poverty (10⋅7 %)(24) as well as moderate-severe HFI (32⋅7 %)(23). The sole Ecuadorian study to report ECDD prevalence estimates for preschoolers noted that 73 % of the 48–61-month-old children studied had difficulties with problem-solving and 28 % had fine motor skill delay(Reference Handal, Lozoff and Breilh25). Although one Ecuadorian study linked HFI to psychosocial dysfunction and social-emotional delay (i.e., internalising and externalising behaviours) in 6–12-year-old public elementary school children, none have previously examined the relationship between HFI and ECDD in preschool-age children in this population(Reference Weigel and Armijos26).

The present study analysed the data from the nationally representative 2018 Ecuadorian Nutrition and Health National Survey (ENSANUT) to examine the relationship of HFI with developmental delay in preschool children aged 36–59 months. Our working hypothesis was that HFI would be associated with global and domain-specific developmental delays in preschoolers due to its adverse effects on the nutrition and physical and/or psychological health of preschoolers and their maternal caregivers.

Methods

Study design and setting

We analysed cross-sectional data from the most recent 2018 Ecuadorian Nutrition and Health National Survey (ENSANUT) to examine the study hypothesis. The survey design, sampling method and other methodological characteristics have been previously described in detail in publicly available survey documents published by the Ecuadorian National Institute of Statistics and Censuses(27,28) . Briefly, the survey used a stratified multistage probabilistic sampling design to collect data during 2018–2019 from a nationally representative sample of households and children under the age of 5 years living in these households. The research universe consisted of all households and non-institutionalised adults and children living in households in the twenty-four Ecuadorian provinces. Most (80 %) of the survey data were collected from November 2018–January 2019 and the rest from June–July 2019(28). All data were collected prior to the first confirmed COVID-19 case (29 February 2020) in Ecuador.

The sampling frame for the survey was drawn from 2010 to 2017 Ecuadorian population census data. In the first stage, a stratified sample of primary sampling units (PSU) was selected with the probability of selection proportional to the size of the Ecuadorian population. A total of 2590/2591 (99⋅96 %) of the selected PSUs were successfully interviewed. In the second stage, dwellings within each PSU were randomly selected. These were then were stratified into two groups: households having at least one child under the age of 5 years and households without any children under the age of 5 years. Of the 16 982 households selected with a child under the age of 5, 16 430 or 96⋅7 % were successfully interviewed by the survey team. Data - entered into the database were systematically evaluated by survey personnel to identify missing or improbable values and were also inspected for discrepancies against what had been recorded by field interviewers in the written interview documents. When any such problems were identified, the survey team contacted the respondent by telephone or returned to their household to obtain and/or verify the data in question. Fewer than 1⋅2 % of the responses in the ENSANUT 2018 survey were unable to be corrected or verified using these procedures(27,28) .

The present analysis used data from two modules in the 2018 ENSANUT survey, i.e., Child Development and Household Modules. Within the surveyed households containing one or more children under the age of 5 years, only one child per household aged 36–59 months was selected to participate in the Child Development module interviews in order to prevent household oversampling. The child in this age range whose birthday fell closest to the date (month and day) when their household was surveyed was selected for inclusion. The total number of preschool child cases potentially available for analysis was 9028. We sequentially excluded cases missing data on one or more of the four ECDI domain-specific questions (n 1727) or covariates (n 296). This resulted in 7005 cases being available for the final analyses. The publicly available data used in the study were fully de-identified(28). For this reason, the Indiana University Institutional Review Board classifies the study as, ‘research not subject to human subject regulations.’

Measures

Early childhood developmental delay

The Early Childhood Development Index (ECDI) was used by the survey to collect data on suspected developmental delays in preschool children. The ECDI is an international population-based screening measure developed by the United Nations/UNICEF for Multiple Indicator Cluster Surveys (MICS). The instrument was designed to capture the achievement of key developmental milestones in early childhood and facilitate comparisons across diverse populations(Reference Loizillon, Petrowski and Britto29). As Table 1 shows, it consists of ten questions focused on milestones pertaining to four developmental domains: literacy-numeracy, physical, cognitive and social-emotional. Maternal caregivers were asked to report on whether their child met each of these. The three literacy-numeracy domain questions asked caregivers if their child could identify or name at least ten letters of the alphabet, read at least four simple, common words and knew the name of or could recognise the symbols of all numbers from 1 to 10. The two physical domain questions asked whether the child was able to pick up a small object with two fingers such as a stick or rock from the ground and whether they are sometimes too sick to play. The two cognitive domain questions questioned caregivers if their child is able to follow simple directions on how to do something correctly and when given something to do, they can do it independently. Finally, the three social-emotional domain questions asked whether the child gets along well with other children, whether they kick, bite or hit other children and whether they become easily distracted.

Table 1. Early childhood development index (ECDI) milestones and coding: domain-specific and global developmenta

a Table modified from Bliznashka et al. (Reference Bliznashka, Perumal and Yousafzai6)

As Table 1 indicates, the ECDI classifies children as being developmentally ‘on track’ if they pass at least one milestone in the physical and cognitive domains and at least two milestones in the social-emotional and literacy-numeracy domains(Reference Loizillon, Petrowski and Britto29). For assessing global or overall development delays, children are considered ‘on track’ if they pass at least three of the four individual developmental domains. However, since our study was focused on the association of HFI with suspected developmental delays, we used developmentally ‘off-track’ as the outcome (Table 1). Thus, preschoolers in the study were classified as ‘off track’ if they failed both milestones in the physical and cognitive domains and failed one or more of those in the social-emotional and literacy-numeracy domains. Children who did not achieve the benchmarks for two or more individual domains were classified with suspected global developmental delay (Table 1).

Household food insecurity

HFI was the main exposure variable in this study. The food security situation of households was assessed using the Food Insecurity Experience Scale (FIES)(30). The FIES includes eight ‘yes/no’ questions focused on the self-reported, food-related behaviours and experiences of households in the previous 12 months that were associated with difficulties in accessing food resulting from resource constraints(30). Affirmative responses by the maternal caregiver or other household head to the eight items were summed to produce a raw score (range 0–8) with higher scores indicating more severe food insecurity. Households who reported no affirmative responses were classified as food secure, those with 1–3 affirmative responses as marginally food insecure and those with 4–8 affirmative responses with moderate-severe food insecurity.

Covariates

Child, maternal caregiver and household variables available in the ENSANUT survey database were considered for potential inclusion in the statistical models based on their reported theoretical or reported associations with ECDI and HFI. These were child age (36–47 months, 48–59 months), gender (female, male) and adequate stimulation exposure (yes, no). The survey assessed child exposure to adequate stimulation using the Demographic Health Surveys (DHS) home stimulation module questions. This instrument collected data from maternal caregivers on whether the child had been provided with six specific types of stimulation activities in the prior 3 d by any household adults(31). These activities included reading books or looking at pictures, telling stories, naming, counting, drawing, singing, taking the child outside and/or playing with the child. The total number of possible positive responses on the instrument ranged from 0 to 6. Adequate stimulation was defined as the provision of four or more stimulation activities to the child(32).

Exposure to violent disciplinary measures was measured in the survey using items from the UNICEF-modified version of the Parent–Child Conflict Tactics Scale(33). This instrument questioned maternal caregivers about whether they or any other household adults had subjected the child to any sort of violent physical or psychological disciplinary practices in the prior 30 d. The six yes/no questions on the physical aggression subscale included shook the child, spanked the child, hit or slapped the child on the arm or leg, hit the child with an object, hit or slapped the child on the face, head or ears and beat the child as hard as one could. The two questions on the psychological aggression subscale (yes/no) were that they or another household adult shouted at the child or called them names. If the child was reported to have experienced any type of physically or psychologically violent discipline during the prior 30-d period, they were recorded as positive for exposure to violent discipline(33).

Maternal covariates considered for inclusion included maternal ethnoracial minority (indigenous, afro-descendant) v. mestizo majority/other), education (none/basic education/primary school, secondary school or higher education) and depressive symptoms in maternal caregivers which were assessed using the Patient Health Questionnaire-8 (PHQ-8)(Reference Kroenke, Strine and Spitzer34). The PHQ-8 was scored from 0 to 24, with higher scores indicating greater depressive symptoms. Maternal caregivers reporting a positive score of ≥10 on the scale were classified as positive for moderate to severe depressive symptoms(Reference Wu, Levis and Riehm35). Two household-level covariates considered for potential inclusion were region of residence (Andean highlands region, Pacific coastal region, Amazonian region, Galapagos insular region) and handwashing behaviour. The UN/UNICEF Joint Monitoring Programme for Water Supply, Sanitation and Hygiene (JMP) criteria were used to classify the reported handwashing behaviour of household members, i.e., soap or detergent used (i.e., bar or liquid soap/powder/liquid/paste detergent) v. water only or other (i.e., ash/clay/sand)(36).

Data analysis

We conducted descriptive analyses of the outcomes (global and domain-specific developmental delays), independent variables (HFI) and covariates. We used log-binomial regression analysis to estimate the association of HFI with global and domain-specific developmental delays. The findings from these analyses are presented as unadjusted and adjusted prevalence ratios (PR) with their respective 95 % confidence intervals (CI). For the adjusted models, covariates associated with HFI or global or domain-specific developmental delay of P < 0⋅10 were selected for inclusion. These were entered simultaneously (forced entry) in the models. We investigated potential multicollinearity by examining the variance of inflation factors (VIF) and tolerance. VIF values ranged between 1⋅01 and 1⋅09 and tolerance values ranged between 0⋅92 and 1⋅0 suggesting that multicollinearity was not a significant issue(Reference Drennen, Coleman and Ettinger de Cuba37). The models were also examined for potential interaction effects but no meaningful effect modifications were identified. We used survey-provided sample weights for the analyses to account for the complex design of the 2018 ENSANUT national survey(28). The data were analysed using Stata (StataCorp. 2021. Stata Statistical Software: Release 17. College Station, TX: StataCorp LLC. All statistical tests were two-tailed.

Results

Sample characteristics

Table 2 displays the household food security situation and other characteristics of the Ecuadorian preschool children in the study. Close to half of all the households were affected by marginal (24⋅5 %) or moderate-severe food insecurity (21⋅7 %). Slightly more than half of the children were between the ages of 48 and 59 months and were male. A majority were reported to have received adequate stimulation in the home from maternal caregivers and/or other household adults (79 %). Most (70 %) also were reported to have been disciplined by household adults using violent psychological or physical measures. Maternal caregivers were predominantly from the mestizo ethnoracial majority group (84 %), two-thirds had at least some secondary or higher formal education (66 %) and 5 % reported PHQ-8 symptom scores of ≥10 suggesting possible moderate-severe depression. Two-thirds of the preschoolers resided in households located in the provinces in the Andean highlands (34 %) or the Pacific coastal plain region (34 %) with the remainder in Ecuadorian Amazonian provinces (29 %) or the Galapagos Islands (3 %). Most households reported using soap or detergent (90 %) for handwashing rather than water only or another substance such as ash, clay or sand.

Table 2. Child, maternal and household characteristics (n 7005)

Early childhood developmental delay

Based on their ECDI scores, 64 % of the preschool children were identified as being off-track in literacy-numeracy development, 21⋅5 % in social-emotional development, 3⋅3 % in physical development and 3⋅1 % in cognitive development. Eighteen percent were identified with global (overall) developmental delay (GDD) as they were off-track in two or more individual domains and 28 % showed no evidence of delay in any of the four developmental domains.

Table 3 displays the unadjusted findings from the log-binomial regression analyses examining the association of HFI with GDD and domain-specific development delay. Preschoolers living in households with marginal or moderate-severe HFI were 1⋅42 and 1⋅51 times more likely, respectively, than children from the food secure reference group to be identified with GDD. The table also shows findings from the analyses examining the association of HFI with delays in the four individual developmental domains. Children from households with either marginal or moderate HFI were about 1⋅5 times more likely than those from the reference group to show evidence of delayed social-emotional development. Those from households with moderate-severe but not marginal HFI were 1⋅10, 1⋅40 and 1⋅42 times more likely, respectively, to show evidence of delays in literacy-numeracy, physical or cognitive development compared to reference group children.

Table 3. Unadjusted association of HFI with off-track domain-specific and global development (n 7005)

PR,  prevalence ratio; %, weighted percent.

a P = 0⋅001.

b P = 0⋅03.

c P = 0⋅04, all other P-values > 0⋅05.

In addition to HFI, certain child, maternal and household characteristics were associated with developmental delays among the Ecuadorian preschoolers in the study. As Table 4 shows, younger children (36–47 months) were more likely than older ones (48–59 months) to be identified with GDD and delayed literacy-numeracy and social-emotional development. Male children were more likely to have GDD and delayed social-emotional development. Those reported as having adequate stimulation were less likely than those without it to be identified with GDD and developmental delays in all four individual domains. Children disciplined by household adults using physical or psychological violence were more likely to be identified with GDD and individual delays in the social-emotional and literacy-numeracy domains. The unadjusted findings also suggested that children with better-educated maternal caregivers were less likely to be identified with GDD and with domain-specific delays in literacy-numeracy, cognitive or social-emotional development compared to those with more poorly educated caregivers. Children whose maternal caregivers self-identified as an ethnoracial minority (indigenous, afro-descendant) were more likely to be classified with GDD and with delayed cognitive development.

Table 4. Unadjusted association of other child, maternal and household characteristics with off-track domain-specific and global development (n 7005)

PR , prevalence ratio; %, weighted percent.

a P = 0⋅001.

b P = 0⋅03.

c P = 0⋅01.

d P = 0⋅04.

e P = 0⋅002, all other P-values > 0⋅05.

The analyses also showed that GDD and delayed social-emotional development were more prevalent in the children whose maternal caregivers reported greater depressive symptoms. Children from households reporting the use of soap/detergent for handwashing rather than plain water or another substance were less likely to be identified with GDD or domain-specific delays in literacy-numeracy, cognitive or social-emotional development. In contrast, the region where a child lived was not associated with GDD nor any individual developmental delay.

Table 5 displays the findings from the adjusted log-binomial regression models suggesting that HFI was a major independent risk factor for global or overall developmental delay and one domain-specific delay in the Ecuadorian preschool children. Specifically, preschoolers from households having either marginal or moderate-severe HFI were 1⋅3–1⋅4 times more likely than those from food-secure homes to be identified with GDD or delayed social-emotional development after adjusting for model covariates. In addition to HFI, the adjusted analyses also identified certain other risk and protective factors independently associated with GDD and social-emotional developmental delay. Risk factors for these included younger age, being male, exposure to violent discipline and greater maternal caregiver depressive symptoms. In contrast, receiving adequate stimulation from household adults appeared protective against GDD and social-emotional developmental delay. Having a better-educated maternal caregiver also was associated with reduced GDD-.

Table 5. Adjusted association of HFI and other child, maternal and household characteristics with off-track global and domain-specific child development indicators (n 7005)

aPR, adjusted prevalence ratio.

a P = 0⋅001.

b P = 0⋅03.

c P = 0⋅005.

d P = 0⋅002.

e P = 0⋅05.

f P = 0⋅02.

g P = 0⋅009, all other P-values > 0⋅05.

h P = 0⋅007.

The findings also indicated that after adjustment for model covariates, the previously identified associations of HFI with literacy-numeracy, physical and cognitive developmental delays were no longer evident. However, certain child, maternal and/or household-level factors were independently associated in the adjusted analyses with delays in literacy-numeracy and cognitive development but not physical development. Risk factors for delayed literacy-numeracy development included younger child age, exposure to violent disciplinary measures and self-reported maternal minority group membership. In contrast, adequate stimulation and higher maternal caregiver education appeared protective. Adequate child simulation, higher maternal education and household handwashing with soap/detergent appeared protective against delayed cognitive development.

Discussion

This study is the first to report on the relationship between HFI and developmental delays in either Ecuadorian or other Latin American preschool child populations. It furthers our understanding of the influence of HFI on development during the second 1000 d of life and adds to the limited studies published on this important topic for preschool children living in LMICs. The 22 % prevalence of moderate-severe HFI identified for the child households is consistent with pre-pandemic estimates recorded for Ecuador and other LMICs in the Latin America region(23). The 18 % prevalence of suspected global developmental delay identified for the Ecuadorian preschoolers in this study was within the 11⋅4–18⋅9 % range recorded by other pre-covid pandemic national surveys for the same age group in other Latin American LMICs using the same assessment tool(Reference Gil, Ewerling and Ferreira2). Likewise, the prevalence of reported delays in the four individual developmental domains was also similar(Reference Gil, Ewerling and Ferreira2).

The study findings suggest that marginal and as well as moderate-severe HFI is independently associated with overall or global delays in development and, in particular, delays in the social-emotional development of preschool children. These findings underscore the importance of reducing exposure to any level of HFI during this important life course period. Our findings align with many, but not all, prior studies reporting on the association of HFI with ECDD in preschoolers in high-income populations(Reference Drennen, Coleman and Ettinger de Cuba37Reference Fuller, Caspary and Kagan43). They are also partially consistent with those reported by the two studies published on the relationship of HFI with social-emotional development indicators in preschool children from LMICs in other regions. One of these longitudinal studies reported that exposure to HFI was associated with greater externalising behaviour, an indicator of delayed social-emotional development in 4-year-old Pakistani children(Reference Aurino, Wolf and Tsinigo21). The other reported that although HFI was not associated with overall social-emotional development in Ghanese preschoolers, exposure to transitory HFI was marginally associated with poorer self-regulation, another social-emotional development indicator(Reference Jackson, Testa and Semenza15).

We surmise that HFI was associated with social-emotional and global developmental delays in the Ecuadorian preschool children due to its direct effects on child diet, nutrition and psychosocial pathways and/or indirect effects on that of maternal caregivers. A number of studies have linked HFI with lower dietary quality(Reference Landry, van den Berg and Asigbee44Reference Gassara and Chen47) and poorer nutritional status manifested as stunting(Reference Gassara and Chen47,Reference Schmeer and Piperata48) and nutritional anaemia in children(Reference Rocha, de Abreu and Lopes49,Reference Palacios-Rodríguez, Mundo-Rosas and Parra-Cabrera50) Such undernutrition can have harmful effects on the brain structures and processes of young children which manifests as poorer self-regulation and negative social behaviour(Reference Johnson and Markowitz16,Reference King38,Reference Black51) . In addition, parents are not always able to shield children from HFI(Reference Bernal, Frongillo and Herrera52,Reference Harvey53) . Meal skipping, portion size reductions and other coping strategies used by food-insecure households can cause hunger pangs and reduced blood glucose levels in children which promote increased irritability, distractedness and/or fatigue. In addition, feeling hunger pangs has been linked to behaviours in children such as a reluctance to share or engage in fair play(Reference Huppert, Shaw and Decety54). For these reasons, food insecure children may be less likely to engage in positive interactions with their caregivers, siblings or other adults and children. Furthermore, children living in food-insecure households are reported to often be aware of and stressed out by their home food situation(Reference Fram, Ritchie and Rosen55). The effects of this stress and the negative behavioural manifestations it may engender could also reduce the likelihood of positive social interactions of children with their caregivers, siblings and others in their households, at school and other social networks.

HFI may also indirectly affect overall development and social-emotional development in particular through its adverse effects on maternal caregivers. For example, maternal caregivers in food-insecure households often experience stress caused by concerns over being able to provide sufficient food for their children, other household members and themselves which can increase anxiety and depressive symptoms in vulnerable individuals. HFI has been linked to ethnospecific illnesses (nervousness, irritability, angry outbursts)(Reference Weigel, Armijos and Racines56) and depressive symptoms in Ecuadorian maternal caregivers(Reference Weigel, Armijos and Racines56Reference Wu, Harwood and Feng58). In addition, similar to children, insufficient caloric intake, meal skipping and other disordered eating patterns by adults in food-insecure households may promote irritability, distractibility and fatigue. Likewise, both hunger and low glucose levels are associated with negative emotionality in adults, i.e., increased impulsivity, anger, aggression(Reference Betley, Xu and Cao59,Reference Swami, Hochstöger and Kargl60) , which could cause maternal and other caregivers to be less responsive to the needs of their child and reduce positive parenting behaviours. Inadequate stimulation, less frequent positive interactions and low attachment by caregivers are well documented to have a negative impact on child development(Reference Johnson and Markowitz16,Reference King38,Reference Huang, Potochnick and Heflin61,Reference Pérez-Escamilla and Vianna62) . Another potential mechanism involves the effects of HFI on the micronutrient status of maternal caregivers. HFI has been linked to lower intakes and/or expenditures on micronutrient-rich foods and anaemia in maternal caregivers and other reproductive-age women in Ecuador(Reference Weigel, Armijos and Racines63,Reference Weigel and Armijos64) and other LMICs(Reference McDonald, McLean and Kroeun65,Reference Fischer, Shamah-Levy and Mundo-Rosas66) . Women with anaemia are more likely to suffer from depression, anxiety, other mental health disorders and sleep disturbances(Reference Vulser, Wiernik and Hoertel67) which also can impede the ability of maternal to positively interact with, stimulate or otherwise care for their child.

Different from social-emotional development, HFI was not associated with delayed literacy-numeracy and cognitive development in the Ecuadorian preschool children consistent with the mixed findings published on this topic in high-income(Reference Schwarzenberg and Georgieff12,Reference Brown and Jernigan13,Reference Finch, Yousafzai and Rasheed20) and LMIC populations(Reference Jackson, Testa and Semenza15,Reference Aurino, Wolf and Tsinigo21) . However, we did identify several other potentially modifiable household-level risk and protective factors associated with literacy-numeracy and/or cognitive developmental delays such as exposure to adequate stimulation or violent disciplinary measures, maternal education and handwashing behaviour. The reason for the lack of association of HFI or other independent risk or protective factors with physical developmental delay is uncertain but could be because this indicator is less susceptible to environmental influences compared to the three other developmental domains(Reference Schwarzenberg and Georgieff12).

Study strengths and limitations

This study had certain strengths and limitations that should be considered when interpreting its findings. Some strengths included a nationally representative sample with a large sample size, a low non-response rate (1⋅2 %) and the use of standardised data collection procedures and quality control methods(28). The Early Child Development Index (ECDI) is a widely used brief screener used to estimate the prevalence of suspected developmental delay in child populations. It has been validated in national surveys including in Latin American LMICs(Reference Loizillon, Petrowski and Britto29). Likewise, the PHQ-8 is a well-validated scale used to screen for depressive symptoms in Latin American and other diverse populations(Reference Wu, Levis and Riehm35,Reference Schantz, Reighard and Aikens68) . Another strength includes the use of multivariable analysis to adjust for child stimulation, violent disciplinary measures, maternal depressive symptoms and other factors known to influence ECDD as well as a novel factor, i.e., household handwashing behaviour, which has been linked to both ECD(Reference Ngure, Reid and Humphrey69) and HFI(Reference Choudhary, Schuster and Brewis70) but not examined in prior studies investigating the HFI-ECDD relationship.

One of the limitations was that the measurement of food security was restricted to the previous 12-month period, so it might not necessarily reflect the food situations of households over a longer period of time. Different persons living in the same household may experience different levels of food insecurity such as young children whom adults may try to protect from its consequences. Another limitation is that maternal caregivers were the source of information on child variables so reporting bias could have influenced the study results. It is also possible that maternal caregivers with higher levels of depressive symptoms or other mental health issues could make them more inclined to report socio-emotional and/or other developmental domain delays in their children. Moreover, while HFI may increase the risk for ECD through child and maternal mental health pathways, it is also possible that the relationship is bidirectional whereby these could also promote food insecurity in households. In this study, HFI was an important but not the only factor identified as influencing global development and three domain-specific delays. While we included a number of known confounders in our multivariable analysis, others were not because they were not contained in the survey database such as child and maternal micronutrient deficiencies and exposure to environmental toxicants known to adversely impact child development. Both nutritional anemias(22,27) and high blood levels of lead and other neurotoxicants(Reference Armijos, Weigel and Obeng-Gyasi71) are reported as highly prevalent among children and reproductive-age women in Ecuador. Future studies should include these factors when assessing the HFI-developmental delay relationship in preschoolers. Finally, while the findings allow for inference about the HFI-developmental delay relationship, the study's cross-sectional survey design does not permit establishing causal or temporal effects.

Conclusion

The preschool years represent a critical period of early child development that sets the stage for future success in social relationships, education and employment and other important life aspects. Our study findings suggest that HFI is an independent risk factor for global and social-emotional developmental delay in Ecuadorian preschool children. However, several other potentially modifiable household-level risk and protective factors were also associated with developmental delay in this population. In conclusion, our findings underscore the importance of strengthening and expanding existing national poverty reduction, food security and ECDD policies and interventions to improve the child opportunities for achieving their full developmental potential.

Acknowledgments

Both authors contributed to the study conception and design. The data were obtained and analysed by Dr. Weigel. Dr. Weigel also wrote the first draft of the manuscript. Both Dr. Weigel and Dr. Armijos participated in the review and writing of subsequent versions. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

This secondary analysis of publicly available hospital discharge data. received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors

There are no conflicts of interest.

The analysis of this publicly available, de-identified hospital discharge data is classified as, ‘exempt research not subject to human subject regulations,’ by the Indiana University Institutional Review Board (IRB).

The data used for the secondary analysis is published online by the Ecuadorian Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Censos (Spanish acronym: INEC). These de-identified data are freely available from the following public domain resource: Salud, Salud Reproductiva y Nutrición | (ecuadorencifras.gob.ec)

References

Black, MM, Walker, SP, Fernald, LCH, et al. (2017) Early childhood development coming of age: science through the life course. Lancet 389, 7790. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31389-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gil, JD, Ewerling, F, Ferreira, LZ, et al. (2020) Early childhood suspected developmental delay in 63 low- and middle-income countries: large within- and between-country inequalities documented using national health surveys. J Glob Health 10, 010427. doi:10.7189/jogh.10.010427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization (WHO). The 17 Goals. Geneva: WHO, THE 17 GOALS | Sustainable Development (un.org) (accessed 12 January 2023).Google Scholar
Sania, A, Sudfeld, CR, Danaei, G, et al. (2019) Early life risk factors of motor, cognitive and language development: a pooled analysis of studies from low/middle-income countries. BMJ Open 9, e026449. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2018-026449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, M, Tolar-Peterson, T, Reynolds, A, et al. (2022) The effects of nutritional interventions on the cognitive development of preschool-age children: a systematic review. Nutrients 14, 532. doi:10.3390/nu14030532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bliznashka, L, Perumal, N, Yousafzai, A, et al. (2022) Diet and development among children aged 36–59 months in low-income countries. Arch Dis Child 107, 719725. doi:10.1136/archdischild-2021-323218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCoy, DC, Peet, ED, Ezzati, M, et al. (2016) Early childhood developmental status in low- and middle-income countries: national, regional, and global prevalence estimates using predictive modeling. PLoS Med 13, e1002034. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1002034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
UNESCO. SDG indicator 4.2.3: Measurement of positive and stimulating home environments. TCG Fifth Meeting 15–16 November 2018 Mexico City, Mexico TCG5/REF/2. The Global Alliance to Monitor Learning Concept Note (unesco.org) (accessed 1 March 2023).Google Scholar
Crouch, E, Radcliff, E, Merrell, MA, et al. (2022) A national examination of poverty and interactive caregiving practices among parents of young children. J Child Fam Studies 31, 22662274. doi:10.1007/s10826-022-02349-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ward, KP, Lee, SJ, Grogan-Kaylor, AC, et al. (2022) Patterns of caregiver aggressive and nonaggressive discipline toward young children in low- and middle-income countries: a latent class approach. Child Abuse Negl 128, 105606. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2022.105606.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andersen, SA (ed) (1990) Core indicators of nutritional state for difficult-to-sample populations. J Nutr 120, 15591600. doi:10.1093/jn/120.suppl_11.1555.Google Scholar
Schwarzenberg, SJ, Georgieff, MK & Committee on Nutrition (2018) Advocacy for improving nutrition in the first 1000 days to support childhood development and adult health. Pediatrics 141, e20173716. doi:10.1542/peds.2017-3716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, TT & Jernigan, TL (2012) Brain development during the preschool years. Neuropsychol Rev 22, 313333. doi:10.1007/s11065-012-9214-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alderman, H & Fernald, L (2017) The nexus between nutrition and early childhood development. Annual Rev Nutr 37, 447476. doi:10.1146/annurev-nutr-071816-064627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, DB, Testa, A & Semenza, DC (2021) Household food insecurity and school readiness among preschool-aged children in the USA. Public Health Nutr 24, 14691477. doi:10.1017/S1368980021000550.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, AD & Markowitz, AJ (2018) Food insecurity and family well-being outcomes among households with young children. J Pediatr 196, 275282. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2018.01.026.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Oliveira, KHD, de Almeida, GM, Gubert, MB, et al. (2020) Household food insecurity and early childhood development: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Matern Child Nutr 16, e12967. doi:10.1111/mcn.12967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallegos, D, Eivers, A, Sondergeld, P, et al. (2021) Food insecurity and child development: a state-of-the-Art review. Int J Environ Res Public Health 18, 8990. doi:10.3390/ijerph18178990.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shankar, P, Chung, R & Frank, DA (2017) Association of food insecurity with children's behavioral, emotional, and academic outcomes: a systematic review. J Develop Beh Pediatrics 38, 135150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finch, JE, Yousafzai, AK, Rasheed, M, et al. (2018) Measuring and understanding social-emotional behaviors in preschoolers from rural Pakistan. PLoS One 13, e0207807. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0207807.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aurino, E, Wolf, S & Tsinigo, E (2020) Household food insecurity and early childhood development: longitudinal evidence from Ghana. PLoS One 15, e0230965. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0230965.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
FAO/IFAD/PAHO/UNICEF/WFP (2023) Regional Overview of Food Security and Nutrition-Latin America and the Caribbean 2022: Towards Improving Affordability of Healthy Diets. Santiago: Pan American Health Organization (PAHO). doi:10.4060/cc3859en (accessed 3 March 2023).Google Scholar
FAO/IFAD/UNICEF/WFP/WHO (2022) The State of Food Security and Nutrition in the World 2022. Repurposing Food and Agricultural Policies to Make Healthy Diets More Affordable. Rome: FAO. doi:10.4060/cc0639en (accessed 14 January 2023).Google Scholar
Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Censos (2022) Encuesta Nacional de Empleo, Desempleo y Subempleo 2022 (ENEMDU) Indicadores de Pobreza y Desigualdad Junio, 2022. Boletín Técnico N° 11-2022-ENEMDU. 202206_Boletin_pobreza.pdf (ecuadorencifras.gob.ec) (Accessed 21 November 2022).Google Scholar
Handal, AJ, Lozoff, B, Breilh, J, et al. (2007) Sociodemographic and nutritional correlates of neurobehavioral development: a study of young children in a rural region. Rev Panam Salud Publica 21, 292300. doi:10.1590/s1020-49892007000400004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weigel, MM & Armijos, RX (2018) Household food insecurity and psychosocial dysfunction in Ecuadorian elementary schoolchildren. Int J Pediatr 2018, 6067283. doi:10.1155/2018/6067283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Censos (2018) Documento Metodológico de la Encuesta Nacional de Salud y Nutrición (ENSANUT). Metodologia ENSANUT 2018.pdf (ecuadorencifras.gob.ec) (Accessed 16 September 2022).Google Scholar
Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Censos (2018) Metodología de Diseño Muestral de la Encuesta Nacional de Salud y Nutrición (ENSANUT). https://www.ecuadorencifras.gob.ec/documentos/web-inec/Estadisticas_Sociales/ENSANUT/ENSANUT_2018/Metodologia%20del%20disenio%20muestral%20ENSANUT%202018.pdf (Accessed 20 June 2023).Google Scholar
Loizillon, A, Petrowski, N, Britto, P, et al. (2017). Development of the early childhood development index in MICS surveys. MICS Methodological Papers, No. 6, Data and Analytics Section, Division of Data, Research and Policy, UNICEF New York. files (unicef.org) (accessed 15 December 2022).Google Scholar
Food and Agriculture Organization (2017) The Food Insecurity Experience Scale: Measuring food insecurity through people's experiences (fao.org) (accessed 23 September 2023).Google Scholar
United Nations Children's Fund (UNICEF) (2018) Multiple Indicator Cluster Surveys (MICS). Questionnaire for Children Under Five. Tools – UNICEF MICS (accessed 18 October 2023).Google Scholar
United Nations Children's Fund (UNICF) (2021). SDG indicator 4.2.3: Measurement of Positive and Stimulating Home Environments TCG Fifth Meeting 15–16 November 2018 Mexico City, Mexico TCG5/REF/2. The Global Alliance to Monitor Learning Concept Note (unesco.org).Google Scholar
United Nations Children's Fund (UNICEF) (2010) Child Disciplinary Practices at Home. Evidence From A Range of Low- and Middle-Income Countries. New York: UNICEF, ChildDiscipline_report_Eng_44 (4).pdf (accessed 20 December 2022).Google Scholar
Kroenke, K, Strine, TW, Spitzer, RL, et al. (2009) The PHQ-8 as a measure of current depression in the general population. J Affect Disord 114, 163173. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2008.06.026.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, Y, Levis, B, Riehm, KE, et al. (2020) Equivalency of the diagnostic accuracy of the PHQ-8 and PHQ-9: a systematic review and individual participant data meta-analysis. Psychol Med 50, 13681380. doi:10.1017/S0033291719001314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
United Nations Children's Fund (UNICEF) and World Health Organization (WHO) (2018) Core Questions on Drinking Water, Sanitation and Hygiene for Household Surveys: 2018 Update. New York: UNICEF and WHO, JMP-2018-core-questions-for-household-surveys.pdf (washdata.org) (accessed 30 November 2022).Google Scholar
Drennen, CR, Coleman, SM, Ettinger de Cuba, S, et al. (2019) Food insecurity, health, and development in children under age four years. Pediatrics 144, e20190824. doi: 10.1542/peds.2019-0824.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, C (2018) Food insecurity and child behavior problems in fragile families. Econ Hum Biol 28, 1422. doi:10.1016/j.ehb.2017.11.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitaker, RC, Phillips, SM & Orzol, SM (2006) Food insecurity and the risks of depression and anxiety in mothers and behavior problems in their preschool-aged children. Pediatrics 118, e859e868. doi:10.1542/peds.2006-0239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slack, KS, Yoo, J (2004) Food hardship and child behavior problems among low-income children. Institute for Research on Poverty Discussion Paper no. 1290–04. Effect of food insecurity on children's externalizing behaviors (3–5yrs) (wisc.edu) (accessed 1 March 2023).Google Scholar
Weinreb, L, Wehler, C, Perloff, J, et al. (2002) Hunger: Its impact on children's health and mental health. Pediatrics 110, e41. doi:10.1542/peds.110.4.e41, PMID: 12359814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagata, JM, Gomberg, S, Hagan, MJ, et al. (2018) Food insecurity is associated with maternal depression and child pervasive developmental symptoms in low-income Latino households. J Hunger Environ Nut 14, 526539. doi:10.1080/19320248.2018.1434101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuller, B, Caspary, G, Kagan, SL, et al. (2002) Does maternal employment influence poor children's social development? Early Childhood Res Q 7, 470497. doi:10.1016/S0885-2006(02)00187-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landry, MJ, van den Berg, AE, Asigbee, FM, et al. (2019) Child-report of food insecurity is associated with diet quality in children. Nutrients 11, 1574. doi:10.3390/nu11071574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jun, S, Cowan, AE, Dodd, KW, et al. (2021) Association of food insecurity with dietary intakes and nutritional biomarkers among US children, national health and nutrition examination survey (NHANES) 2011–2016. Am J Clin Nutr 114, 10591069. doi:10.1093/ajcn/nqab113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jun, S, Zeh, MJ, Eicher-Miller, HA, et al. (2019) Children's dietary quality and micronutrient adequacy by food security in the household and among household children. Nutrients 11, 965. doi:10.3390/nu11050965.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gassara, G & Chen, J (2021) Household food insecurity, dietary diversity, and stunting in sub-Saharan Africa: a systematic review. Nutrients 13, 4401. doi:10.3390/nu13124401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmeer, KK & Piperata, BA (2017) Household food insecurity and child health. Matern Child Nutr 13, e12301. doi:10.1111/mcn.12301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rocha, ÉMB, de Abreu, LC, Lopes, AF, et al. (2018) Relation of food insecurity and hemoglobin level in preschool aged children. Anemia 2018, 3950687. doi:10.1155/2018/3950687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palacios-Rodríguez, GO, Mundo-Rosas, V, Parra-Cabrera, S, et al. (2012) Household food insecurity and its association with anaemia in Mexican children: national health and nutrition survey 2012. Int J Public Health 64, 12151222. doi:10.1007/s00038-019-01305-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Black, MM (2018) Impact of nutrition on growth, brain, and cognition. Nestle Nutr Inst Workshop Series 89, 185195. doi:10.1159/000486502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernal, J, Frongillo, EA, Herrera, H, et al. (2012) Children live, feel, and respond to experiences of food insecurity that compromise their development and weight status in peri-urban Venezuela. J Nutr 142, 13431349. doi:10.3945/jn.112.158063.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, K (2016) “When I go to bed hungry and sleep, I'm not hungry”: children and parents’ experiences of food insecurity. Appetite 99, 235244. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2016.01.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huppert, E, Shaw, A & Decety, J (2020) The effect of hunger on children's sharing behavior and fairness preferences. J Exp Child Psychol 192, 104786. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fram, MS, Ritchie, LD, Rosen, N, et al. (2015) Child experience of food insecurity is associated with child diet and physical activity. J Nutr 145, 499504. doi:10.3945/jn.114.194365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weigel, MM, Armijos, RX, Racines, M, et al. (2016) Association of household food insecurity with the mental and physical health of low-income urban Ecuadorian women with children. J Environ Public Health 2016, 5256084. doi:10.1155/2016/5256084.Google ScholarPubMed
Gebreyesus, SH, Endris, BS, Hanlon, C, et al. (2018) Maternal depression symptoms are highly prevalent among food-insecure households in Ethiopia. Public Health Nutr 21, 849856. doi:10.1017/S1368980017003056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, Q, Harwood, RL & Feng, X (2018) Family socioeconomic status and maternal depressive symptoms: mediation through household food insecurity across five years. Soc Sci Med 215, 16. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.08.043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Betley, JN, Xu, S, Cao, ZFH, et al. (2015) Neurons for hunger and thirst transmit a negative-valence teaching signal. Nature 521, 180185. doi:10.1038/nature14416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swami, V, Hochstöger, S, Kargl, E, et al. (2022) Hangry in the field: an experience sampling study on the impact of hunger on anger, irritability, and affect. PLoS One 17, e0269629. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0269629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang, Y, Potochnick, S & Heflin, CM (2018) Household food insecurity and childhood health and cognitive development among children of immigrants. Journal of Family Issues 39, 14651497. doi:10.1177/0192513X17710772.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Escamilla, R & Vianna, RPT (2012) Food insecurity and the behavioral and intellectual development of children: A review of the evidence. J Applied Res Child 3, 116. http://digitalcommons.library.tmc.edu/childrenatrisk/vol3/iss1/9.Google Scholar
Weigel, MM, Armijos, RX, Racines, M, et al. (2016) Food insecurity is associated with undernutrition but not overnutrition in Ecuadorian women from low-income urban neighborhoods. J Environ Public Health 2016, 8149459. doi:10.1155/2016/8149459.Google Scholar
Weigel, MM & Armijos, MM (2015) Food insufficiency in the households of reproductive-age Ecuadorian women: association with food and nutritional status indicators. Ecol Food Nutr 54, 2042. doi:10.1080/03670244.2014.953249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, CM, McLean, J, Kroeun, H, et al. (2015) Household food insecurity and dietary diversity as correlates of maternal and child undernutrition in rural Cambodia. Eur J Clin Nutr 69, 242246. doi:10.1038/ejcn.2014.161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, NC, Shamah-Levy, T, Mundo-Rosas, V, et al. (2014) Household food insecurity is associated with anemia in adult Mexican women of reproductive age. J Nutr 144, 20662072. doi:10.3945/jn.114.197095.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vulser, H, Wiernik, E, Hoertel, N, et al. (2016) Association between depression and anemia in otherwise healthy adults. Acta Psychiatr Scand 134, 150160. doi:10.1111/acps.12595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schantz, K, Reighard, C, Aikens, JE, et al. (2017) Screening for depression in Andean Latin America: factor structure and reliability of the CES-D short form and the PHQ-8 among Bolivian public hospital patients. Int J Psychiatry Med 52, 315327. doi: 10.1177/0091217417738934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ngure, FM, Reid, BM, Humphrey, JH, et al. (2014) Water, sanitation, and hygiene (WASH), environmental enteropathy, nutrition, and early child development: making the links. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1308, 118128. doi:10.1111/nyas.12330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choudhary, N, Schuster, RC, Brewis, A, et al. (2021) Household water insecurity affects child nutrition through alternative pathways to WASH: Evidence from India. Food Nutr Bull 42, 170187. doi:10.1177/0379572121998122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Armijos, RX, Weigel, MM, Obeng-Gyasi, E, et al. (2021) Elevated blood lead and metal/metalloid levels and environmental exposure sources in urban Ecuadorian school-age children and mothers. Int J Hyg Environ Health 235, 113770. doi:10.1016/j.ijheh.2021.113770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Early childhood development index (ECDI) milestones and coding: domain-specific and global developmenta

Figure 1

Table 2. Child, maternal and household characteristics (n 7005)

Figure 2

Table 3. Unadjusted association of HFI with off-track domain-specific and global development (n 7005)

Figure 3

Table 4. Unadjusted association of other child, maternal and household characteristics with off-track domain-specific and global development (n 7005)

Figure 4

Table 5. Adjusted association of HFI and other child, maternal and household characteristics with off-track global and domain-specific child development indicators (n 7005)