Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T08:28:23.621Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How Will Route Guidance Information Affect Cognitive Maps?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 October 2009

P. G. Jackson
Affiliation:
(Centre for Transport Studies, Imperial College, London)

Abstract

This paper considers the effect of receiving a secondary source of information upon ability to acquire and use spatial knowledge (i.e. the formation of a cognitive map). Participants watch videos of three journeys through an unfamiliar area. They then perform tasks measuring ability to integrate the routes into a cognitive map of the area. Subjects viewing the routes whilst hearing route guidance information performed consistently worse than those receiving no guidance. The presence of non-salient information, however, did not impair performance.

Existing studies highlighting differences between gender and age groups regarding spatial ability receive only partial support: the results suggest that ability to drive negates these effects. Female drivers perform as well as, if not better than, their male counterparts, and outperform male non-drivers. This same ‘driving’ effect was also evident in the over-55s group, with both male and female drivers performing significantly better than non-drivers in this age group. The results also show that driving ability enhances ability to cope with and use guidance information, suggesting that systems’ designers should make varying levels of spatial information available for less experienced drivers.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal Institute of Navigation 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Burnett, G. E. and Joyner, S. M. (1994). An assessment of route guidance systems and their implications for driver safety. In Proceedings of International Ergonomics Association Conference, Vol. 4, pp. 156158, 1014 August, 1994, Toronto, Canada.Google Scholar
Dingus, T. A. and Hulse, M. C. (1993). Some human factors design issues and recommendations for automobile navigation information systems. Transportation Research C, 1 (2), 119131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, G. W. (1980). Environmental cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 259287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golledge, R. G. (1987). Environmental cognition. In Handbook of Environmental Psychology, Vol 1 (ed. D., Stokols and I., Altman), pp. 131174. Wiley, New York.Google Scholar
Golledge, R. G.Smith, T. R.Pellegrino, J. W.Doherty, S. and Marshall, S. P. (1985). A conceptual model and empirical analysis of children's acquisition of spatial knowledge. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 5 (2), 125152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golledge, R. G.Ruggles, A. J.Pellegrino, J. W. and Gale, N. D. (1993). Integrating route knowledge in an unfamiliar neighborhood: along and cross-route experiments. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 13 (4), 293307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gotts, N. M. and Bonsall, P. W. (1993). A new technique for investigating drivers’ response to In-Vehicle Route Guidance Information (IVRGI) Systems. Paper presented at the 25th Annual Conference of the Universities’ Transport Study Group, University of Southampton, January 1993.Google Scholar
Horowitz, J. L. (1984). The stability of stochastic equilibrium in a two-link transportation network. Transportation Research B, 18 (1), 1328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, P. G. (1994). Experimental procedures for studying the updating of spatial knowledge. In Proceedings of the First Erasmus-Network Conference on Transportation and Traffic Engineering (ed. Mortelmanns, J. F.), 46 September, Kerkrade, Netherlands. Acco, Leuven.Google Scholar
Jackson, P. G. (1995). Using video to simulate wayfinding and the effects of route guidance information. Paper presented to the 25th Annual Conference of the Universities Transport Studies Group, 46 January 1995, Cranfield University.Google Scholar
Kitchin, R. M. (1994). Cognitive maps: what are they and why study them? Journal of Environmental Psychology, 14 (1), 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schofer, J. L.Khattak, A. and Koppelman, F. S. (1993). Behavioral issues in the design and evaluation of Advanced Traveller Information Systems. Transport Research C, 1 (2), 107117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Srinivasan, R.Jovanis, P.Yang, C-Z. and Kitamura, R. (1994). Simulation tests of driving performance with selected route guidance devices. In Proceedings of International Ergonomics Association Conference, Vol. 4, pp. 151155, 1014 August, 1994, Toronto, Canada.Google Scholar
Staughton, G. C. and Storie, V. J. (1977). Methodology of an in-depth accident investigation survey. Report LR 762, Transport Road Research Laboratory, Crowthorne, Berks.Google Scholar
Streeter, L. A. Vitello, D. and Wonsiewicz, S. (1986). How to tell people where to go: comparing navigational aids. International Journal of Man/Machine Interaction, 22 (4), 549562.Google Scholar
Van Winsum, W.Alm, H.Schraagen, J. M. and Rothengatter, T. (1990). Laboratory and field studies on route representation and drivers’ cognitive models of routes. Drive project V1041 Generic Intelligent Driver Support Systems, Traffic Research Centre VSC, University of Groningen, Netherlands.Google Scholar
Wierda, M. and Aasman, J. (1992). Seeing and driving: computation, algorithms and implementation. Report No. VK 91–06, Traffic Research Centre VSC, University of Groningen, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
Wierwille, W. W. (1993). Demands on drivers’ resources associated with introducing advanced technology into the vehicle. Transportation Research C, 1 (2), 133142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar