Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T11:55:12.647Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sensory Deprivation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2018

J. G. Thorpe*
Affiliation:
Banstead Hospital, Sutton, Surrey

Extract

Exploratory experimental work into the effects of sensory deprivation on human subjects has developed rapidly during the past decade and many reports have been published. It would appear to be necessary, however, to pause for a moment and to review the work which has so far been executed with a view to facilitating the systematic investigation of this phenomenon.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1961 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

VIII.—References

1 Azima, H., and Cramer, F. J., “Effects of partial perceptual isolation in mentally-disturbed individuals”, Dis. nerv. Syst., 1956, 17, 38.Google Scholar
2 Bexton, W. H., Heron, W., and Scott, T. H., “Effects of decreased variation in the sensory environment”, Canad. J. Psychol., 1954, 8, 7076.Google Scholar
3 Bruner, J. S., “The cognitive consequences of early sensory deprivation”, Psychosomat. med., 1959, 21, 89.Google Scholar
4 Clarke, A. D. B., and Clarke, A., “Some recent advances in the study of early deprivation”, J. Child Psychol. Psychiat., 1960, 1, 2636.Google Scholar
5 Doane, B. K., Mahatoo, W., Heron, W., and Scott, T. H., “Changes in perceptual function after isolation”, Canad. J. Psychol., 1959, 13, 210219.Google Scholar
6 Eysenck, H. J., The Dynamics of Anxiety and Hysteria, 1957. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
7 Foulds, G. A., “Clinical research in psychiatry”, J. Ment. Sci., 1958, 104, 259265.Google Scholar
8 Freedman, S. J., “Perceptual changes in sensory deprivation: suggestions for a conative theory”, J. nerv. ment. Dis., 1961, 132, 1721.Google Scholar
9 Gibby, R. G., Adams, H. B., and Carrera, R. N., “Therapeutic changes in psychiatric patients following partial sensory deprivation”, A.M.A. Arch. Gen. Psychiat., 1960, 3, 3342.Google Scholar
10 Harris, A., “Sensory deprivation and schizophrenia”, J. ment. Sci., 1959, 105, 235237.Google Scholar
11 Hebb, D. O., Discussion of papers inSensory deprivation: facts in search of a theory”, J. nerv. ment. Dis., 1961, 132, 4043.Google Scholar
12 Held, R., “Exposure history as a factor in maintaining stability of perception and co-ordination”, ibid., 1961, 132, 2632.Google Scholar
13 Lilly, J. C., “Mental effects of reduction of ordinary levels of physical stimuli on intact, healthy persons”, Psychiat. Res. Rep., 1956, 5, 19.Google Scholar
14 Mendelson, J., and Foley, J., “An abnormality of mental function affecting patients with poliomyelitis in a tank type respirator”, Tr. Amer. Neurol. Assoc., 1956.Google Scholar
15 Petrie, A., Collins, W., and Solomon, P., “Pain sensitivity, sensory deprivation, and susceptibility to satiation”, Science, 1958, 128, 14311433.Google Scholar
16 Iidem , “The tolerance for pain and for sensory deprivation”, Amer. J. Psychol., 1960, 73, 8090.Google Scholar
17 Idem , “Some psychological aspects of pain and the relief of suffering”, Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 1960, 86, 1327.Google Scholar
18 Riesen, A. H., “Studying perceptual development using the technique of sensory deprivation”, J. nerv. ment. Dis., 1961, 132, 2125.Google Scholar
19 Scott, T. H., Bexton, W. H., Heron, W., and Doane, B. K., “Cognitive effects of perceptual isolation”, Canad. J. Psychol., 1959, 13, 200209.Google Scholar
20 Shurley, J. T., “Profound experimental sensory isolation”, Amer. J. Psychiat., 1960, 117, 539545.Google Scholar
21 Solomon, P., Herbert, L., Mendelson, J., and Wexler, D., “Sensory deprivation: A review”, ibid., 1957, 114, 357363.Google Scholar
22 Teuber, H. L., “Sensory deprivation, sensory suppression, and agnosia: notes for a neurologic theory”, J. nerv. ment. Dis., 1961, 132, 3240.Google Scholar
23 Vernon, J., and Hoffman, J., “Effect of sensory deprivation on learning rate in human beings”, Science, 1956, 123, 10741075.Google Scholar
24 Idem , McGill, T. E., and Schiffman, H., “Visual hallucinations during perceptual isolation”, Canad. J. Psychol., 1958, 12, 3134.Google Scholar
25 Vernon, J., and McGill, T. E., “Sensory deprivation and pain thresholds”, Science, 1961, 133, 330331.Google Scholar
26 Wertheimer, M., “Figural after-effect as a measure of metabolic efficiency”, J. Pers., 1955, 24, 56.Google Scholar
27 Wexler, D., Mendelson, J., and Leiderman, P. H., “Sensory deprivation: A technique for studying psychiatric aspects of stress”, A.M.A. Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., 1958, 79, 225233.Google Scholar
28 Williams, H. L., Lubin, A., and Goodnow, J. J., “Impaired performance with acute sleep loss”, Psychol. Mon. (Gen. and App.), 1959, 73, No. 14.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.