Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T14:27:11.810Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Drugs and Personality

IV. The Effects of Stimulant and Depressant Drugs on the Rate of Fluctuation of a Reversible Perspective Figure

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2018

H. J. Eysenck
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry (Maudsley Hospital), University of London
H. Holland
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry (Maudsley Hospital), University of London
D. S. Trouton
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry (Maudsley Hospital), University of London

Extract

In the first paper of this series, it was pointed out that one of the reasons why McDougall's theory of drug action and personality was not accepted at all widely was connected with the fact that he failed to provide an objective, experimental test which could be used to diagnose extraversion-introversion, and to assess drug effects. This argument is not entirely correct; McDougall did in fact suggest one such test, namely the rate of fluctuation of so-called reversible perspective figures. Many varieties of these are known, and have been used experimentally; the Necker cube, the staircase, the vase-face, and the windmill patterns being probably the best known. In all of these, there is an ambiguity in the drawing which makes it possible to perceive two distinct patterns in the stimulus; on prolonged inspection these patterns alternate, and it is the rate of alternation, signalled verbally or by suitable mechanical arrangement, which constitutes the score on this test. It is known that different types of pattern give reasonably reliable scores, and also that rates of alternation on different patterns correlate quite highly together, thus demonstrating that one and the same tendency is being measured. That this tendency is of central rather than peripheral character is indicated by the fact that changes in the rate of reversal due to fatigue and other causes can be transferred from one eye to the other.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1957 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

1 Bergman, P. S., Nathanson, M., and Bender, M. B., “Electrical recordings of normal and abnormal eye movements modified by drugs”, A.M.A. Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., 1952, 67, 357374.Google Scholar
2 Brown, K. J., “Studies of apparent change as a function of observation time, using a new type of dynamic ambiguous figure”, WADC Technical Rep., 1954, 54139.Google Scholar
3 Carlson, V. R., “Satiation in a reversible perspective figure”, J. exp. Psychol., 1953, 45, 442448.Google Scholar
4 Ewen, J. H., “The psychological estimation of the effects of certain drugs upon the syntonic and schizophrenic psychoses”, J. Ment. Sci., 1931, 77, 742766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
5 Eysenck, H. J., “Drugs and personality: I. Theory and methodology”, J. Ment. Sci., 1957, 103, 119131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6 Idem , Holland, H., and Trouton, D. S., “Drugs and personality: III. The effects of stimulant and depressant drugs on visual after-effects”, J. Ment. Sci., 1957, 103, 650.Google Scholar
7 Eysenck, H. J., “A dynamic theory of anxiety and hysteria”, J. Ment. Sci., 1955, 101, 2851.Google Scholar
8 Idem , “Cortical inhibition, figural after-effect, and the theory of personality”, J. abn. soc. Psychol., 1955, 51, 94106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9 George, R. W., “The significance of the fluctuations experienced in observing ambiguous figures and in binocular rivalry”, J. gen. Psychol., 1936, 15, 3961.Google Scholar
10 Guilford, J. G., and Braly, K. W., “An experimental test of extraversion-introversion”, J. abn. soc. Psychol., 1930, 25, 382389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
11 Hochberg, J. E., “Figure-ground reversal as a function of visual satiation”, J. exp. Psychol., 1950, 40, 682686.Google Scholar
12 Lebensohn, J. E., and Sullivan, R. R., “Temporary stimulation of emmetropic visual acuity”, U.S. Naval med. Bull., 1944, 43, 9095.Google Scholar
13 McDougall, W., “The chemical theory of temperament applied to introversion and extraversion”, J. abn. soc. Psychol., 1929, 24, 293309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14 McDougall, W., and Smith, M., The effects of alcohol and some other drugs during normal and fatigued conditions, 1920. London: H.M.S.O.Google Scholar
15 Payne, R. W., An investigation into the possibility of defining “dissociation” as a personality trait by means of adjective tests, 1957. London: Ph.D. Thesis.Google Scholar
16 Reifenstein, E. C. Jr., and Davidoff, E., “The psychological effects of benzedrine sulfate”, Amer. J. Psychol., 1939, 52, 5664.Google Scholar
17 Tainter, M. L., “Effects of certain analeptic drugs on spontaneous running activity of the white rat”, J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1943, 36, 143155.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.