Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T12:18:32.476Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Contribution to the Objective Measurement of the Cathartic Process—I

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2018

Frieda Goldman-Eisler*
Affiliation:
Medical Research Council Unit for Research in Occupational Adaptation, Maudsley Hospital

Extract

In previous work (8, 9) the writer has found that measures of breathing activity during speech, and of output of speech per breath are related to the emotional intensity and to the extent to which emotions are restrained or given free expression in the exchanges during psychiatric interviews. A high level of ventilation, i.e. high breathing activity with low output of speech per breath was shown to belong to content implying outgoing affect (jealousy, appetite, sex, aggression, wishes and hopes), and a low level of ventilation in speech to be associated with topics of restricted emotionality, such as fear, inhibition, deprivation, as well as to reasoning.

Tension states as rated by physicians or as experienced subjectively, as well as anxiety states and other emotional disturbances have been shown to be associated with physiological changes in the muscular system as measured by the electromyograph (15, 19, 21). Increased muscular tension was also found to accompany hyperventilation (1, 6) and increased muscular tension as well as inhibited breathing have been reported to accompany intellectual effort (11, 22, 23). Rising gradients of tension from forehead and chin have recently been shown to accompany sustained attention when listening to read material (26).

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1956 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Altschule, M. D., Bodily Physiology in Mental and Emotional Disorders, 1953. Chapter: Respiration. New York: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
Baldwin, A. L., “Statistical Problems in the Treatment of Case Histories,” J. Clin. Psychol., 1950, 4, 612.Google Scholar
Bernfield, Siegfried, Freud's Earliest Theories and the School of Helmholtz, The Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 1944, 13, 341362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breuer, and Freud, , “Über den Psychischen Mechanismus Hysterischer Phänomene, Vorläufige Mitteilung,” 1893. Ges. Sch. I. Google Scholar
Breuer, and Freud, , Studies in Hysteria, Chapter III: Theoretical Material Published (1944) in Nervous and Mental Disease Monographs: New York.Google Scholar
Clausen, J., Acta Psychiatrica et Neurologica, 1951. Supplementum 68. Copenhagen, Ejnar Munksgaard.Google Scholar
Freud, Sigmund, The Origins of Psychoanalysis, 1954. Chapter: Project for a Scientific Psychology. London: Imago Publishing Co., Ltd.Google Scholar
Goldman-Eisler, Frieda, “Speech-Breathing Activity—a Measure of Tension and Affect during Interviews,” Brit. J. Psychol., 1955, 46, 5363.Google Scholar
Idem , “Speech-Breathing Activity and Content in Psychiatric Interviews,” Brit. J. Med. Psychol., 1956, 29, Part I.Google Scholar
Goldman, S., Information Theory, 1953. London: Constable & Co., Ltd.Google Scholar
Golla, F. L., and Antonovitch, B. S., “The Respiratory Rhythm in its Relation to the Mechanism of Thought,” Brain, 1929, 52, 491509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham-Brown, T., “On the Nature of the Fundamental Activity of the Nervous Centers,” J. Physiol., 1914, 48, 1846.Google Scholar
Hoagland, Hudson, Pacemakers in Relation to Aspects of Behaviour, 1935. New York: The Macmillan Co.Google Scholar
Jackson, Hughlings, On Affections of Speech from Disease of the Brain, 1932. Selected Writings, Vol. II. London: Hodder & Stoughton, Ltd. Google Scholar
Jacobsen, E., Progressive Relaxation, 1938. Pp. 494. Inc. ed., Chicago Google Scholar
Kubie, L. S., “Instinct and Homeostasis,” J. Psychosomatic Med., 1948, 10, 1530.Google Scholar
Idem , “The Central Representation of the Symbolic Process in Psychosomatic Disorders”, J. Psychosomatic Med., 1953, 15, 17.Google Scholar
Lashley, K. S., The Problem of Serial Order in Behaviour, 1951. In: “Cerebral Mechanisms in Behaviour.” The Hixon Symposium, New York and London.Google Scholar
Malmo, R. B., and Shagass, C., “Physiologic Studies of Reaction to Stress in Anxiety and Early Schizophrenia,” J. Psychosomatic Med., 1949, 11, 929.Google Scholar
Reich, Wilhelm, Character-Analysis, 1949. New York: Orgone Institute Press.Google Scholar
Sainsbury, P., and Gibson, J. G., “On Symptoms of Anxiety and Tension and Physiological Changes in the Muscular System Accompanying Them,” J. Neurol., Neurosurg. and Psychiatry, 1954, 17, 216224.Google Scholar
Schilling, R., Über “Inneres Sprechen,” Zschr. f. Psychologie III, 1929, 204255.Google Scholar
Sitter, J., “Die Beziehung zwischen Aufmerksamkeit und Atmung,” Archiv. für die Gesamte Psychologie, 1912, 25, 78150.Google Scholar
Tintner, G., “The Analysis of Economic Time Series,” J. Amer. Statistical Association, 1940, 35, 93100.Google Scholar
Idem , The Variate Difference Method, 1940. Bloomington, Indiana: Principia Press Inc.Google Scholar
Wallerstein, H., “An Electromyographic Study of Attentive Listening,” Canadian J. Psychol., 1954, 8, 228238.Google Scholar
Wiener, N., Extrapolation, Interpolation and Smoothing of Stationary Time Series, 1949. New York, London. The Technology Press of the Mass. Instit. of Technology, and John Wiley & Sons, Inc., New York. Chapman & Hall, Ltd., London.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.