Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-29T19:17:01.278Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Re-Evaluation of the Fugue

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2018

W. P. Berrington
Affiliation:
Downshire Hospital, Downpatrick, Northern Ireland
G. A. Foulds
Affiliation:
Runwell Hospital, near Wickford, Essex

Extract

The fugue has received little recognition in the literature in recent years. Stengel (1941, 1943) defines the fugue state as consisting of “transitory abnormal behaviour characterized by aimless wandering, and more or less marked alteration of consciousness, usually, but not necessarily, followed by amnesia”, According to Henderson and Gillespie (1944) the patient in a true fugue suddenly leaves his previous activity and goes on a journey which has no apparent relation to what he has just been doing, and for which he has complete amnesia afterwards. William McDougall (1926) states: “Fugue is the name given to those instances in which a person suddenly disappears from his accustomed haunts and re-appears at some distant place, astonished and puzzled to find himself there, and unable to give any account of himself in the period between his disappearance and his re-appearance.” In a footnote McDougall adds: “This word is sometimes used in a wider sense to include wandering of a seemingly aimless kind for which the subject (generally a psychopath) can assign no adequate reason or motive, but of which, nevertheless, he can give some description.” In our cases the criteria for a fugue have been sudden onset of wandering with clouding of consciousness and a more or less complete amnesia for the event.

Over the past 3 years we have collected 37 cases, either from the hospital records or from cases personally seen by us. Interesting features have emerged which, as far as we can ascertain, have not previously been described. A control group matched for sex, age and clinical diagnosis has been studied to exclude fortuitous associations. In the 37 cases of fugue under review, 31 are men and only 6 women. This incidence in favour of males is in accordance with the findings of other workers who have dealt with mixed groups, but differs from Stengel (1941), who picked his cases mainly from the female wards of Bristol Mental Hospital.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1956 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abeles, M., and Schilder, P., Amer. Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., 1935, 34, 587604.Google Scholar
Gill, M., and Rapaport, D., Unpublished paper quoted by Rapaport, D., in Emotions and Memory, 1950. New York.Google Scholar
Henderson, D. K., and Gillespie, R. D., Textbook of Psychiatry, 1944. London.Google Scholar
Janet, P., The Major Symptoms of Hysteria, 1907. London.Google Scholar
Kanzer, M., Amer. J. Psychiat., 1939, 96, 711.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDougall, W., Outline of Abnormal Psychology, 1926. London.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGeoch, J. A., in Introduction to Psychology, 1939. New York.Google Scholar
Parfitt, D. N., and Carlyle-Gall, C. M., J. ment. Sci., 1944, 90, 379.Google Scholar
Rapaport, D., Emotions and Memory, 1950. New York.Google Scholar
Sargant, W., and Slater, E., Proc. Roy. Soc. Med., 1941, 34, 757.Google Scholar
Stengel, E., J. ment. Sci., 1941, 87, 512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Idem , ibid., 1943, 89, 375.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.