Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T10:45:23.171Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Parotid Gland Secretion in Affective Mental Disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2018

H. J. Eysenck
Affiliation:
Mill Hill Emergency Hospital
P.-M. Yap
Affiliation:
Mill Hill Emergency Hospital

Extract

Parotid gland secretion is one of the most easily observable manifestations of autonomic activity, and a study of the secretion of this gland should be helpful both in the elaboration of the theory of autonomic activities and perhaps also in practical diagnosis (1, 2, 3). Strongin and Hinsie have shown that whereas normal subjects secrete on the average.07 c.c. per 5-minute period, varying from 02 c.c. to .15 c.c., none of a number of manic-depressive patients studied by them secreted more than .01 c.c. per 5-minute period (4, 5).

Type
Part I.—Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1944 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

(1) Eppinger, H. (1917), Vagotonia: A Clinical Study in Vegetative Neurology. New York: Nerv. and Ment. Dis. Mon., No. 20.Google Scholar
(2) Sachs, W. (1936), The Vegetative Nervous System. London: Cassell.Google Scholar
(3) Guillaume, A. (1928), Vagotonics, Sympathicotonics, Neurotomes. Paris: Masson et Cie.Google Scholar
(4) Strongin, E. I., and Hinsie, L. E. (1938), Am. J. Psychiat., 94, 1459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
(5) Idem (1939), Psychiat. Quart., 13, 697.Google Scholar
(6) Brunacci, B., and de Sanctis, E. (1914), Arch. di Fisiol., 12, 441.Google Scholar
(7) Lashley, K. S. (1914), Psychol. Rev., 23, 446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
(8) Idem (1916), J. Exp. Psychol., 1, 461.Google Scholar
(9) Winsor, A. L. (1931), Am. J. Psychol., 43, 434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
(10) Richter, C. P., and Wada, T. (1924), J. Lab. and Clin. Med., 9, 271.Google Scholar
(11) Wenger, M. A. (1942), Human Biol., 14, 69.Google Scholar
(12) Idem (1941), Psychosom. Med., 3, 427; (1942), 4, 94.Google Scholar
(12a ) Wittkower, E., and Pile, W. (1932), Klin. Wochenschr., 11, 717718.Google Scholar
(13) Mitscherlich, C. G. (1833), Poggendorff's An. d. Phys. Chem., 27, 320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
(14) Zebrowski, E. (1905), Arch. f. d. ges. Physiol., 110, 105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
(15) Brunacci, B. (1910), Arch. di Fisiol., 8, 421.Google Scholar
(16) Krasnogorski, N. I. (1926), Jahrb. d. Kinderheilkunde, 114, 268.Google Scholar
(17) Winsor, A. L. (1928), J. Exp. Psychol., 11, 355; (1930), 13, 423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
(18) Eysenck, , Davies, M.; A Statistical Study of Individual Preferences with Olfactory Stimuli. Thesis, Univ. of London.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.