Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T11:29:21.684Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Behaviour, Subsequent Sleep, and Dreaming

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2018

Ralph J. Berger
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Edinburgh
Ian Oswald
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Edinburgh

Extract

In the course of the past few years, a series of related studies (Aserinsky and Kleitman, 1955; Goodenough et al., 1959; Wolpert and Trosman, 1958) has demonstrated beyond doubt the association of normal dreaming with the appearance of rapid, binocularly synchronous eye-movements. It has also been claimed that the rapid eye-movements (REMs) represent scanning movements made by the dreamer as he “watches” the visual events of the dream (Dement and Kleitman, 1957a; Dement and Wolpert, 1958). The REMs are absent during dreaming among those with life-long blindness, but are retained for some years by those whose blindness arises later than childhood (Berger et al., 1962a). In a study of undisturbed nocturnal sleep by Dement and Kleitman (1957b) periods of eye-movements were observed to occur fairly regularly at about 90-minute intervals throughout the night in association with the lightest phases of cyclic variation in depth of sleep, as indicated by the electroencephalogram (EEG). These REM periods had a mean duration of about 20 minutes, and 4–6 occurred per night.

Type
Psychological
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1962 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aserinsky, E., and Kleitman, N., J. appl. Physiol., 1955, 8, 1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benoit, O., and Bloch, V., J. Physiol. (Paris), 1960, 52, 17.Google Scholar
Berger, R. J., Science, 1961, 134, 840.Google Scholar
Idem , Olley, P. C., and Oswald, I., Quart. J. exp. Psychol., 1962a, 14 (in press).Google Scholar
Idem , Jaramilo, R. A., Keddie, K. M. G., Olley, P. C., Oswald, I., and Plunkett, G. B., 1962b (to be published).Google Scholar
Bliss, E. L., Clark, L. D., and West, C. D., Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., 1959, 81, 348.Google Scholar
Brauchi, J. T., and West, L. J., J. Amer. med. Ass., 1959, 171, 11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dement, W., Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol., 1958, 10, 291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Idem , Science, 1960a, 131, 1705.Google Scholar
Idem , Science, 1960b, 132, 1420.Google Scholar
Idem , and Kleitman, N., J. exp. Psychol., 1957a, 53, 339.Google Scholar
Idem and Kleitman, N., Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol., 1957b, 9, 673.Google Scholar
Idem and Wolpert, E. A., J. exp. Psychol., 1958, 55, 543.Google Scholar
Goodenough, D. R., Shapiro, A., Holden, M., and Steinschriber, L., J. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1959, 59, 295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horovitz, Z. P., and Chow, M. I., Science, 1961, 134, 945.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jouvet, M., Michel, F., and Mounier, D., Rev. neurol., 1960, 103, 189.Google Scholar
Kraepelin, E., Psychol. Arb., 1906, 5, 1.Google Scholar
Loomis, A. L., Harvey, E. N., and Hobart, G. A., J. exp. Psychol., 1937, 21, 127.Google Scholar
Marbach, G., and Schaff, G., C.R. Soc. Biol. (Paris), 1960, 154, 408.Google Scholar
Morris, G. O., Williams, H. L., and Lubin, A., Arch. gen. Psychiat., 1960, 2, 247.Google Scholar
Oswald, I., Sleeping and Waking: Physiology and Psychology, 1962. Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, D. B., Dis. nerv. Syst., 1955, 16, 293.Google Scholar
Ullman, M., Science, 1960, 132, 1418.Google Scholar
Wolpert, E. A., and Trosman, H., Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., 1958, 79, 603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.