Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T09:11:52.053Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Multiple dependencies and the role of the grammar in real-time comprehension1

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 June 2009

MATTHEW W. WAGERS*
Affiliation:
Department of LinguisticsUniversity of California, Santa Cruz
COLIN PHILLIPS*
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics and Neuroscience & Cognitive Science ProgramUniversity of Maryland, College Park
*
Authors' addresses: Department of Linguistics, University of California, Santa Cruz, 1156 High Street, Santa Cruz, CA 95064, USA[email protected]
Department of Linguistics, University of Maryland, 1401 Marie Mount Hall, College Park, MD 20742, USA[email protected]

Abstract

Wh-dependencies are known to be formed rapidly in real-time comprehension. The parser posits the location of gap sites in advance of the bottom-up evidence for missing constituents, and must therefore have a means of deciding when and where to project dependencies. Previous studies have observed that the parser avoids building ungrammatical wh-dependencies, for example, by restricting the search for gap sites from island domains. This paper tests the stronger claim that constraints are not merely respected, but that grammatical knowledge actively prompts the construction of some representations in advance of the input. Three self-paced reading experiments examined patterns of wh-dependency formation in multiple-dependency constructions: obligatory across-the-board (ATB) extraction from coordinated verb phrases, and from optional parasitic gaps in post-verbal adjunct clauses. The key finding is that comprehenders immediately enforce the requirement for extraction from coordinates, and hence actively search for multiple gap sites within a coordinate VP; but they do not search for post-verbal parasitic gaps. This difference cannot be attributed to relative differences in acceptability, as comprehenders rated both of these multiple-gap constructions equally highly, nor can it be explained by general parsing incentives to develop maximal incremental interpretations of partial strings. More plausibly, the difference reflects the deployment of detailed grammatical knowledge in a parser that is motivated to satisfy structural licensing requirements in real time.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[1]

The authors acknowledge the support of grants from the National Science Foundation (BSC-0196004) and the Human Frontiers Science Program (RGY-0134). Thanks to Norbert Hornstein, Amy Weinberg, the audience of the CUNY Sentence Processing Conference (19), and the two anonymous Journal of Linguistics referees for helpful discussion and comment.

References

REFERENCES

Ades, Anthony & Steedman, Mark. 1982. On the order of words. Linguistics and Philosophy 4, 517558.Google Scholar
Altmann, Gerry T. M. & Kamide, Yuki. 1999. Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition 73, 247264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, Stephen R. 1971. On the role of deep structure in semantic interpretation. Foundations of Language 7, 387396.Google Scholar
Aoshima, Sachiko, Phillips, Colin & Weinberg, Amy S.. 2004. Processing filler–gap dependencies in a head-final language. Journal of Memory and Language 51, 2354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boland, Julie E., Tanenhaus, Michael K., Garnsey, Susan M. & Carlson, Greg N.. 1995. Verb argument structure in parsing and interpretation: Evidence from wh-questions. Journal of Memory and Language 34, 774806.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bourdages, Johanne S. 1992. Parsing complex NPs in French. In Goodluck, Helen & Rochemond, Michael S. (eds.), Island constraints: Theory, acquisition and processing, 6187. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crain, Stephen & Fodor, Janet D.. 1985. How can grammars help parsers? In Dowty, David, Karttunen, Lauri & Zwicky, Arnold (eds.), Natural language parsing, 94128. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deane, Paul 1991. Limits to attention: A cognitive theory of island constraints. Cognitive Linguistics 2, 163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engdahl, Elisabet. 1983. Parasitic gaps. Linguistics and Philosophy 6, 534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, Janet D. 1978. Parsing strategies and constraints on transformations. Linguistic Inquiry 9, 427473.Google Scholar
Fraser, Bruce. 1971. A note on the spray-paint cases. Linguistic Inquiry 2, 603607.Google Scholar
Frazier, Lyn. 1987. Syntactic processing: Evidence from Dutch. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 5, 519560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frazier, Lyn & Clifton, Charles. 2001. Parsing coordinates and ellipsis: Copy α. Syntax 4, 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frazier, Lyn & Flores D'Arcais, Giovanni B.. 1989. Filler-driven parsing: A study of gap filling in Dutch. Journal of Memory and Language 28, 331–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frazier, Lyn, Munn, Alan & Clifton, Charles. 2000. Processing coordinate structures. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 29, 343370.Google Scholar
Freedman, Sandra E. & Forster, Kenneth I.. 1985. The psychological status of overgenerated sentences. Cognition 19, 101131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garnsey, Susan M., Tanenhaus, Michael K. & Chapman, Robert M.. 1989. Evoked potentials and the study of sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 18, 5160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gazdar, Gerald, Klein, Ewan, Pullum, Geoffrey K. & Sag, Ivan A.. 1985. Generalized Phrase Structure Grammar. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Gibson, Edward. 1998. Syntactic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition 68, 176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, Edward & Hickok, Gregory. 1993. Sentence processing with empty categories. Language and Cognitive Processes 8, 147161.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, John. 1985. A principled exception to the coordinate structure constraint. Twenty-First Annual Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society (CLS 21), 133143. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Gordon, Peter C., Hendrick, Randall & Johnson, Marcus. 2001. Memory interference during language processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 27, 14111423.Google ScholarPubMed
Gorrell, Paul. 1993. Evaluating the direct association hypothesis: A reply to Pickering and Barry 1991. Language and Cognitive Processes 8, 129146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaan, Edith. 1997. Processing subject–object ambiguities in Dutch. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Groningen.Google Scholar
Kaan, Edith, Harris, Anthony, Gibson, Edward & Holcomb, Phillip. 2000. The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty. Language and Cognitive Processes 15, 159201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kayne, Richard S. 1983. Connectedness. Linguistic Inquiry 14, 223249.Google Scholar
Kluender, Robert. 2004. Are subject islands subject to a processing account? In Chand, Vineeta, Kelleher, Ann, Rodriguez, Angelo J. & Schmeiser, Benjamin (eds.), 23rd West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (WCCFL 23), 514527. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Kluender, Robert & Kutas, Marta. 1993. Subjacency as a processing phenomenon. Language and Cognitive Processes 8, 573633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurtzman, Howard S. & Crawford, Loren E.. 1991. Processing parasitic gaps. In Sherer, Tim (ed.), 21st Annual Meeting of the North Eastern Linguistics Society (NELS 21), 217231. Amherst, MA: LSA Publications.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George. 1986. Frame semantic control of the coordinate structure constraint. Twenty-Second Annual Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society (CLS 22), 152167. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Lau, Ellen F., Yeung, Henny H., Hashimoto, Ryu, Braun, Allen & Phillips, Colin. 2006. Time-course and localization of syntactic and semantic anomaly responses in sentence processing: A within-subjects fMRI/MEG design. Presented at the Society for Neuroscience Annual Meeting, October.Google Scholar
Lee, Ming-Wei. 2004. Another look at the role of empty categories in sentence processing (and grammar). Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 33, 5173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levin, Beth. 1993. English verb classes and alternations: A preliminary investigation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, Richard L. & Vasishth, Shravan. 2005. An activation-based model of sentence processing as skilled memory retrieval. Cognitive Science 29, 375419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McElree, Brian & Bever, Thomas. 1989. The psychological reality of linguistically defined gaps. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 18, 2135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McElree, Brian, Foraker, Stephanie & Dyer, Lisbeth. 2003. Memory structures that subserve sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language 48, 6791.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKinnon, Richard & Osterhout, Lee. 1996. Constraints on movement phenomena in sentence processing: Evidence from event-related potentials. Language and Cognition 11, 495523.Google Scholar
Meng, Michael & Bader, Markus. 2000. Ungrammaticality detection and garden path strength: Evidence for serial parsing. Language and Cognitive Processes 16, 615666.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neville, Helen J., Nicol, Janet L., Barss, Andrew, Forster, Kenneth & Garrett, Merrill. 1991. Syntactically-based sentence processing classes: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 3, 151165.Google Scholar
Nicol, Janet L., Fodor, Janet D. & Swinney, David. 1994. Using cross-modal lexical decision tasks to investigate sentence processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition 20, 12291238.Google ScholarPubMed
Nicol, Janet L. & Swinney, David. 1989. The role of structure in coreference assignment during sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 18, 5–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearlmutter, Neal J., Garnsey, Susan M. & Bock, Kathyrn. 1999. Agreement processes in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language 41, 427456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, Colin. 2006. The real-time status of island phenomena. Language 82, 795823.Google Scholar
Phillips, Colin, Kazanina, Nina & Abada, Shani H.. 2005. ERP effects of the processing of syntactic long-distance dependencies. Cognitive Brain Research 22, 407428.Google Scholar
Phillips, Colin & Wagers, Matthew W.. 2007. Relating structure and time in linguistics and psycholinguistics. In Gaskell, Gareth (ed.), Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics, 739756. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pickering, Martin J. & Barry, Guy D.. 1991. Sentence processing without empty categories. Language and Cognitive Processes 6, 229259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, Martin J., Barton, Stephen & Shillcock, Richard. 1994. Unbounded dependencies, island constraints and processing complexity. In Clifton, Charles, Frazier, Lyn & Rayner, Keith (eds.), Perspectives on sentence processing, 199224. London: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pickering, Martin J. & Traxler, Matthew J.. 1998. Plausibility and recovery from garden paths: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 24, 940961.Google Scholar
Pickering, Martin J. & Traxler, Matthew J.. 2003. Evidence against the use of subcategorisation frequency in the processing of unbounded dependencies. Language and Cognitive Processes 18, 469503.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven. 1989. Learnability and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Postal, Paul M. 1993. Parasitic gaps and the across-the-board phenomenon. Linguistic Inquiry 24, 735754.Google Scholar
Postal, Paul. 1998. Three investigations of extraction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pritchett, Bradley L. 1992. Grammatical competence and parsing performance. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Radó, Janina. 1999. Discourse effects in gap-filling. Presented at AMLaP-99, University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Raaijmakers, Jeroen G. W., Schrijnemakers, Joseph M. C. & Gremmen, Frans. 1999. How to deal with ‘The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy’: Common misconceptions and alternative solutions. Journal of Memory and Language 41, 416426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rappaport, Malka & Levin, Beth. 1986. What to do with theta roles. Lexicon Project Working Papers 11. Cambridge, MA: Center for Cognitive Science, MIT.Google Scholar
Rohde, Douglas. 2003. Linger, version 2.94. http://tedlab.mit.edu/~dr/linger/.Google Scholar
Ross, John R. 1967. Constraints on variables in syntax. Ph.D. dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
Schlesewsky, Matthias, Fanselow, Gisbert, Kliegl, Reinhold & Krems, Josef. 2000. The subject preference in the processing of locally ambiguous wh-questions in German. In Hemforth, Barbara & Konieczny, Lars (eds.), German sentence processing, 6593. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Sedivy, Julie C., Tanenhaus, Michael K., Chambers, Craig G. & Carlson, Greg N.. 1999. Achieving incremental semantic interpretation through contextual representation. Cognition 71, 109147.Google Scholar
Sekerina, Irina A. 2003. Scrambling and processing: Dependencies, complexity, and constraints. In Karimi, Simin (ed.), Word order and scrambling, 301324. Malden, MA: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steiner, Ilona. 2003. Parsing syntactic redundancies in coordinate structures. Presented at the European Cognitive Science Conference (EuroCogSci03), Osnabrück, 10–13 September.Google Scholar
Stowe, Laurie. 1986. Parsing wh-constructions: Evidence for on-line gap location. Language and Cognitive Processes 1, 227246.Google Scholar
Sturt, Patrick. 2003. The time-course of the application of binding constraints in reference resolution. Journal of Memory and Language 48, 542562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sussman, Rachel & Sedivy, Julie C.. 2003. The time course of processing syntactic dependencies. Language and Cognitive Processes 18, 143163.Google Scholar
Szabolcsi, Anna & den Dikken, Marcel. (2002). Islands. In Cheng, Lisa & Sybesma, Rint (eds.), The second state-of-the-article book. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, Michael K., Stowe, Laurie A. & Carlson, Greg N.. 1985. The interaction of lexical expectation and pragmatics in parsing filler–gap constructions. Seventh Annual Cognitive Science Society Conference, 361365. Irvine, CA.Google Scholar
Traxler, Matthew J. & Pickering, Martin J.. 1996. Plausibility and the processing of unbounded dependencies. Journal of Memory and Language 35, 454475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vasishth, Shravan, Brüssow, Sven, Lewis, Richard L. & Drenhaus, Heiner. 2008. Processing polarity: How the ungrammatical intrudes on the grammatical. Cognitive Science 32, 685712.Google Scholar
de Vincenzi, Marica. 1991. Syntactic parsing strategies in Italian. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagers, Matthew W. 2008. The structure for memory meets memory for structure in linguistic cognition. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Maryland, College Park.Google Scholar
Wanner, Eric & Maratsos, Michael. 1978. An ATN approach to comprehension. In Halle, Morris, Bresnan, Joan & Miller, George A. (eds.), Linguistic theory and psychological reality, 119161. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Weinberg, Amy S. 1992. Parameters in the theory of sentence processing: Minimal commitment theory goes east. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 3, 339364.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: PDF

Wagers Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material.pdf

Download Wagers Supplementary Material(PDF)
PDF 46.4 KB