Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T02:14:45.321Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Coding causal–noncausal verb alternations: A form–frequency correspondence explanation1

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 August 2014

MARTIN HASPELMATH*
Affiliation:
Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology
ANDREEA CALUDE
Affiliation:
University of Waikato
MICHAEL SPAGNOL
Affiliation:
University of Malta
HEIKO NARROG
Affiliation:
Tohoku University
ELİF BAMYACI
Affiliation:
University of Würzburg
*
Authors’ address: (Haspelmath)Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig, Germany[email protected]

Abstract

We propose, and provide corpus-based support for, a usage-based explanation for cross-linguistic trends in the coding of causal–noncausal verb pairs, such as raise/rise, break (tr.)/break (intr.). While English mostly uses the same verb form both for the causal and the noncausal sense (labile coding), most languages have extra coding for the causal verb (causative coding) and/or for the noncausal verb (anticausative coding). Causative and anticausative coding is not randomly distributed (Haspelmath 1993): Some verb meanings, such as ‘freeze’, ‘dry’ and ‘melt’, tend to be coded as causatives, while others, such as ‘break’, ‘open’ and ‘split’, tend to be coded as anticausatives. We propose an explanation of these coding tendencies on the basis of the form–frequency correspondence principle, which is a general efficiency principle that is responsible for many grammatical asymmetries, ultimately grounded in predictability of frequently expressed meanings. In corpus data from seven languages, we find that verb pairs for which the noncausal member is more frequent tend to be coded as anticausatives, while verb pairs for which the causal member is more frequent tend to be coded as causatives. Our approach implies that linguists should not rely on form–meaning parallelism when trying to explain cross-linguistic or language-particular patterns in this domain.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[1]

We are grateful to three anonymous referees for the Journal of Linguistics as well as to Bernard Comrie for very useful comments on this paper. In addition, we are grateful to the audiences in several places where we presented this work: at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (which also deserves thanks for bringing several of the authors together), at the 4th UK Cognitive Linguistics Conference (London), at the Societas Linguistica Europaea 2012 in Stockholm, and at the Syntax of the World's Languages 5 in Dubrovnik.

References

Alexiadou, Artemis. 2010. On the morpho-syntax of (anti-)causative verbs. In Hovav, Malka Rappaport, Doron, Edit & Sichel, Ivy (eds.), Syntax, lexical semantics and event structure, 177203. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Alexiadou, Artemis, Anagnostopoulou, Elena & Schäfer, Florian. 2006. The properties of anti-causatives crosslinguistically. In Frascarelli, Mara (ed.), Phases of interpretation, 187211. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borer, Hagit. 1991. The causative–inchoative alternation: A case study in parallel morphology. The Linguistic Review 8, 119158.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 2006. From usage to grammar: The mind's response to repetition. Language 82.4, 711733.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. & Clark, Herbert H.. 1978. Universals, relativity, and language processing. In Greenberg, Joseph H. (ed.), Universals of human language, vol. 1: Method and theory, 225277. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Comrie, Bernard. 1985. Causative verb formation and other verb-derivational morphology. In Shopen, Timothy (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description, vol. 3: Grammatical categories and lexicon, 309348. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Croft, William. 1990. Possible verbs and the structure of events. In Tsohatzidis, Savas L. (ed.), Meaning and prototypes: Studies in linguistic categorization, 4873. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Croft, William. 2003. Typology and universals, 2nd edn. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cysouw, Michael. 2008. Generalizing scales. In Richards, Marc & Malchukov, Andrej L. (eds.), Scales (Linguistische Arbeitsberichte 86), 379396. Leipzig: Institut für Linguistik, University of Leipzig.Google Scholar
Dixon, R. M. W. & Aikhenvald, Alexandra Y.. 2000. Introduction. In Dixon, R. M. W. & Aikhenvald, Alexandra Y. (eds.), Changing valency: Case studies in transitivity, 129. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Doron, Edit. 2003. Agency and voice: The semantics of Semitic templates. Natural Language Semantics 11, 167.Google Scholar
Givón, Talmy. 1991. Markedness in grammar: Distributional, communicative and cognitive correlates of syntactic structure. Studies in Language 15.2, 335370.Google Scholar
Greenberg, Joseph H. 1966. Language universals, with special reference to feature hierarchies. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Guerssel, Mohamed, Hale, Kenneth, Laughren, Mary, Levin, Beth & Eagle, Josie White. 1985. A cross-linguistic study of transitivity alternations. Chicago Linguistic Society 21, part 2, 4862.Google Scholar
Haiman, John. 1983. Iconic and economic motivation. Language 59.4, 781819.Google Scholar
Hakkani-Tür, Dilek Zeynep. 2000. Statistical language modeling for agglutinative languages. Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Computer Engineering, Bilkent University, Ankara.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin. 1987. Transitivity alternations of the anticausative type (Arbeitspapiere Des Instituts Für Sprachwissenschaft N.F. Nr. 4). Cologne: Universität zu Köln.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin. 1993. More on the typology of inchoative/causative verb alternations. In Comrie, Bernard & Polinsky, Maria (eds.), Causatives and transitivity, 87120. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin. 2004. Does linguistic explanation presuppose linguistic description? Studies in Language 28, 554579.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin. 2006. Against markedness (and what to replace it with). Journal of Linguistics 42.1, 2570.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin. 2008a. Frequency vs. iconicity in explaining grammatical asymmetries. Cognitive Linguistics 19.1, 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin. 2008b. Creating economical morphosyntactic patterns in language change. In Good, Jeff (ed.), Language universals and language change, 185214. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin. 2010. Comparative concepts and descriptive categories in cross-linguistic studies. Language 86.3, 663687.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin. 2011. The indeterminacy of word segmentation and the nature of morphology and syntax. Folia Linguistica 45.2, 3180.Google Scholar
Hawkins, John A. 2004. Efficiency and complexity in grammars. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Heidinger, Steffen. 2010. French anticausatives: A diachronic perspective. Berlin: De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heidinger, Steffen. 2012. Frequenz und die Kodierung der Kausativ–Antikausativ-Alternation im Französischen. Romanistisches Jahrbuch 62, 3158.Google Scholar
Heidinger, Steffen. To appear. Causalness and the encoding of the causative–anticausative alternation in French and Spanish. Journal of Linguistics.Google Scholar
Koontz-Garboden, Andrew. 2009. Anticausativization. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 27, 77138.Google Scholar
Lehmann, Christian. 2007. On the upgrading of grammatical concepts. In Moerdijk, Fons, van Santen, Ariane & Tempelaars, Rob (eds.), Leven met woorden: Opstellen aangeboden aan Piet van Sterkenburg, 409422. Leiden: Brill.Google Scholar
Letuchiy, Alexander. 2010. Lability and spontaneity. In Brandt, Patrick & García, Marco García (eds.), Transitivity: Form, meaning, acquisition, and processing, 237256. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Levin, Beth & Hovav, Malka Rappaport. 1995. Unaccusativity: At the syntax–lexical semantics interface. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Marantz, Alec. 1984. On the nature of grammatical relations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mel’čuk, Igor’ A. 1967. K ponjatiju slovoobrazovanija. Izvestija Akademii Nauk SSSR, Serija literatury i jazyka 26(4), 352362.Google Scholar
Mihalcea, Rada F. & Năstase, Vivi A.. 2002. Letter level learning for language independent diacritics restoration. In Roth, Dan & van den Bosch, Antal (eds.), 6th Conference on Computational Natural Language Learning (CoNLL 2002), 105111. Taipei. [http://www.clips.uantwerpen.be/conll2002/proceedings.html]Google Scholar
Narrog, Heiko. 2007. Nihongo zita dōshi ni okeru yūhyōsei-sa no dōki-duke [On the motivation for markedness differences in Japanese transitivity verb pairs]. In Tsunoda, Mie, Sasaki, Kan & Shioya, Tōru (eds.), Tadōsei no tsūgengoteki kenkyū – Tsunoda Tasaku Hakase kanreki kinen ronbunshū [Cross-linguistic studies of transitivity – a Festschrift for the 60th birthday of Doctor Tsunoda Tasaku], 295306. Tokyo: Kuroshio Shuppan.Google Scholar
Narrog, Heiko, Pardeshi, Prashant & Sanada, Haruko. 2014. Motivating coding differences in transitivity pairs with frequency: A case study from Japanese. Ms., Tohoku University & NINJAL Tokyo.Google Scholar
Nedjalkov, Vladimir P. & Sil'nickij, Georgij G.. 1969. Tipologija morfologičeskogo i leksičeskogo kauzativov [Typology of morphological and lexical causatives]. In Xolodovič, Alexander A. (ed.), Tipologija kauzativnyx konstrukcij [Typology of causative constructions], 2060. Leningrad: Nauka.Google Scholar
Nedyalkov [Nedjalkov], Vladimir P. & Silnitsky, Georgy G.. 1973. The typology of morphological and lexical causatives. In Kiefer, Ferenc (ed.), Trends in Soviet theoretical linguistics, 132. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Nichols, Johanna, Peterson, David A. & Barnes, Jonathan. 2004. Transitivizing and detransitivizing languages. Linguistic Typology 8.2, 149211.Google Scholar
Payne, Thomas. 1997. Describing morphosyntax: A guide for field linguists. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Piñón, Christopher. 2001. A finer look at the causative–inchoative alternation. In Hastings, Rachel, Jackson, Brendan & Zvolenszky, Zsofia (eds.), Semantics and linguistic theory, vol. 11, 346364. Ithaca, NY: CLC Publications.Google Scholar
Pylkkänen, Liina. 2008. Introducing arguments. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
R Development Core Team. 2004. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. http://www.R-project.org.Google Scholar
Rosch, Eleanor. 1978. Principles of categorization. In Rosch, Eleanor & Lloyd, Barbara (eds.), Cognition and categorization, 189206. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Samardžić, Tanja & Merlo, Paola. 2012. The meaning of lexical causatives in cross-linguistic variation. Linguistic Issues in Language Technology 7.12, 114.Google Scholar
Schäfer, Florian. 2008. The syntax of (anti-)causatives: External arguments in change-of-state contexts. Amsterdam & Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Schäfer, Florian. 2009. The causative alternation. Language and Linguistics Compass 3.2, 641681.Google Scholar
Siewierska, Anna. 1984. The passive: A comparative linguistic analysis. London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Spagnol, Michael. 2011. A tale of two morphologies: Verb structure and argument alternations in Maltese. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Konstanz.Google Scholar
Wright, Saundra K. 2001. Internally caused and externally caused change of state verbs. Ph.D. dissertation, Northwestern University.Google Scholar
Zipf, George K. 1935. The psycho-biology of language. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar