Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T14:11:12.609Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sociophonetics, semantics, and intention

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 January 2022

ERIC K. ACTON*
Affiliation:
Department of English Language & Literature Eastern Michigan University 612 Pray-Harrold, Ypsilanti, MI48197, [email protected]

Abstract

Kathryn Campbell-Kibler observes that the role of speaker intention seems to differ in the meanings of primary interest in variationist sociolinguistics on one hand and semantics and pragmatics on the other. Taking this observation as its point of departure, the central goal of the present work is to clarify the nature of intention-attribution in general and, at the same time, the nature of these two types of meaning. I submit general principles by which observers determine whether to attribute a particular intention to an agent – principles grounded in observers’ estimation of the agent’s beliefs, preferences, and assessment of alternative actions. These principles and the attendant discussion clarify the role of alternatives, common ground, and perceptions of naturalness in intention-attribution, illuminate public discourses about agents’ intentions, point to challenges for game-theoretic models of interpretation that assume cooperativity, and elucidate the nature of the types of meaning of interest. Examining the role of intention vis-à-vis findings and insights from variationist research and the formally explicit game-theoretic models just mentioned foregrounds important differences and similarities between the two types of meaning of interest and lays bare the contingent nature of all meaning in practice.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

My thanks to Sara Acton, John Acton, John Cadell, Penny Eckert, Phil Huyck, Chris Potts and audiences at Stanford’s SemFest 20, Meaning and Indexicality Across Subfields and Theories, and Agency and Intentions in Language 1. I am also very grateful to the article’s three anonymous referees and the editors of the journal, whose feedback greatly improved this work.

References

Acton, Eric K. 2021. Pragmatics and the third wave: The social meaning of definites. In Hall-Lew, Lauren, Moore, Emma & Podesva, Robert J. (eds.), Social meaning and linguistic variation: Theorizing the third wave, 105126. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ariel, Mira. 2004. Most. Language 80.4, 658706.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Austin, J. L. 1962. How to do things with words. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bach, Kent. 2006. Review of Christopher Potts, The logic of conventional implicatures, 2005. Journal of Linguistics 42.2, 490495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bach, Kent. 2012. Saying, meaning, and implicating. In Allan, Keith & Jaszczolt, Kasia M. (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of pragmatics, 4768. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, John A., Green, Michelle & Fitzsimons, Gráinne. 2008. The selfish goal: Unintended consequences of intended goal pursuits. Social Cognition 26.5, 534554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauchamp, Zack. 2019. The Ilhan Omar anti-Semitism controversy, explained. Vox, March 6. https://www.vox.com/policy-and-politics/2019/3/6/18251639/ilhan-omar-israel-anti-semitism-jews (accessed July 30, 2019).Google Scholar
Benor, Sarah Bunin. 2001. The learnèd /t/: Phonological variation in Orthodox Jewish English. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 7.3, 116.Google Scholar
Blakemore, Diane. 2015. Slurs and expletives: A case against a general account of expressive meaning. Language Sciences 52, 2235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bucholtz, Mary. 2001. The whiteness of nerds: Superstandard English and racial markedness. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 11.1, 84100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buettner, Russ & Craig, Susanne. 2019. Decade in the red: Trump tax figures show over $1 billion in business losses. New York Times, May 7. https://www.nytimes.com/interactive/2019/05/07/us/politics/donald-trump-taxes.html (accessed September 21, 2019).Google Scholar
Burnett, Heather. 2017. Sociolinguistic interaction and identity construction: The view from game-theoretic pragmatics. Journal of Sociolinguistics 21.2, 238271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burnett, Heather. 2019. Signalling games, sociolinguistic variation and the construction of style. Linguistics and Philosophy 42, 419450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, Judith. 1993. Bodies that matter: On the discursive limits of ‘sex’. Abingdon: Routledge.Google Scholar
Campbell-Kibler, Kathryn. 2008. I’ll be the judge of that: Diversity in social perceptions of (ING). Language in Society 37.5, 637659.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell-Kibler, Kathryn. 2009. The nature of sociolinguistic perception. Language Variation and Change 21.1, 135156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Herbert H. & Fox Tree, Jean E.. 2002. Using uh and um in spontaneous speaking. Cognition 84.1, 73111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Condoravdi, Cleo & Lauer, Sven. 2012. Imperatives: Meaning and illocutionary force. Empirical Issues in Syntax and Semantics 9, 3758.Google Scholar
Du Bois, John W. 1993. Meaning without intention: Lessons from divination. In Hill, Jane H. & Irvine, Judith T. (eds.), Responsibility and evidence in oral discourse, 4871. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Eckert, Penelope. 2008. Variation and the indexical field. Journal of Sociolinguistics 12.4, 453476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckert, Penelope. 2012. Three waves of variation study: The emergence of meaning in the study of variation. Annual Review of Anthropology 41, 87100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckert, Penelope. 2019. The limits of meaning: Social indexicality, variation, and the cline of interiority. Language 95.4, 751776.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckert, Penelope & McConnell-Ginet, Sally. 2013. Language and Gender, 2nd edn. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frank, Michael C. & Goodman, Noah D.. 2012. Predicting pragmatic reasoning in language games. Science 336.6084, 998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franke, Michael. 2009. Signal to act: Game theory in pragmatics. Ph.D. dissertation, Universiteit van Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Franke, Michael. 2013. Game theoretic pragmatics. Philosophy Compass 8.3, 269284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franke, Michael, de Jager, Tikitu & van Rooij, Robert. 2012. Relevance in cooperation and conflict. Journal of Logic and Computation 22.1, 2354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaenszle, Martin. 2016. Meaning, intention, and responsibility in Rai divinatory discourse. Oral Tradition 30(2), 263280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, Noah D. & Frank, Michael C.. 2016. Pragmatic language interpretation as probabilistic inference. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20.11, 818829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grice, H. Paul. 1957. Meaning. The Philosophical Review 66.3, 377388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grice, H. Paul. 1975. Logic and conversation. In Cole, Peter & Morgan, Jerry L. (eds.), Syntax and semantics. Vol. 3: Speech acts, 4158. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Horn, Laurence R. 1984. Toward a new taxonomy for pragmatic inference: Q-based and R-based implicature. In Schiffrin, Deborah (ed.), Meaning, form, and use in context: Linguistic applications, 1142. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Horn, Laurence R. 2004. Implicature. In Horn & Ward (eds.), 328.Google Scholar
Horn, Laurence R. & Ward, Gregory (eds.). 2004. Handbook of pragmatics. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kao, Justine T. & Goodman, Noah D.. 2015. Let’s talk (ironically) about the weather: Modeling verbal irony. Proceedings of the 37th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 10511056.Google Scholar
Kao, Justine T., Wu, Jean Y., Bergen, Leon & Goodman, Noah D.. 2014. Nonliteral understanding of number words. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 111.33, 1200212007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, Brian W. 2018. Hip Hop headz in sex ed: Gender, agency, and styling in New Zealand. Language in Society 47.4, 487512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William. [1966] 2006. The social stratification of English in New York City, 2nd edn. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William. 2012. Dialect diversity in America: The politics of language change. Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia Press.Google Scholar
Lavandera, Beatriz R. 1978. Where does the sociolinguistic variable stop? Language in Society 7.2, 171182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, Stephen C. 1988. Putting linguistics on a proper footing: Explorations in Goffman’s concepts of participation. In Drew, Paul & Wootton, Anthony (eds.), Erving Goffman: Exploring the interaction order, 161227. Oxford: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Levon, Erez. 2014. Categories, stereotypes, and the linguistic perception of sexuality. Language in Society 43.5, 539566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCready, E. 2015. Reliability in pragmatics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Milne, Alan A. [1926] 2009. Winnie-the-Pooh. New York: Dutton Children’s Books.Google Scholar
Montague, Richard. 1970. English as a formal language. In Visentini, Bruno (ed.), Linguaggi nella società e nella tecnica, 189224. Milan: Edizioni di Communità. Reprinted in 1974 in Formal philosophy: Selected papers of Richard Montague, 188–221.Google Scholar
Podesva, Robert J. 2011. Salience and the social meaning of declarative contours: Three case studies of gay professionals. Journal of English Linguistics 39.3, 233264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Podesva, Robert J., Reynolds, Jermay, Callier, Patrick & Baptiste, Jessica. 2015. Constraints on the social meaning of released /t/: A production and perception study of US politicians. Language Variation and Change 27.1, 5987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Potts, Christopher. 2007. The expressive dimension. Theoretical Linguistics 33.2, 165198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Potts, Christopher. 2015. Presupposition and implicature. In Lappin, Shalom & Fox, Chris (eds.), The handbook of contemporary semantic theory, 2nd edn, 168202. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, Craige. 1996. Information structure: Towards an integrated formal theory of pragmatics. OSU Working Papers in Linguistics 49, 91136.Google Scholar
Romaine, Suzanne. 1984. On the problem of syntactic variation and pragmatic meaning in sociolinguistic theory. Folia Linguistica 18.3–4, 409438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shakespeare, William. 1998. The tragedy of Hamlet, Prince of Denmark. New York: Signet Classics.Google Scholar
Smith, E. Allyn, Hall, Kathleen Currie & Munson, Benjamin. 2010. Bringing semantics to sociophonetics: Social variables and secondary entailments. Laboratory Phonology 1.1, 121155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stalnaker, Robert C. 2002. Common ground. Linguistics and Philosophy 25.5–6, 701721.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephens, Bret. 2019. Ilhan Omar knows exactly what she is doing. New York Times, March 7. https://www.nytimes.com/2019/03/07/opinion/ilhan-omar-anti-semitism.html (accessed March 14, 2019).Google Scholar
Tamminga, Meredith. 2017. Matched guise effects can be robust to speech style. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 142, 1823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiss, Bari. 2019. Ilhan Omar and the myth of Jewish hypnosis. New York Times, January 21. https://www.nytimes.com/2019/01/21/opinion/ilhan-omar-israel-jews.html (accessed July 30, 2019).Google Scholar
Wharton, Tim. 2016. That bloody so-and-so has retired: Expressives revisited. Lingua 175–176, 2035.Google Scholar
Wilson, Deirdre & Sperber, Dan. 2004. Relevance theory. In Horn & Ward (eds.), 607632.Google Scholar