We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
References
REFERENCES
Borer, H. & Wexler, K. (1987). The maturation of syntax. In Roeper, T. & Williams, E. (eds.) Parameter setting. Dordrecht: Reidel. 123–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Budwig, N. (1989). The linguistic marking of agentivity and control in early child language. Journal of Child Language16. 263–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clancy, P. (1985). The acquisition of Japanese. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.) The crosslinguislic study of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 373–524.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. & Carpenter, K. L. (1989). The notion of source in language acquisition. Lg65. 1–30.Google Scholar
Grimshaw, J. & Thomas Rosen, S. (1990). Obeying the binding theory. In Frazier, L. & De Villiers, J. (eds.) Language processing and language acquisition. Dordrecht: Kluwer. 357–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyams, N. (1986). Language acquisition and the theory of parameters. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Radford, A. (1988). Transformational grammar: a first course. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1985). The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, vols, 1 & 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Valian, V. (1990). Null subjects: a problem for parameter-setting models of language acquisition. Cognition35. 105–122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed