Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T16:26:37.718Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Diagnosing Consciousness: Neuroimaging, Law, and the Vegetative State

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2021

Extract

Recent studies indicate that patients who are diagnosed with vegetative states may retain more awareness than their clinical assessments suggest. Disorders of consciousness traditionally have been diagnosed on the basis of outwardly observable behaviors alone, but new functional imaging studies have shown surprising levels of brain activity in some patients, indicating that even higher-level cognitive functions like language processing and visual imagery may be preserved. For example, one recently developed method purports to detect voluntary mental imagery solely on the basis of neural response patterns observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). This line of research has already led to some widely reported cases of patients who were discovered to have more brain activity than expected. In November 2009, Rom Houben, a Belgian man who was thought to be in a coma for 23 years, was diagnosed with locked-in syndrome through the use of neuroimaging; however, that assessment was later overturned by further testing.

Type
Independent
Copyright
Copyright © American Society of Law, Medicine and Ethics 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

“Book-Writing Man in Coma Fails Communication Test,” National Public Radio, February 17, 2010, available at <http://www.npr.org/blogs/health/2010/02/bookwriting_man_in_coma_funks.html> (last visited May 11, 2010).+(last+visited+May+11,+2010).>Google Scholar
Monti, M. M., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Coleman, M. R., Boly, M., Pickard, J. D., Tshibanda, L., Owen, A. M., and Laureys, S., “Willful Modulation of Brain Activity in Disorders of Consciousness,” New England Journal of Medicine 362, no. 7 (2010): 579589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skene, L., Wilkinson, D., Kahane, G., and Savulescu, J., “Neuroimaging and the Withdrawal of Life-Sustaining Treatment from Patients in Vegetative State,” Medical Law Review 17, no. 2 (2009): 245261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Though for discussion of withdrawal of life saving therapy in light of these developments, see Wilkinson, D. J., Kahane, G., Horne, M., and Savulescu, J., “Functional Neuroimaging and Withdrawal of Life-Sustaining Treatment from Vegetative Patients,” Journal of Medical Ethics 35, no. 8 (2009): 508511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meisel, A. and Cerminara, K. L., The Right to Die: The Law of End-of-Life Decisionmaking (New York: Aspen Publishers, 2004): at 6–128.1.Google Scholar
Plum, F. and Posner, J. B., The Diagnosis of Stupor and Coma (Philadelphia: F. A. Davis Co., 1980).Google Scholar
Laureys, S., Owen, A. M., and Schiff, N. D., “Brain Function in Coma, Vegetative State, and Related Disorders,” Lancet Neurology 3, no. 9 (2004): 537546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
The Multi-Society Task Force on PVS, “Medical Aspects of the Persistent Vegetative State (1),” New England Journal of Medicine 330, no. 21 (1994): 14991508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Id., at 6.Google Scholar
Giacino, J. T., Ashwal, S., Childs, N., Cranford, R., Jennett, B., Katz, D. I., Kelly, J. P., Rosenberg, J. H., Whyte, J., Zafonte, R. D., and Zasler, N. D., “The Minimally Conscious State: Definition and Diagnostic Criteria,” Neurology 58, no. 3 (2002): 349–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fins, J. J., Schiff, N. D., and Foley, K. M., “Late Recovery from the Minimally Conscious State: Ethical and Policy Implications,” Neurology 68, no. 4 (2007): 304307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnakers, C., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Giacino, J., Ventura, M., Boly, M., Majerus, S., Moonen, G., and Laureys, S., “Diagnostic Accuracy of the Vegetative and Minimally Conscious State: Clinical Consensus Versus Standardized Neurobehavioral Assessment,” BMC Neurology 9 (2009): 35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laureys, S., Perrin, F., and Bredart, S., “Self-Consciousness in Non-Communicative Patients,” Consciousness and Cognition 16, no. 3 (2007): 722741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owen, A. M. and Coleman, M. R., “Functional Neuroimaging of the Vegetative State,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9, no. 3 (2008): 235243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koch, C., The Quest for Consciousness: A Neurobiological Approach (Denver: Roberts and Co., 2004)Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Changeux, J. P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., and Sergent, C., “Conscious, Preconscious, and Subliminal Processing: A Testable Taxonomy,” Trends in Cognitive Science 10, no. 5 (2006): 204211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Block, N. J., Flanagan, O. J., and Güzeldere, G., The Nature of Consciousness: Philosophical Debates (Cambridge: MIT Press, 1997).Google Scholar
Block, N. J., “Concepts of Consciousness,” in Chalmers, D. J., ed., Philosophy of Mind: Classical and Contemporary Readings (New York: Oxford University Press, 2002): at 206.Google Scholar
Güzeldere, G., “The Many Faces of Consciousness: A Field Guide,” in Block, N. J., Flanagan, O. J., and Güzeldere, G., eds., The Nature of Consciousness: Philosophical Debates (Cambridge: MIT Press, 1997): 168.Google Scholar
See Block, , supra note 18, at 206.Google Scholar
Zola, E., Thérèse Raquin (Paris: E. Fasquelle, 1867).Google Scholar
Laureys, S., Lemaire, C., Maquet, P., Phillips, C., and Franck, G., “Cerebral Metabolism During Vegetative State and After Recovery to Consciousness,” Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 67, no. 1 (1999): 121122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Jong, B. M., Willemsen, A. T., and Paans, A. M., “Regional Cerebral Blood Flow Changes Related to Affective Speech Presentation in Persistent Vegetative State,” Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery 99, no. 3 (1997): 213216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menon, D. K., Owen, A. M., Williams, E. J., Minhas, P. S., Allen, C. M., Boniface, S. J., and Pickard, J. D., “Cortical Processing in Persistent Vegetative State,” The Lancet 352, no. 9123 (1998): 200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiff, N. D., Ribary, U., Moreno, D. R., Beattie, B., Kronberg, E., Blasberg, R., Giacino, J., McCagg, C., Fins, J. J., Llinas, R., and Plum, F., “Residual Cerebral Activity and Behavioural Fragments Can Remain in the Persistently Vegetative Brain,” Brain 125, no. 6 (2002): 12101234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiff, N. D., Rodriguez-Moreno, D., Kamal, A., Kim, K. H., Giacino, J. T., Plum, F., and Hirsch, J., “fMRI reveals Large-Scale Network Activation in Minimally Conscious Patients,” Neurology 64, no. 3 (2005): 514523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hildebrandt, H., Happe, S., Deutschmann, A., Basar-Eroglu, C., Eling, P., and Brunhober, J., “Brain Perfusion and VEP Reactivity in Occipital and Parietal Areas are Associated to Recovery from Hypoxic Vegetative State,” Journal of the Neurological Sciences 260, no. 1–2 (2007): 150158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kassubek, J., Juengling, F. D., Els, T., Spreer, J., Herpers, M., Krause, T., Moser, E., Lucking, C. H., “Activation of a Residual Cortical Network During Painful Stimulation in Long-Term Postanoxic Vegetative State: A 15O-H2O PET Study,” Journal of the Neurological Sciences 212, no. 1–2 (2003): 8591.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laureys, S., Faymonville, M. E., Peigneux, P., Damas, P., Lambermont, B., Del Fiore, G., Degueldre, C., Aerts, J., Luxen, A., Franck, G., Lamy, M., Moonen, G., and Maquet, P., “Cortical Processing of Noxious Somatosensory Stimuli in the Persistent Vegetative State,” Neuroimage 17, no. 2 (2002): 732741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Di, H. B., Yu, S. M., Weng, X. C., Laureys, S., Yu, D., Li, J. Q., Qin, P. M., Zhu, Y. H., Zhang, S. Z., and Chen, Y. Z., “Cerebral Response to Patient's Own Name in the Vegetative and Minimally Conscious States,” Neurology 68, no. 12 (2007): 895899.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
See Owen, and Coleman, , supra note 14.Google Scholar
Staffen, W., Kronbichler, M., Aichhorn, M., Mair, A., and Ladurner, G., “Selective Brain Activity in Response to One's Own Name in the Persistent Vegetative State,” Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 77, no. 12 (2006): 13831384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perrin, F., Schnakers, C., Schabus, M., Degueldre, C., Goldman, S., Bredart, S., Faymonville, M. E., Lamy, M., Moonen, G., Luxen, A., Maquet, P., Laureys, S., “Brain Response to One's Own Name in Vegetative State, Minimally Conscious State, and Locked-In Syndrome,” Archives of Neurology 63, no. 4 (2006): 562569.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owen, A. M., Coleman, M. R., Menon, D. K., Berry, E. L., Johnsrude, I. S., Rodd, J. M., Davis, M. H., and Pickard, J. D., “Using a Hierarchical Approach to Investigate Residual Auditory Cognition in Persistent Vegetative State,” Progress in Brain Research 150, (2005): 457471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coleman, M. R., Rodd, J. M., Davis, M. H., Johnsrude, I. S., Menon, D. K., Pickard, J. D., and Owen, A. M., “Do Vegetative Patients Retain Aspects of Language Comprehension? Evidence from fMRI,” Brain 130, no. 10 (2007): 24942507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, M. H., Coleman, M. R., Absalom, A. R., Rodd, J. M., Johnsrude, I. S., Matta, B. F., Owen, A. M., and Menon, D. K., “Dissociating Speech Perception and Comprehension at Reduced Levels of Awareness,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104, no. 41 (2007): 1603216037.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnakers, C., Perrin, F., Schabus, M., Majerus, S., Ledoux, D., Damas, P., Boly, M., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Bruno, M. A., Moonen, G., and Laureys, S., “Voluntary Brain Processing in Disorders of Consciousness,” Neurology 71, no. 20 (2008): 16141620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnakers, C., Perrin, F., Schabus, M., Hustinx, R., Majerus, S., Moonen, G., Boly, M., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Bruno, M. A., and Laureys, S., “Detecting Consciousness in a Total Locked-In Syndrome: An Active Event-Related Paradigm,” Neurocase (2009): 17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laureys, S., Pellas, F., Van Eeckhout, P., Ghorbel, S., Schnakers, C., Perrin, F., Berre, J., Faymonville, M. E., Pantke, K. H., Damas, F., Lamy, M., Moonen, G., and Goldman, S., “The Locked-In Syndrome: What Is It Like to Be Conscious but Paralyzed and Voiceless?” Progress in Brain Research 150 (2005): 495511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owen, A. M., Coleman, M. R., Boly, M., Davis, M. H., Laureys, S., and Pickard, J. D., “Detecting Awareness in the Vegetative State,” Science 313, no. 5792 (2006): 1402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owen, A., Coleman, M. R., Boly, M., Davis, M. H., Laureys, S., Jolles, D., and Pickard, J. D., “Response to Comments on “Detecting Awareness in the Vegetative State,” Science 315, no. 5816 (2007): 1221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boly, M., Coleman, M. R., Davis, M. H., Hampshire, A., Bor, D., Moonen, G., Maquet, P. A., Pickard, J. D., Laureys, S., and Owen, A. M., “When Thoughts Become Action: An fMRI Paradigm to Study Volitional Brain Activity in Non-Communicative Brain Injured Patients,” Neuroimage 36, no. 3 (2007): 979992.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
See Monti, et al., supra note 2.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, J. B., “Schiavo on the Cutting Edge: Functional Brain Imaging and its Impact on Surrogate End-of-Life Decision-Making,” Neuroethics 1 (2008): 7583.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
See Meisel, and Cerminara, , supra note 5, at 6125.Google Scholar
Henceforth, we use the term “unconscious state” or “unconsciousness” to refer to the formal state-law definition of consciousness, though it bears noting that states vary in these overall labels; i.e., some incorrectly use the term “persistent VS” instead of “permanent VS” or “permanent unconsciousness.” In these cases, the accompanying state-law definition might bear little resemblance to the clinical definition of VS.Google Scholar
See Meisel, and Cerminara, , supra note 5, at 6–128.1.Google Scholar
A note on methods: Our aim was to identify explicit state-laws definitions of consciousness, which are found most frequently in advance directive or surrogate decisionmaking statues. To generate this list we took as our starting point the thorough review of Meisel, et al., supra note 5, which we supplemented with our own review of primary sources. Many end-of-life statutes do not provide such a definition (e.g., Alaska Stat. § 13.52, which does not explicitly define a vegetative state or other conditions having to do with consciousness). While we attempted to be as exhaustive as possible in this review, it is difficult to determine with certainty the absence of a law about a particular topic.Google Scholar
Cruzan v. Director, Missouri Department of Health. 261 U.S.344 (1990).Google Scholar
Me. Rev. Stat. Ann. tit. 18-A § 5–801(s) (West 2009).Google Scholar
S.C. Code Ann. § 44-77-20(7) (1976).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Idaho Code Ann. § 39–4502(10) (2009).Google Scholar
See Skene, et al., supra note 3.Google Scholar
Ala. Code § 22–8A-3(10) (1975); 755 Ill. Comp. Stat. Ann. 40/10 (West 2009).Google Scholar
Wis. Stat. Ann. § 154.01(5m).Google Scholar
See Meisel, and Cerminara, , supra note 5, at 783.Google Scholar
Fla. Stat § 765.101(12) (West 2006).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ala. Code § 22–8A-3(10) (1975).Google Scholar
S.D. Codified Laws § 34–12D-1(7).Google Scholar
Fla. Stat § § 765.101(12) (West 2006).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ga. Code Ann. § 31-32-2(13) (West 2009); Va. Code Ann. § 54.1–2982 (West 2009).Google Scholar
Ga. Code Ann. § 31-32-2(13) (West 2009).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
755 Ill. Comp. Stat. Ann. 40/10 (West 2009).Google Scholar
Conn. Gen. Stat. Ann. § 19a-570(10) (West 2009); Ga. Code Ann. § 31-32-2.Google Scholar
See Jong, et al., supra note 23.Google Scholar
See Menon, et al., supra note 24; see Schiff, et al., supra note 26.Google Scholar
Ohio Rev. Code Ann. § 1337.11(V) (West 2009).Google Scholar
Ala. Code § 22–8A-3(10) (1975); 755 Ill. Comp. Stat. Ann. 40/10 (West 2009); Ark. Code. Ann. § 20-17-201(6) (West 2009).Google Scholar
See Laureys, et al., supra note 29.Google Scholar
See Coleman, et al., supra note 35.Google Scholar
See Davis, et al., supra note 36.Google Scholar
Fla. Stat § 765.101(12) (West 2006); Ala. Code § 22–8A-3(10) (1975); Conn. Gen. Stat. Ann. § 19a-570(10) (West 2009); Ga. Code Ann. § 31-32-2(13) (West 2009); 755 Ill. Comp. Stat. Ann. 40/10 (West 2009)Google Scholar
Ala. Code § 22–8A-4 (1975)Google Scholar
Ala. Code § 22–8A-3(10) (1975)Google Scholar
See Carey, B., “Trace of Thought Is Found in ‘Vegetative’ Patient,” The New York Times, February 4, 2010, at A1; Groopman, J., “Silent Minds,” The New Yorker, October 15, 2007, at 38–43; and supra note 1.Google Scholar
See Schnakers, et al., supra note 35.Google Scholar
Wegner, D. M., White Bears and Other Unwanted Thoughts: Suppression, Obsession, and the Psychology of Mental Control (New York: Viking, 1989).Google Scholar
Logothetis, N. K., “What We Can Do and What We Cannot Do with fMRI,” Nature 453, no. 7197 (2008): 869878.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
For an extensive review of other technologies that might be used for this purpose, see the recent theme issue of Progress in Brain Research on “Coma Science: Clinical and Ethical Implications,” especially Tshibanda, L., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Galanaud, D., Boly, M., Laureys, S., and Puybasset, L., “Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy and Diffusion Tensor Imaging in Coma Survivors: Promises and Pitfalls,” Progress in Brain Research 177 (2009): 215229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poldrack, R. A., “Can Cognitive Processes be Inferred from Neuroimaging Data?” Trends in Cognitive Science 10, no. 2 (2006): 5963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
See Tshibanda, et al., supra note 79.Google Scholar
Pettit, M. Jr., “FMRI and BF meet FRE: Brain Imaging and the Federal Rules of Evidence,” American Journal of Law & Medicine 33, no. 2–3 (2007): 319–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feigenson, N. and Park, J., “Emotions and Attributions of Legal Responsibility and Blame: A Research Review,” Law and Human Behavior 30, no. 2 (2006): 143161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frye v. U.S., 293 F. 1013 (1923).Google Scholar
Daubert v. Merrell-Dow Pharmaceuticals, 509 U.S. 579 (1993).Google Scholar
Khoshbin, L. S. and Khoshbin, S., “Imaging the Mind, Minding the Image: An Historical Introduction to Brain Imaging and the Law,” American Journal of Law & Medicine 33, nos. 2–3 (2007): 171192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCabe, D. P. and Castel, A. D., “Seeing is Believing: The Effect of Brain Images on Judgments of Scientific Reasoning,” Cognition 107, no. 1 (2008): 343–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, M. R., Dennett, D., Hacker, P., and Searle, J., Neuroscience and Philosophy: Brain, Mind, and Language (New York: Columbia University Press, 2007); Churchland, P. M., “Eliminative Materialism and the Propositional Attitudes,” Journal of Philosophy 78, no. 2 (1981): 6790.Google Scholar
Neb. Rev. Stat. § 20–403(6) (West 2009); Me. Rev. Stat. Ann. tit. 18-A § 5–801(s) (West 2009).Google Scholar
Kahane, G. and Savulescu, J., “Brain Damage and the Moral Significance of Consciousness,” The Journal of Medicine and Philosophy 34, no. 1 (2009): 626.Google Scholar
See Meisel, and Cerminara, , supra note 3, at 474.Google Scholar
Levy, N. and Savulescu, J., “Moral Significance of Phenomenal Consciousness,” Progress in Brain Research 177 (2009): 361370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Id., at 367.Google Scholar
Id., at 368. Note that this conception of access consciousness (originally defined as the state of having mental information available for reasoning, the control of action, and reporting, see Block, supra note 15) also invokes the issues of chronicity and prognosis, which, while beyond the scope of this paper, may have significant ethical and legal relevance. For example, the fixed futility of the vegetative state is one of the foundational justifications of right-to-die movement in the United States, as reviewed in Fins, J. J., “The Ethics of Measuring and Modulating Consciousness: The Imperative of Minding Time,” Progress in Brain Research 177 (2009): 371–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
See Wilkinson, et al., supra note 4.Google Scholar
See Skene, et al., supra note 3.Google Scholar
See Laureys, et al., supra note 39.Google Scholar
Lulé, D., Zickler, C., Häcker, S., Bruno, M. A., Demertzi, A., Pellas, F., Laureys, S., Kübler, A., “Life Can Be Worth Living in Locked-In Syndrome,” Progress in Brain Research 177 (2009): 339351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D.C., Consciousness Explained (Boston: Little, Brown and Co., 1991).Google Scholar
AL: Ala. Code § 22–8A-3(10) (1975); AR: Ark. Code. Ann. § 20-17-201(6) (West 2009); CT: Conn. Gen. Stat. Ann. § 19a-570(10) (West 2009); DE: Del. Code Ann. Tit. 16, “2501 (r) (1) (West 2009); FL: Fla. Stat § 765.101(12) (West 2009); GA: Ga. Code Ann. § 31-32-2(13) (West 2009); ID: Idaho Code Ann. § 39–4502(10) (2009); IL: 755 Ill. Comp. Stat. Ann. 40/10 (West 2009); MD: Md. Code Ann., [Health-Gen.] § 5–601(q) (West 2009); ME: Me. Rev. Stat. Ann. tit. 18-A § 5–801(s) (West 2009); NE: Neb. Rev. Stat. § 20–403(6) (West 2009); NH: N.H. Rev. Stat. Ann. § 137-J:2(XVII) (2009); NJ: N.J. Stat. Ann. § 26:2H-55 (West 2009); OH: Ohio Rev. Code Ann. § 1337.11(V) (West 2009); OR: Or. Rev. Stat § 127.505(19) (West 2009); PA: 20 Pa. Cons. Stat. Ann. § 5422 (West 2009); SC: S.C. Code Ann. § 44-77-20(7) (1976); SD: S.D. Codified Laws § 4–12D-1(7) (2009); VA: Va. Code Ann. § 54.1–2982 (West 2009); WI: Wis. Stat. Ann. § 154.01(5m) (West 2009); WV: W. Va. Code § 16-30-3(s) (West 2009).Google Scholar