Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T23:07:36.442Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Weird reciprocity? A ‘within-culture across-country’ trust experiment and methodological implications

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 April 2012

BJÖRN VOLLAN*
Affiliation:
Leibniz Center for Marine Tropical Ecology (ZMT), Fahrenheitstrasse 6, D-28359 Bremen, Germany and University of Mannheim, Department of Economics, L7, 3–5, D-68131 Mannheim, Germany

Abstract:

Economic experiments carried out in the computer laboratory seldom account for broader real-world contextual variables that affect humans as learning and norm-adopting individuals. The here presented ‘within-culture across-country’ design of a standard trust experiment reveals an interesting phenomenon which most probably is related to the context that people live in: South African communities expressed extremely low trust while participants from Namibia exhibited high trust, but low reciprocity, although both share the same ethnic background. The country effect between the two regions remained large even after using a matching estimator to substantiate that these differences were not driven by sample selection bias. Qualitative evidence from the study area suggests that corrupt local institutions have led to lower trust in South African communities while participants in Namibia seemingly applied a deep-rooted behavioural norm of the Nama society which they perceived to be appropriate for the exchange in the experiment.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Millennium Economics Ltd 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

This work was supported by the German Ministry of Education and Research (commission no. 01 LC 0024A). I greatly appreciate the logistic support of the BIOTA Southern Africa Project (Biodiversity Monitoring Transect Analysis) and thank Millie Saul, Richard Isaacks and Johanna Brüggemann for research assistance in the field and Lizande Kellerman for translating the instructions into Afrikaans. I received valuable comments from two anonymous reviewers, Bernd Hayo, Adam Henry, Michael Kirk, Andreas Landmann, Elinor Ostrom, Robert Poppe, Sebastian Prediger and Achim Schlüter. All remaining errors are of course mine.

References

Acemoglu, D., Johnson, S., and Robinson, J. A. (2001), ‘The Colonial Origins of Comparative Development: An Empirical Investigation’, American Economic Review, 91 (5): 13691401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreoni, J. (1990), ‘Impure Altruism and Donations to Public Goods: A Theory of Warmglow Giving’, Economic Journal, 100 (June): 464477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bardsley, N. (2010), ‘Sociality and External Validity in Experimental Economics’, Mind and Society, 9 (2): 119138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, G. S. (1974), ‘Theory of Social Interactions’, Journal of Political Economy, 82 (6): 10631093.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., and McCabe, K. A. (1995), ‘Trust, Reciprocity, and Social History’, Games and Economic Behavior, 10: 122142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boonzaier, E. (1996), The Cape Herders: A History of the Khoikhoi of Southern Africa, Cape Town and Athens, OH: Ohio University Press.Google Scholar
Cardenas, J. C. and Carpenter, J. P. (2008), ‘Behavioural Development Economics: Lessons from Field Labs in the Developing World’, Journal of Development Studies, 44 (3): 311338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cárdenas, J.-C. and Ostrom, E. (2004), ‘What do People Bring into the Game? Experiments in the Field about Cooperation in the Commons’, Agricultural Systems, 82: 307326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cashdan, E. (1985), ‘Coping with Risk: Reciprocity Among the Basarwa of Northern Botswana’, Man (New Series), 20 (3): 454474.Google Scholar
Cousins, B. (1996), Range Management and Land Reform Policy in Post-Apartheid South Africa, Occasional Paper Series 2, Cape Town: Programme for Land and Agrarian Studies, University of Western Cape.Google Scholar
Cronk, L. (2007), ‘The Influence of Cultural Framing on Play in the Trust Game: A Maasai Example’, Evolution and Human Behavior, 28: 352358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ensminger, J. (2000), ‘Market Integration and Fairness: Evidence from Ultimatum, Dictator, and Public Goods Experiments in East Africa’, in Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C. F., Fehr, E., and Gintis, H. (eds.), Foundations of Human Sociality: Economic Experiments and Ethnographic Evidence from Fifteen Small-Scale Societies, Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 356381.Google Scholar
Fischbacher, U., Gächter, S., and Fehr, E. (2001), ‘Are People Conditionally Cooperative? Evidence from a Public Goods Experiment’, Economics Letters, 71 (3): 397404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gächter, S. and Herrmann, B. (2009), ‘Reciprocity, Culture and Human Cooperation: Previous Insights and a New Cross-Cultural Experiment’, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences, 364 (1518): 791806.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gächter, S., Herrmann, B., and Thöni, C. (2004), ‘Trust, Voluntary Cooperation, and Socio-Economic Background: Survey and Experimental Evidence’, Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 55 (4): 505531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gouldner, A. W. (1960), ‘The Norm of Reciprocity – A Preliminary Statement’, American Sociological Review, 25 (2): 161178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greig, F. and Bohnet, I. (2008), ‘Is There Reciprocity in a Reciprocal-Exchange Economy? Evidence of Gendered Norms from a Slum in Nairobi, Kenya’, Economic Inquiry, 46 (1): 7783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckman, J. J., Ichimura, H., and Todd, P. (1998), ‘Matching as an Econometric Evaluation Estimator’, Review of Economic Studies, 65 (2): 261294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helmke, G. and Levitsky, S. (2004), ‘Informal Institutions and Comparative Politics: A Research Agenda’, Perspectives on Politics, 2 (4): 725740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C. F., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., McElreath, R., Alvard, M. S., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Henrich, N. S., Hill, K., Gil-White, F., Gurven, M., Marlowe, F. W., Patton, J. Q., and Tracer, D. P. (2005), ‘“Economic Man” in Cross-Cultural Perspective: Behavioral Experiments in 15 Small-Scale Societies’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28: 795815.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., Ensminger, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., Cardenas, J. C., Gurven, M., Gwako, E., Henrich, N. S., Lesorogol, C., Marlowe, F. W., Tracer, D. P., and Ziker, J. (2010)a, ‘Markets, Religion, Community Size, and the Evolution of Fairness and Punishment’, Science, 327 (5972): 14801484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., and Norenzayan, A. (2010)b, ‘The Weirdest People in the World’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33 (2–3): 6183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrmann, B. and Thöni, C. (2008), ‘Measuring Conditional Cooperation: A Replication Study in Russia’, Experimental Economics, 12: 8792.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrmann, B., Thöni, C., and Gächter, S. (2008), ‘Antisocial Punishment Across Societies’, Science, 319 (5868): 13621367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodgson, G. (2001), How Economics Forgot History: The Problem of Historical Specificity in Social Science, London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holm, H. J. and Danielson, A. (2005)a, ‘Tropic Trust Versus Nordic Trust: Experimental Evidence From Tanzania and Sweden’, Economic Journal, 115 (503): 505532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janssen, M. A. and Anderies, J. M. (2011), ‘Governing the Commons: Learning from Field and Laboratory Experiments’, Ecological Economics, 70 (9): 15691620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, N. D. and Mislin, A. A. (2011), ‘Trust Games: A Meta-Analysis’, Journal of Economic Psychology, 32 (5): 865889.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiyonari, T., Yamagishi, T., Cook, K. S., and Cheshire, C. (2006), ‘Does Trust Beget Trustworthiness? Trust and Trustworthiness in Two Games and Two Cultures: A Research Note’, Social Psychology Quarterly, 69 (3): 270283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klocke-Daffa, S. (1999), ‘Wenn du hast, musst du geben. Traditionelle Sicherungssysteme im neuen Staat. Das Beispiel der Nama in Namibia’, Zeitschrift für Ethnologie, 124 (2): 299317.Google Scholar
Kocher, M. G., Cherry, T., Kroll, S., Netzer, R., and Sutter, M. (2008), ‘Conditional Cooperation on Three Continents’, Economics Letters, 101 (3): 175178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kössler, R. (2001), ‘The Berseba Captaincy After 1938. Collective Identity and Conflict in a Traditional Community in Southern Namibia’, Afrika Spectrum, 36 (3): 347362.Google Scholar
Lamba, S. and Mace, R. (2011), ‘Demography and Ecology Drive Variation in Cooperation Across Human Populations’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U S A, 108 (35): 1442614430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levitt, S. D. and List, J. A. (2007), ‘What do Laboratory Experiments Measuring Social Preferences Reveal About the Real World?’, Journal of Economic Perspectives, 21 (2): 153174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mauss, M. (1990), The Gift: The Form and Reason for Exchange in Archaic Societies, London: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
May, H. and Lahiff, E. (2007), ‘Land Reform in Namaqualand, 1994–2005: A Review’, Journal of Arid Environments, 70: 782798.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
North, D. C. (1990), Institutions, Institutional Change, and Economic Performance, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nunn, N. and Wantchekon, L. (2009), ‘The Transatlantic Slave Trade and the Evolution of Mistrust in Africa: An Empirical Investigation’, Journal of Economic History, 69 (2): 574575.Google Scholar
Ostrom, E. (1990), Governing the Commons: The Evolution of Institutions for Collective Action, New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poteete, A. R., Janssen, M. A., and Ostrom, E. (2009), Working Together: Collective Action, the Commons, and Multiple Methods in Practice, Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Prediger, S., Vollan, B., and Frölich, M. (2011), ‘The Impact of Culture and Ecology on Cooperation in a Common-Pool Resource Experiment’, Ecological Economics, 70 (9): 15991608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rohde, R., Hoffman, T. M., and Cousins, B. (1999), Experimenting with the Commons: A Comparative History of the Effects of Land Policy on Pastoralism in Two Former ‘Reserves’ in Namibia and South Africa, Occasional Paper Series No.12, Cape Town: Programme for Land and Agrarian Studies, University of Western Cape.Google Scholar
Rosenbaum, P. R. and Rubin, D. B. (1983), ‘The Central Role of the Propensity Score in Observational Studies for Causal Effects’, Biometrika, 70 (1): 4155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roth, A. E., Prasnikar, V., Okunofujiwara, M., and Zamir, S. (1991), ‘Bargaining and Market Behaviour in Jerusalem, Ljubliana, Pittsburgh, and Tokyo – An Experimental Study’, American Economic Review, 81 (5): 10681095.Google Scholar
Sachdeva, S., Iliev, R., and Medin, D. L. (2009), ‘Sinning Saints and Saintly Sinners: The Paradox of Moral Self-Regulation’, Psychological Science, 20 (4): 523528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sahlins, M. (1972), Stone Age Economics, Chicago: Aldine-Atherton.Google Scholar
Shackleton, S., von Maltitz, G., and Evans, J. (1998), Factors, Conditions and Criteria for Successful Management of Natural Resources Held Under a Common Property Regime: A South African Perspective, Occasional Paper Series 8, Cape Town: Programme for Land and Agrarian Studies, University of Western Cape.Google Scholar
Tracer, D. P. (2003), ‘Selfishness and Fairness in Economic and Evolutionary Perspective: An Experimental Economic Study in Papua New Guinea’, Current Anthropology, 44 (3): 432438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tracer, D. P. (2004), ‘Market Integration, Reciprocity, and Fairness in Rural Papua New Guinea: Results from a Two-Village Ulitmatum Game Experiment’, in Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C. F., Fehr, E., and Gintis, H. (eds.), Foundations of Human Sociality Economic Experiments and Ethnographic Evidence from Fifteen Small-Scale Societies, Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 232260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vollan, B. (2008), ‘Socio-Ecological Explanations for Crowding-Out Effects from Economic Field Experiments in Southern Africa’, Ecological Economics, 67 (4): 560573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vollan, B. (2011), ‘The Difference between Kinship and Friendship: (Field-) Experimental Evidence on Trust and Punishment’, Journal of Socio-Economics, 40 (1): 1425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vollan, B. (2012), ‘Pitfalls of Externally Initiated Collective Action: A Case Study from South Africa’, World Development, 40 (4): 758770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westaway, A. (1994), Maladminstration, Manoeuvring, and the Meandering Transition to Democracy in Namaqualand, Athlone: Surplus People Project.Google Scholar
Wiessner, P. (1977), Hxaro, a Regional System of Reciprocity for Reducing Risk Among the !Kung San, Ann Arbor: University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Yamagishi, T. and Kiyonari, T. (2000), ‘The Group as the Container of Generalized Reciprocity’, Social Psychology Quarterly, 63 (2): 116132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar