1. Introduction
This article addresses regional differences between syllable structures of different German dialects. While syllable structure of Standard German has been subject of extensive research (see Menzerath Reference Menzerath1954, Seiler Reference Seiler1962, Wiese Reference Wiese1988, Ortmann Reference Ortmann1991, Hall Reference Hall1992, Kohler Reference Kohler1995, Wiese Reference Wiese2000, Best Reference Best2013, Levickij Reference Levickij2014 among many others), very few studies are dedicated to—usually very specific aspects of—regional phonotactics (for example, Karch Reference Karch1981, Liberman Reference Liberman and Winter1997, Spiekermann Reference Spiekermann2000, Kraehenmann Reference Kraehenmann2003, Hall Reference Hall2009, Seiler & Würth Reference Seiler, Würth, Caro Reina and Szczepaniak2014, Kleber Reference Kleber2017, Klingler, Moosmüller & Scheutz Reference Klingler, Moosmüller, Scheutz and Lacerda2017, Caro Reina Reference Caro Reina2019). A comparative examination of the phonotactic particularities of individual dialect areas is sadly lacking. It is, for example, unclear whether there are regional preferences for monosyllabic or polysyllabic words, preferences for specific consonant clusters or particular sequences within the sonority scale, however defined, or violations of the Sonority Sequencing Generalization (Sievers Reference Sievers1881, Selkirk Reference Selkirk, Mark Aronoff, Oehrle and Stephens1984). More generally, with regard to the major linguistic areas the question arises as to whether Low German dialects prefer different syllable structures from High German dialects, and if so, what the preferred structures are in each case. By answering such questions, a conclusive, typological differentiation of German as a variative language becomes possible.
This is the starting point for the present study, which aims at a micro-typological description of German dialects, focusing on the structure of ca. 13,500 monosyllables across 182 locations, within the Federal Republic of Germany. Monosyllables were chosen because they usually contain the most extensive consonant clusters and thus provide information not only on the maximum number of consonants in the syllables of a language, but also on the prosodic properties of the syllable structure (see, for instance, the contributions in Stolz et al. Reference Stolz, Nau and Stroh2012). Based on a corpus of phonetically-transcribed, phonological words (Phonetischer Atlas der Bundesrepublik Deutschland, PAD; see Göschel Reference Göschel1992, Reference Göschel2000), systematic geographical differences in both the segmental and prosodic organization of segments within the onset and the coda in different German dialects are demonstrated. In so doing, this article sheds new light on the phonotactics of German from the perspective of language geography.
The structure of this article is as follows: Section 2 provides background information on the phonotactics of both Standard German and German dialects. Section 3 describes material and methods, while sections 4 to 6 present the results of the individual analyses. Section 4 outlines the regional distribution of monosyllables, while section 5 analyzes the syllable structure of dialects. Section 6 provides a model of monosyllables for selected regions. Section 7 includes a discussion and section 8 concludes.Footnote 1
2. Background
According to Clements & Keyser (1983:29), German belongs to Type IV languages, which are languages with a maximum number of core syllables (CV, V, CVC, VC, with V indicating both long and short vowels), as is typical for most Germanic languages (Oostendorp Reference Oostendorp2020). Reflecting on the ways in which segments are combined within a syllable, Maddieson (Reference Maddieson, Dryer and Haspelmath2013) classifies German, on the basis of an evaluation of empirical studies, as a language with a “complex syllable structure,” of the type (C)(C)(C)V(C)(C)(C)(C), subsequently referred to as (C3)V(C4). By “complexity” Maddieson means clustering of consonants within onset and/or coda.Footnote 2 For the sake of exemplification, 1 presents two monosyllables of the type C3VC3 and CVC4 (see also Seiler Reference Seiler1962:382f.), which fit in with Maddieson’s onset/coda model. Note that V and C refer to individual segments, which is why sequences such as /pf/ are defined as CC (see section 3.2).
A more detailed view of syllable structure in Standard German is provided by Kohler (Reference Kohler1995:175f.) with reference to monosyllables. Kohler counts the number of consonants in a cluster across morpheme boundaries, and so under his approach not only the …VC4 but also the …VC5 pattern is possible (see 2; + indicates the morphological boundary).Footnote 3 The special status of these patterns becomes clear because, in contrast to less complex monosyllables with, for example, CV structure, these words typically do not form minimal pairs, with the exception of a few examples, as in 2d. However, monosyllables with more than five segments are rather rare in Standard German, according to Menzerath (Reference Menzerath1954:96).
From such configurations Kohler (Reference Kohler1995) derives a linear syllable scheme with four structural positions (“Strukturpositionen”; SP) -1 –– 0 –– +1 –– +2 with up to three Cs in the onset (=SP -1) followed by the nucleus (=SP 0), which is typically a vowel, another set of maximally three consonants (=SP +1) and, finally, another set with up to two consonants (=SP +2). While positions SP -1 to SP +1 are found in noninflected forms, SP +2 is found in morphologically complex forms, as in 2. Kohler’s model implies that, as far as the onset is concerned, a counterpart of the morphologically conditioned SP +2—a possible SP -2— does not exist in Standard German. Modifying Maddieson’s template to incorporate Kohler’s proposal leads to a possible syllable structure of the type (C3)V(C3)+(C2), where V and + indicate the boundaries of the structural positions.
The examples provided by 2 demonstrate that the preferred domain of more complex C clusters is the coda rather than the onset, which might conflict with the Coda Law, formulated by Vennemann (Reference Vennemann1988:21), according to which (additional prosodic characteristics aside) a “syllable coda is the more preferred […] the smaller the number of speech sounds in the coda.” However, considering, for example, the highly complex syllables in Pacific languages analyzed by Easterday (Reference Easterday2019), it is also clear that complex codas are by no means uncommon; there are language systems with much more complex C clusters in both onset and coda, which at the same time follow different prosodic organization.
Considering that Germanic languages “are […] rather similar in the kinds of syllables they allow” (Oostendorp Reference Oostendorp2020:33), one might assume that Standard German syllable structure as described above also holds for syllables in German dialects. This may even be the reason why dialect phonotactics has not been thoroughly investigated: To date, there is no comparative study focusing on the phonotactic characteristics of regional varieties within German-speaking areas. This is unfortunate, as there is evidence of certain remarkable differences between Standard German and German dialects. For example, Alber & Meneguzzo (Reference Alber, Meneguzzo, Bidese, Cognola and Moroni2016:34) point to morphologically complex onset clusters such as in 3a, suggesting that the SP -2—which, according to Kohler (Reference Kohler1995), does not exist in Standard German—can be documented rather easily in dialects (see also Bachmann Reference Bachmann2000:54 and Lameli & Werth Reference Lameli, Werth and Hennig2017:75). Such morphologically complex clusters are found in family names such as Gsell, where the historical gi- or ge- prefix, as in 3b, is reduced.
At the same time, example 3a may indicate a possible preference for monosyllabic structures in dialects, for example, due to schwa loss. However, to date there has been no study dedicated to the phonotactic relationship between monosyllables and polysyllables across German language regions. Provided that there are regions with preferences for monosyllables—as suggested by 3a—it is highly likely that these are regions with more complex C clusters. This hypothesis is partially confirmed, as discussed in section 5.2.
Another well-known difference between the standard language and dialects is described by Lass (Reference Lass1984:206f.; see also Hall Reference Hall1992:201, 2011:147). In some Standard German words, the velar nasal is to be found in a final syllable/word position, as in 4a, while the same words in Low German dialects come with an additional stop (=oral release) following the nasal, as in 4b.
This difference has been used as an argument for different rule orderings in Standard German versus dialects. Starting from the representation /dɪng/, Standard German follows the path in 5a, whereas Low German follows the path in 5b. In 5a, final obstruent devoicing fails because /g/ is already deleted, whereas in 5b g-deletion fails because /g/ has been devoiced before g-deletion could occur.
Note that in the same dialects, in the plural form /dɪngə/, /g/ is deleted, just as in Standard German: [dɪŋə]. This is because in Low German dialects, g-deletion also occurs in the coda before stops, as in [an.faŋt] ‘begin-3SG.PRS’, which suggests that the rule order in 5b applies only syllable/word finally (see, for example, Map 39 and the appropriate sound recordings in Schmidt et al. 2008–2021ff. from the South of Hamburg). However, in the region in the North of Hamburg as well as in the East-Frisian dialect area [an.faŋkt] also occurs, which suggests that even between Low German dialects there might be different rule orders. Consequently, 5b possibly holds for most, but not necessarily for all Low German dialects. How these differences are distributed geographically is another unanswered question.
The examples in 3 and 4 alone suggest that i) German dialects might follow—at least in part—a different organization of syllables from Standard German; ii) there are regional differences in the structuring of monosyllables in German dialects; iii) dialects differ in the complexity of C clusters in both onset and coda, and iv) from the perspective of generative phonology, dialects might follow different rule orderings. Above all, however, the examples make it clear that a comprehensive description and analysis of the regional variation of German phonotactics is highly desirable. This article focuses on the regional distribution of monosyllabic structuring in German dialects. In particular, items i), ii), and iii) form the three basic hypotheses addressed in the present study.
3. Material and Methods
3.1 Corpus
The PAD corpus is a collection of narrow phonetic transcriptions, originally assembled as part of the PAD project during the 1980s and 1990s (see Göschel Reference Göschel1992, Reference Göschel2000).Footnote 4 The PAD project documented the speech of native dialect speakers (NORMs according to Chambers & Trudgill Reference Chambers and Trudgill1998:29), who were asked to translate the so-called Wenker sentences into their local dialects. The Wenker sentences are a standard instrument used to analyze phonological and morphological particularities of both High German and Low German dialects. They were developed from 1876 onward, as the basis for the linguistic atlas of the German Empire (Sprachatlas des Deutschen Reichs, see Lameli Reference Lameli2014), which was compiled between 1889 and 1923. Originally, the atlas was based on 40 sentences (see Wenker Reference Wenker, Lameli, von Johanna Heil and Wellendorf2013:23). Later, however, depending on the objectives of specific research, more sentences and individual words were added (Bellmann Reference Bellmann1970:24–27, Fleischer Reference Fleischer2017). Between 1956 and 1990, sound recordings were also made of both the original 40 sentences (ca. 450 words) and additional items.
From these data, Göschel selected recordings from 182 locations throughout the Federal Republic of Germany and constructed a subsample of 201 monosyllabic words (simplex words and compounds), which form the basis of the PAD corpus (see Göschel Reference Göschel1992:64; Nerbonne & Siedle Reference Nerbonne and Siedle2005 provide a list of the words). Between 1980 and 1995, a team of professional phonetic transcribers at the Research Center Deutscher Sprachatlas (Marburg) generated narrow IPA transcriptions from this material. The transcriptions were recorded during a supervised process. First, the independent transcriptions of two transcribers were evaluated. Then these two transcriptions were merged into a single transcription, which was understood to be the best possible nonacoustic approximation to the sound signal (Nerbonne Reference Nerbonne, Lameli, Kehrein and Rabanus2010:478). To ensure an optimal reliability of the transcriptions, the transcribers’ work was evaluated on a regular basis using quantitative measures (see Almeida & Braun Reference Almeida and Braun1986). As a result, the PAD corpus mainly focuses on segmental issues but also reflects prosodic particularities (for example, accents). In order to express the fine-grained phonetic detail in the PAD corpus, the phonetic transcriptions make extensive use of diacritics. These diacritics pose a challenge for a phonetic comparison of the dialects but are of minor relevance for the phonotactic analysis, as is demonstrated in the present study.
Even though the transcriptions were completed, the intended atlas was not. From 2003 onward, the handwritten transcriptions were recoded as machine-readable X-SAMPA code at Groningen University. Based on this code, a reconversion into machine-readable IPA transcription was compiled at the Research Center in Marburg. The PAD material, as it is used in the present study, contains 30,422 tokens across a total of 182 locations, documenting ca. 3,800 phonetically-specified allophones, which are in contrast with the ca. 40 vowel and consonant phonemes of Standard German (Wiese Reference Wiese2000:20–23). Among these tokens are a total of 13,492 occurrences of monosyllables (44%), which are analyzed in the subsequent sections.
In recent years, the PAD corpus has been used in different studies with different aims, such as the classification of the German-speaking region (Nerbonne & Siedle Reference Nerbonne and Siedle2005), the comparison of dialect regions within different languages (Nerbonne Reference Nerbonne, Lameli, Kehrein and Rabanus2010), the development of quantitative methodology (Prokić et al. Reference Prokić, Çöltekin, Nerbonne, Butt, Carpendale, Penn, Prokić and Cysouw2012, Prokić Reference Prokić, Wieling, Kroon, van Noord and Bouma2017), and the analysis of phonological complexity (Lameli & Werth Reference Lameli, Werth and Hennig2017). In all of these instances, linguistic analysis mainly focuses on phonological or morphophonological issues.
3.2. Data Preparation and Representation of Segments
Considering Nerbonne’s (2010:480) results, according to which the six words he selected from the PAD corpus alone provide 322 vowel variants, an extraordinary narrowness of the phonetic transcriptions becomes evident. However, as this narrowness is impossible to handle both in a phonetic and in a phonological analysis, the transcriptions require certain modifications.
For a more detailed description of syllable structure, segments were represented as vowels, glides, liquids, nasals, and obstruents (VGLNO-tier), which, for example, allows one to see the differences in sonority between the segments (see Clements Reference Clements, Kingston and Mary1990).Footnote 5 In the analysis below Hall’s (1992:64f.) proposal was adopted not to differentiate between stops and fricatives. Furthermore, as this study does not consider possible differences between strong and weak elements of the nucleus, in accordance with Maddieson Reference Maddieson, Dryer and Haspelmath2013 and Kohler Reference Kohler1995, V represents both long and short vowels, as well as diphthongs. Under this approach, a separate nucleus tier, as set by Clements & Keyser (1983:16), can be dispensed with.
Based on this approach, a representation of demisyllables is derived (Clements Reference Clements, Kingston and Mary1990), which differentiates initial demisyllables (onset + nucleus) from final demisyllables (nucleus + coda). In so doing, the segments are integrated into a base model of phonotactic structure, consisting of the three subsyllabic constituents: onset, nucleus, and coda. A rhyme tier is not necessary because it would be coextensive with the final demisyllable. For convenience, the term rhyme is used continuously even though it is to be understood as a cover term for a final demisyllable. Finally, from the VGLNO-tier, a simpler CV-tier is derived, aimed at the description of more common similarities between the dialects regarding syllable peaks (V, here vowels) and nonsyllabic segments (C, here consonants and glides).
The status of [ts] and [pf] needs to be established. Within a syllable or morpheme, [ts] and [pf] are usually considered to be affricates, although their status as monosegmental or bisegmental units has been discussed at length in the literature since Trubetzkoy Reference Trubetzkoy1939. On the one hand, it is not hard to find evidence that they can be replaced by monosegmental sounds to form new words, as in 6, which is one reason in support of defining them as single segments (affricates). This is also reasonable from the perspective of phonotactics: For example, they form the only clusters that occur in both onset and coda (further arguments are provided by Wiese Reference Wiese2000).
On the other hand, there are instances, where in the same environment only one of the segments is replaced by another, as shown in 7a,b (Standard German). In addition, looking at the dialects in the PAD corpus, there are instances where the second component of [ts] can be deleted, as in 7c (Gondorf, Upper Saxon region), possibly due to the quality of the vowel.Footnote 6 Finally, as example 7d (Linz, Upper Saxon region) shows, the stop alone can also be deleted. It is not clear how to handle instances such as 7c,d, which have not been considered in the literature to date.
Furthermore, from an empirical point of view, it should be taken into account that all major corpus-based studies on the German language since the 1950s have been using a biphonematic definition, which raises a problem of comparability. To make sure that the data in this study can be analyzed using the models of Kohler (Reference Kohler1995) and Maddieson (Reference Maddieson, Dryer and Haspelmath2013), I followed the tradition of treating these sounds as individual segments within a cluster (see Prinz & Wiese Reference Prinz and Wiese1991 for a different point of view). The same holds for less homorganic but frequently-realized consonant combinations, such as [tʃ] or [dʒ], which are also defined as bisegmental sounds in the present article.
Finally, the glottal stop must be addressed, the dialectal status of which is somewhat unclear at present. Based on Pröll & Kleiner’s (2016) reading test that involved individual standard German words, the glottal stop is dependent on particular lexemes (see Alber Reference Alber2001 for different results). This particular speech style effect, however, cannot be identified in the PAD corpus. As regards to southern German dialects, Caro Reina (Reference Caro Reina2019:327) finds that glottal stop insertion is “highly dependent on speech tempo since it only applies in lento style.” This observation is in line with an observation made much earlier by Beck (Reference Beck1926) to which Caro Reina (Reference Caro Reina2019:328) additionally refers. This dependency on the speech style explains why in the PAD data the glottal stop occurs only very rarely in prevocalic word initial position, and when it does, it is very unsystematic and without any regional pattern. This is because the PAD data are comprised of longer sentences, not individual words and are thus closer to freely spoken language and allegro style. Consequently, in order not to cause any random effects, the glottal stop is not considered in the present study.
4. Regional Distribution of Monosyllables
Kleiner et al.’s (Reference Kleiner, Knöbel and Mangold2015) examination of the Duden pronunciation dictionary revealed that 43% of Standard German equivalents of the PAD concepts are represented as monosyllables. In the PAD data, the number of monosyllables ranges between 31% and 61% per location (44% on average). This is a considerable variation, with significant spatial clustering, which was confirmed by a test of spatial auto-correlation using the global Moran’s I measure (I=.592, I E=-.005, p<.001).Footnote 7 There are, in other words, geographical preferences for the use of monosyllables versus polysyllables.
Figure 1 shows the areal distribution of 13,492 monosyllabic tokens in the PAD corpus. Diagram A shows the number of monosyllables relative to the total number of words at every PAD location: The larger the extent of the circles, the more frequent are the monosyllables. Diagram B shows the spatial interpolation of the data in diagram A. The preferences for monosyllables are highlighted by an ordinary kriging model: Red indicates a high frequency of monosyllables, blue indicates low frequency.Footnote 8
Interestingly, figure 1 reveals no explicit coincidence in relation to the larger areas of Low German or High German (see figure A in Appendix for an overview of German dialects). Nevertheless, there are core areas of both larger numbers and smaller numbers of monosyllables (red and blue color in figure 1B, respectively) in both the Low German area and the High German area. Larger numbers are concentrated in the northern region of the Low German area (northern Low German) and in the southeastern region of the High German area (Bavaria), while smaller numbers of monosyllables are concentrated in an East-West direction in the central region.
A number of phenomena contribute to the pattern in figure 1. The first group of phenomena encompasses more recent phonological processes, such as schwa apocopation, as in 8a, or t-deletion, as in 8b. It also includes more historical processes, such as g-deletion, as in 8c, or morphophonological processes, such as the deletion of the prefix ge- before stop, as in 8d. There are also cases in which phonological processes typical of Standard German as well as many dialects did not occur for various historical reasons, such as anaptyxis of i, as in 8e, and schwa anaptyxis, as in 8f. Finally, a third group involves phonological processes that are related to schwa but are only sparsely documented, such as schwa anaptyxis, as in 8g (note that the schwa in [dʊəp] is due to /r/ vocalization), schwa syncopation, as in 8h, and lexical reanalysis, as in 8j.
This list is by no means complete, but it gives an idea of the diversity of the most typical phenomena reflected in the PAD corpus. Also, most of these processes are among the typical phonological phenomena in the Germanic languages discussed by Hall (Reference Hall2020).
Figure 2 shows the distribution of the phenomena exemplified in 8 that shape up the pattern in figure 1.
Figure 2 illustrates that the processes in (8) affect different parts of the language region. Furthermore, they obviously do not necessarily hinder one another. For example, schwa apocopation in 8a and schwa anaptyxis in 8f occur in the same region—in western and eastern Germany. In general, processes that involve schwa seem to be especially relevant among these phenomena. This becomes evident if one combines the maps showing schwa apocopation, as in 8a, and the deletion of the prefix ge-, as in 8d, which, in fact, leads to the loss of a schwa syllable. Together the two processes shape the pattern of the bluish band in figure 1B. This is not really surprising given that schwa deletion has been one of the most important processes in the development of German dialects from the 15th century onward (Lindgren Reference Lindgren1953). The loss of schwa not only causes the loss of morphemes (see Lameli Reference Lameli, Nevaci, Floarea and Farcaş2021) but also leads to a reduction in the number of syllables, as schwa serves as a syllable’s nucleus. This, in turn, leads to an increase in the number of monosyllables in German, as stated by Werner (Reference Werner, Birnbaum, Durovic, Jacob, Nilsson, Sjoeberg and Worth1978:483). At the same time, especially in the case of schwa epenthesis, context seems to be important. As figure 2 demonstrates, /l/ in 8e,f seems to provide a more productive context than /r/ in 8g. Many of the instances of schwa anaptyxis, such as in 8g— for example, in the transition zone between Alemannic/Swabian and Bavarian (central South)—could be related to the apical /r/ allophone, so that epenthesis might be due to a more general context [+sonorant, +anterior]. It is highly likely that similar conditions also hold for other phenomena. This is, however, beyond the scope of the present article.
5. The Syllable Structure of German Dialects
5.1. The CV Pattern
According to Maddieson (Reference Maddieson, Pellegrini, Marsico, Chitoran and Coupé2009, Reference Maddieson, Dryer and Haspelmath2013), Standard German is a language with complex syllables, by which he means configurations of the (C3)V(C4) type. Diagram A in figure 3 shows frequency of the CV type among monosyllables in the PAD corpus, whereas B shows the regional distribution of the CVC type, which is the most frequent syllable type. The size of the dots indicates the percentages of CVC-type syllables at the respective location; AW refers to the number of CVC-type syllables in the speech of the news anchor, Anne Will.
On the one hand, figure 3A confirms Maddieson’s (2009, 2013) assessment of Standard German in terms of configurations, with up to three or four Cs in the onset or coda. On the other hand, 7 of the 18 monosyllabic types in the corpus have only one C in the onset and/or coda (CVC, CV, VC), and the top eight among the most frequent syllables are those with up to two Cs in the same positions (CVC, CV, CVCC, VC, CCVC, CCV, VCC, CCVCC). In contrast, syllables with CCC+ clusters are rare. The most frequent type is CVC (S=4,447), the nucleus of which is usually either a diphthong or a long vowel. This finding is consistent with Kohler’s (1995:226) statistics for monosyllables in the standard German language, where CVC is also the most common type. Following Maddieson, this type is characterized by rather moderate complexity. In regard to the …C5 types (as in 2a,b), the PAD data provide no documentatiton, which is obviously due to the lack of particular lexemes in the corpus.
In addition, figure 3B demonstrates that even if the CVC type can be found at every location, it is clearly dominant in northern Germany, in a smaller region in western Germany, and in the central German area. Comparing this distribution with the distribution in figure 1, a correlation between this pattern and the number of monosyllables and polysyllables is evident for the Low German area. Furthermore, in the upper left corner of figure 3B, a dot marks the spoken language of a professional news anchor on German television (Anne Will), who read the PAD items out loud.Footnote 10 Obviously, the news anchor demonstrates a moderate number of CVC-type syllables, thus challenging the assumption that spoken Standard German is more oriented toward northern German phonology (König Reference König and Stickel1997:250).
5.1. Filling the C Position
The areal distribution of the CVC type shown in figure 3B raises the question of whether regional preferences should be expected for other syllable types as well. As it turns out, this is indeed the case: Figure 4 shows the regional distribution of syllable types differing in the number of Cs in the onset and/or coda. Diagrams in row A show the distribution of monosyllables with only one C in onset and/or coda; diagrams in row B show monosyllables with two Cs in onset and/or coda, and diagrams in row C show monosyllables with three or more Cs in onset and/or coda. The colored lines in maps in diagram 1 in each row represent decision boundaries, based on a trained k-nearest neighbor classifier. Percentages refer to the relative number of the particular types in the total of all monosyllabic realizations at the particular location. Diagrams in A/1, B/1, and C/1 at the beginning of each row provide an overview by combining C positions to the left and right of the nucleus. The maps differ only in the number of Cs in onset/coda positions.Footnote 11 As a reference point, the number of clusters corresponding to the spoken language of a professional news anchor is highlighted (AW; see previous section). A more precise differentiation between onsets and codas is provided by the maps on the right (A/2, B/2, and C/2 versus A/3, B/3, and C/3).
As figure 3B reveals, monosyllables with a single consonant in either onset or coda, that is, CV… or …VC (figure 4, row A; S=7,855) are most frequent in the PAD corpus; they represent almost half of all instances. All the more remarkable is the fact that there is a clear preference for the CV and VC types in the northern region and parts of central Germany as opposed to southern Germany.
The distributional pattern of the most complex but also rarest syllables with three or more Cs (CCC+) is almost the opposite. These syllables dominate the southern and central regions of Germany (figure 4, row C; S=567). There are two main reasons for their (relatively) high number in southern Germany. First, some of the CCC+ onset clusters contain an obstruent + /ʃ/, as shown in 9 (see also the examples in 3 and example 12d). Historically, such CCC+ clusters result from schwa syncopation and thus represent a more recent stage of language development (Werner Reference Werner, Birnbaum, Durovic, Jacob, Nilsson, Sjoeberg and Worth1978).
Second, some of the onset clusters are due to the High German consonant shift, which led to the emergence of /pf/ and /ts/ clusters in most parts of the southern (as well as central German) regions and, additionally, the cluster /kx/ in the Southwest, near the border with Switzerland. Naturally, the question arises as to what extent the patterns in row C of figure 4 depend on /pf/ and /ts/ being defined as bisegmental clusters. As it turns out, however, defining /pf/ and /ts/ as single segments would not alter the picture significantly: The southwestern region (Alemannic) would, indeed, thin out slightly, but the North–South contrast would remain. The eastern (Bavarian) region would remain unaffected as well, due to the fact that more consonant clusters in this area come from the loss of schwa, as in 9.
In addition, there are some other, more rare phenomena, which are due not to language history but to particular phonological processes. For example, CCC coda clusters as in POM [ʊntʃ] ‘our.DAT-SG.N’, SWAB [ɛlpf] ‘eleven’ or MERC [ɑlts] ‘as’ are due to a process of stop insertion between a sonorant and a fricative in the coda position of the type ∅ → P/{N,L} F]σ under the condition that P is homorganic with either {N,L} or F.Footnote 12 Although this particular rule does not systematically apply in Standard German (STD [ʊns], [ɛlf], [als]), the phenomenon itself is quite common and is an instance of coarticulation. Although more detailed research is needed on this topic, it is possible that due to regional restrictions there could be a phonological difference between Standard German and dialects.
Finally, the most diffuse clusters are CC (figure 4, row B; S=4,862), which show no clear regional pattern. The only syllable type that shows a slight regional tendency is VCC, as its frequency increases from North to South.
The consolidated data on all C positions in figure 4 (maps A/1–C/1), show that particular syllable types become more common as one moves from North to South. In particular, the number of C positions in both the onset and the coda tends to increase: C > CC > CCC+. It was hypothesized in section 2 that should there be regions with preferences for monosyllables, these regions could reveal larger C clusters. Contrasting figure 4 with figure 1 shows that this is the case in the southern region but not in the northern region, where CVC is the most prominent type.
Finally, syllables without Cs also occur. In this respect, figure 5 indicates first that syllables consisting of only a V are rare (S=172), and second that these syllables mostly occur in the Upper German area, with most of them in the Alemannic language group (including Swabian).
Based on figures 4 and 5, the southwestern region (primarily the Alemannic and Swabian dialect region) is of particular interest, as it exhibits both extremes—the V-only syllables and the CCC+ clusters (even though the latter are not as frequent as they are in the southeastern region—Bavarian). The V-only syllables occur in weak forms, as shown in 10.Footnote 13
Almost none of these syllables come with a glottal stop, and those that do follow no specific regional pattern, which, to some extent, seems to contradict the results of Alber (Reference Alber2001) and Caro Reina (Reference Caro Reina2019), who demonstrate regional preferences for word initial glottal stop insertion, for example. However, as mentioned above, Caro Reina (Reference Caro Reina2019:327f.) also finds glottal stop insertion to be dependent on lento style, which is not documented by the PAD data. Therefore, one can say that the Wenker sentences, as used in the PAD corpus, seem not to trigger glottal stop insertion.
5.3. Modeling of CV/VC Sequences in Monosyllables
While figure 3A shows the frequency of the CV pattern (that is, any syllable consisting of any number of C and/or V), with the CVC type as the most prominent one, figure 3B shows the regional distribution of the latter. In the previous sections, these syllable types were discussed in terms of their position within words, with the most extreme case being a VC4 sequence. The discussion in the following sections moves down to the level of segments: I analyze the internal structure of these syllables in order to identify possible combinations of segments in them. Based on the results of this analysis regional models of segment sequences are then developed.
In order to translate the insight in figure 3 into the actual ordering of segments (for instance, CV versus VC), northern Germany, which is dominated by the CVC structure, was contrasted with southern Germany, where a preference for more complex C clusters was found as well as for V-only syllables. For a more detailed description, 33 locations across three different regions—one from northern Germany and two from southern Germany—were randomly chosen, with 11 locations per region (L=33∼18% of the 182 PAD locations). These locations are highlighted in figure 6.
For all three regions, the different CV types were combined across all locations. On this basis, a transition matrix of Cs and Vs was generated in order to assemble individual Markov chains. The transition matrix contained the relative frequency of the joint occurrence of each pair of segments. By way of example, table 1 reports the transition probabilities between segments in CVC syllables, comparing northern Germany with southern Germany. Regional differences in terms of frequency of CVC syllables can be seen in figure 3B. By breaking CVCs down to their individual C–V–C segments (including the initialization (α) and the closure (ε) of the syllable), table 1 illustrates how frequent the individual combinations of these segments are. An overall probability for the entire syllable can then be derived by multiplying the frequency of every segment.
As can be seen from table 1, the probability of a CVC syllable is 46.26% in northern Germany (that is, .85 × .82 × .79 × .84 = .4626), which is considerably higher than in either southwestern or southeastern Germany. This is to be expected in light of figure 3B. What table 1 does indicate is where transitions between the segments are similar and where they are different. The difference in CVC frequency is thus specified not as the frequency of an individual syllable, but as a difference in possible segment combinations inherent in the dialects. Since these segmental combinations also constitute part of the structure of other syllable types (for example, the VC type), table 1 provides a more refined picture of the possible structure of monosyllables in dialects.
For a better overview of the possible combinations, diagrams of transition probabilities were modeled. This is where Markov chains (which can be understood as finite state automata) become relevant: They provide information on the probability of one particular state becoming another. In the present case, this is the probability P for the transition from one segment i of the CV representation to another one j within the same syllable, taking into account the actual initialization and closure (α and ε) of the syllable (⇒ Pij, with i,j ∈ {α,C,V,ε}).Footnote 14 The actual models obtained from the transition matrix underlying table 1 are specified in figure 7, where Markov chains represent the transition probability P of segments.
The models in figure 7 show sequences of possible C and V states where the probability of the next state depends on the current state. Note that C1 and C2 do not refer to particular sounds but to the positions (or states) where consonants might occur. In the case of C clusters, these positions are filled repeatedly. Accordingly, V represents the position of the nucleus. Since the corpus covers only monosyllables, the condition applies that only one V per chain is possible. Start (α) initializes the left syllable edge, and end (ε) closes the right syllable edge. The models can be read as follows: The probability P of a monosyllable starting with C is P αC1=85% in the northern model, PαC1=84% in the southwestern model, and PαC1=85% in the southeastern model, while the probability of V being the first segment in the word is PαV=15% to 16% to 15%. According to the highest values in the models shown in figure 7, the most likely sequence in all three regions is CVC, which concurs with figure 3A and table 1.
Figure 7 and table 1 demonstrate an almost identical probability of αC transition across all three regions (PαC1=84%–85%). This is consistent with Kohler’s (1995:226) statistics for monosyllables in the German standard language, where almost all monosyllables start with C. At the same time, these results show that dialectal monosyllables tend to comply with the Head Law (Vennemann Reference Vennemann1988:13f.), according to which (additional prosodic characteristics aside) a “syllable head is the more preferred […] the closer the number of speech sounds in the head is to one” (see also figure 3A).
However, most interesting is the replication rate of C states, which is the probability that one C is followed by another. Two findings need to be highlighted. First, replication of C states is much higher in the southern models than in the northern model. This holds for both onsets (Southwest P C1C1=23% and Southeast 26% versus North P C1C1=18%) and codas (P C2C2=32% and 33% versus P C2C2=16%). Second, the same numbers indicate a clear difference between onsets and codas in the southern models but not in the northern model. This difference corresponds with lower values for CV transitions (P C1V=North 82%; Southwest 77%; Southeast 74%) and C transitions in the South (PC2ε=North 84%; Southwest 68%; Southeast 67%). In other words, as in Standard German, the coda is only the preferred domain of complex C clusters in the southern dialects. The conflict with Vennemann’s Coda Law (1988:21) mentioned for Standard German (see example 2) also holds for the South of the language area but not for the North. Regarding the V-only syllables, which were more commonly found in southern Germany, no relevant influence on the southwestern model is obvious.
Taken together, the models thus illustrate important differences in the clustering of consonants, which leads to the question on the prosodic structuring of syllables. For example, are the segments sequenced depending on their degree of sonority and in accordance with the Sonority Sequencing Generalization, which states that in any syllable “there is a segment constituting a sonority peak that is preceded and/or followed by a sequence of segments with progressively decreasing sonority values” (Selkirk Reference Selkirk, Mark Aronoff, Oehrle and Stephens1984:116)? An answer is provided in the following section.
5.4. Sonority Dispersion
In order to identify the possible regional distribution of syllables based on their prosodic properties, an analysis of SONORITY DISPERSION (Clements Reference Clements, Kingston and Mary1990:302–311) was performed. Sonority dispersion is another way to describe syllable complexity, based on the organization of segments within the syllable according to their degree of sonority. In essence, the aim of a sonority dispersion analysis is to identify sonority differences between adjacent segments in a syllable. To this end, syllables are divided into demisyllables: onset + nucleus versus rhyme (that is, coda + nucleus), with V as the segment shared by the initial and the final demisyllable). Thus, each demisyllable is a sequence of the form Cm…CnV or VCm…Cn, with n ≥ m ≥ 0 (Clements Reference Clements, Kingston and Mary1990:303). Based on this configuration, sonority dispersion D is calculated as follows:
In 11, d is the difference between the sonority ranks of every adjacent and nonadjacent pair of segments and i, within each demisyllable, following the sonority scale V > G > L > N > O. This sonority scale represents the ranks 4 > 3 > 2 > 1 > 0, while m refers to the number of pairs in the demisyllable.Footnote 15 For example, the dialect word [mɛlk] ‘milk’ in 8e has the initial demisyllable (ID) [mɛ] and the final demisyllable (FD) [ɛlk], which form the pairs ID={mɛ} and FD={ɛl,ɛk,lk}. Based on these pairings, sonority dispersion is calculated in 12.
Further examples provided by Clements (Reference Clements, Kingston and Mary1990:304) are GV, VG=1.0 or OV, VO=.06 for CV/VC demisyllables and OLV, VLO=.56 or LGV, VGL=2.25 for CCV/VCC demisyllables. From these examples it becomes evident that segment pairs that follow more closely the aforementioned sonority scale have higher D scores, while pairs that skip segments on the sonority scale (for example, VO lacks G, L, N) have lower D scores. At the same time, the examples illustrate that D takes into account not so much the violation of the Sonority Sequencing Generalization but the (non)adjacency of the segments within the defined sonority scale.
In order to evaluate the PAD data, the monosyllables from PAD were represented as VGLNO-sequences and then divided into demisyllables on which the D measure was performed using R programming (V-only demisyllables were excluded). For every location j the mean sonority dispersion Dj was calculated, which leaves one with a total of 182 average Dj measures per demisyllable.
According to Clements’s (1990:304) crosslinguistic observations, the preferred initial demisyllable (onset + nucleus) tends toward lower D, while the preferred final demisyllable (rhyme) tends toward higher D (the so-called Dispersion Principle):
We observe that initial demisyllables with low values for D are those that show an optimal sonority profile, i.e., a sharp and steady rise in sonority, while in the case of final demisyllables, those with high values for D show the best profile, i.e., a gradual drop in sonority.
Thus, according to Clements, the preferred profile of initial demi-syllables is a profile consisting of segments that are not next to each other (that is, nonadjacent) on the sonority scale, such as OV in contrast to GV. Such a profile is expressed by lower D values. However, in final demisyllables, the preferred profile is a profile consisting of segments that are next to each other on the sonority scale (adjacent=gradual drop), such as VGL in contrast to VLO. This preferred profile is expressed by higher D values.
This observation, which corresponds reasonably to both the Head Law and the Coda Law (Vennemann Reference Vennemann1988), was confirmed by an evaluation of the PAD data. On average, sonority dispersion was found to be D̅ID=.148 in initial demisyllables and D̅FD=.193 in final demisyllables. This difference was confirmed by a t-test in statistical terms (t(23,781)=-13.02, p<.001). The values obtained were noticeably rather low, indicating a predominance of CV (79% of which=OV, 13%=NV) and VC demisyllables (65%=VO, 25%=VN), ranging between D=.06 and D=1. It must be assumed that this is not least due to the dominance of CVC syllables. An overview of the most frequent types of both initial and final demisyllables is provided in table 2.
In order to test for regional differences, average D was calculated for every location documented in the corpus and plotted in figure 8. The upper panel (figure 8A,B) shows the mean scores for sonority dispersion per site. For both conditions, initial and final demisyllables, a Moran’s I test revealed significant spatial autocorrelation (I ID=0.172, I E=-0.005, p<.001; I FD=0.294, I E=-0.005, p<.001).Footnote 16 That is, significant regional differences in D values were found to be present. The bottom panel (figure 8C,D) reports the results of a test for spatial association following Getis & Ord 1992, 1996.Footnote 17 The locations identified here are those that statistically i) have particularly high (brown color) or low (green color) D scores, and ii) are adjacent to locations with similar scores. However, because the values are so small, they should only be considered indicative of a tendency.
In diagrams A and B, D is shown for initial and final demisyllables (onset + nucleus versus rhyme) and white color indicates the average of the distribution. Diagrams C and D show Gi* scores of the D distribution for both initial and final demisyllables. Note that D scores in C and D have been z-transformed, which means that the mean of the distribution is zero and the standard deviation is z=1.
In regard to initial demisyllables, diagram C shows a concentration of very low D values (reported as z-transformed values) in the center of the language area (Hessian dialects, southern Thuringian). This area coincides accurately with the southern region, which shows higher CVC frequency (figure 3B). This region tends to have a sharper and steadier rise in sonority (D̅∼.12) than other regions. This is not least due to both more OV onsets (D=.06) and the fact that processes leading to the clustering of Cs, such as schwa syncopation (as in 8h) or the High German consonant shift (as in GERM /p/, /t/, /k/ > OHG /pf/, /ts/, /kx/), did not occur or only partially took place in this region.
In contrast, rather high values (D̅∼.19) occur in the southeastern and the northwestern region, indicating that in these regions, adjacent segments tend to gradually drop in sonority. This is mainly because of the GV pattern (D=1), as in 13a–c, which is due to the retention of original [w] as [w] and the spirantization of /g/.
In regard to the final demisyllable, figure 8B shows fairly low D values in the North and higher D values in the South. Figure 8D indicates that it is specifically the southeastern area where the highest D values occur (D̅∼.3). While this region has already been identified as the region with most CCC+ clusters (figure 5B), higher D values indicate, in addition, that the segments within these clusters are more closely arranged on the sonority scale (see 13d), because C clustering alone would have no influence on the D measure (CC=.0). Furthermore, it is remarkable that in figure 8D the zone with lower DFD scores (D̅ ∼.15) coincides with the zone with lower D ID scores, indicating that in this region, not only more OV onsets but also more VO codas occur, further evidenced by the high number of CVC syllables.
6. Modeling Monosyllables in German Dialects
Finally, in order to obtain a comprehensive answer to the question of the phonotactic structuring of German dialects, the approach described in section 5.3 was used: A total of 55 locations were identified across five different regions, with 11 locations per region (L=55∼30% of PAD locations). In each region, Markov chains for both initial and final demisyllables were calculated, as shown in figure 9. It represents the transition probabilities P for each pair of adjacent segments in both the initial and final demisyllables (probabilities P<5% are not reported). Red arrows indicate high transition probability and blue arrows indicate low transition probability. Location subsets within regions A, D, and E are the same as in the previous analysis (figure 6). B represents the more western region, with a lower occurrence of monosyllables (figure 1), and C is a location subset within the central region, with lower D values in the onset (figure 8A,C).
With respect to the initial demisyllable, in the region A model, a syllable most likely starts with an obstruent (P αO=72%), followed by a vowel (P OV=76%) and another obstruent (P VO=45%). This shows that the coda is the more diverse subsyllabic unit (for example, onset: P OL=14%, coda: P LO=44%; onset: P OG=< 5%, coda: P GO=22%). In regard to the filling of onset and coda, one is more likely to find an open coda (P Vɛ=21%) than an open onset (P αV=15%), which is in line with more general expectations. The same tendency holds for all other regions. However, there is a clear decrease in POV transition from North to South (A: P OV=76%; B: 75%; C: 75%; D: 69%; E: 66%), indicating more complex O clusters, which is confirmed by the increase of P OO (A: P OO=07%; B: 07%; C: 12%; D: 15%; E: 17%).
In relation to the final demisyllable, the situation is rather different. In regard to O clustering, first, there is an increase from North to South (A: P OO = 07%; B: 16%; C: 19%; D: 20%; E: 24%). Second, while P OO is identical in the initial and the final demisyllable, it is much higher in the models of regions B–E. Since the northern area was selected as being representative of the CVC region, this is the expected result.
It is noteworthy that virtually all of the few instances of N, L, G in the first demisyllable across the regions are directly before V (PN/L/GV=.80–1.0). This is also the case for O, but there are at least instances of OL clustering (as in [ɡløːv] ‘believe\1SG.PRS’ in Barßel/North Low German). However, in the final demisyllable, neighboring along the sonority scale is more evident (for example, LN, NO, such as …VLN: [kfoln] ‘PTCP-fallen-CIRC’ in Treffelstein/North Bavarian or …VNO: [gɛns ‘goose.PL’ in Borstendorf/Upper Saxon). As the sonority contrast between C and V is thus sharper in the initial demisyllable, this finding explains the difference between lower and higher D values in figure 8.
Most striking is the fact that among the segment combinations, another increase is evident from North to South, namely, that of NO in the final demisyllable (as in example 8j; A: P NO=< 05%; B: 27%; C: 38%; D: 52%; E: 40%), which contributes to higher D values in the South. Interestingly, this transition is observed in the Alemannic/Swabian region (D in figure 9), and the values are higher here than in the Bavarian region (E in figure 9). In contrast, there are higher values for P Lε and P OO in southeastern Germany, which leads to slightly sharper contrasts in Bavaria. This difference is also evident in figure 8D, where in the eastern region of southeastern Germany D values occur around the mean or even in the first quartile.
More generally, figure 9 demonstrates that on this level of phonological representation, almost no violation of the Sonority Sequencing Generalization is in evidence above the 5% cut. Certainly, a more fine-graded scale differentiating, for instance, stops and fricatives, would reveal a more detailed picture. However, the OLGV representation adopted in this study obviously enables very clear modeling of the given data. The only exceptions are GL transitions in the initial demisyllables in the region A model (P GL=20%), which are due to lenition from [g] to [j], as in 14, which is well known in Low German dialects (Simmler Reference Simmler, Besch, Knoop, Putschke and Wiegand1983).
From a more theoretical perspective, however, this exception can be eliminated if one interprets these /j/ sounds not as glides, but as fricatives (see Hall Reference Hall2014:334 for similar considerations with respect to Westphalian glides). Since fricatives are obstruents, the OLGV order is not violated.
7. Discussion
7.1. Syllable Structure and Regional Patterns
Prior work modeled sound sequencing in syllables of Standard German. Based on previous research, Maddieson (Reference Maddieson, Dryer and Haspelmath2013) defines German as being among the languages with the highest degree of syllable complexity in terms of C clustering in onset and coda. Expanding the focus on language variation, the present study contributes to a more differentiated view. Hypotheses i)–iii) outlined in section 2 have been confirmed: There have been found significant differences in the architecture of syllables between German dialects and Standard German; furthermore, regional differences in the structuring of monosyllables in German dialects have been identified, and these differences mainly concern the complexity of C clusters in both onset and coda.
In regard to Maddieson’s (2013) model, three more general findings can be noted. First, there are C clusters in Standard German which are not captured by the (C3)V(C4) model that Maddieson proposes, such as in 2a,b. At the same time, the model does capture the dialects represented in the PAD corpus, where C clusters are more strictly in line with Maddieson’s model, as in the following examples, which show the maximum complexity for onset (15a) and coda (15b) in the PAD.
Second, considering that the most common syllable type in the PAD corpus is CVC (figure 3A), it is also evident that the examples in 15 contain rather uncommon C clusters for the dialects considered above. Even though it is likely that more data would reveal more complex sequences, it is also likely that those monosyllables will not be prevalent in all regions. The same generalization holds for Standard German, where the frequently discussed examples, such as those in 2, are exceptions, as Kohler (Reference Kohler1995:226) states (see also Menzerath Reference Menzerath1954).
Third, contrary to Maddieson’s (2013) implicit assumption of relative linguistic uniformity, on a micro-typological level German exhibits very clear regional differences. A more general finding concerns preferences for monosyllables: Some dialect areas prefer monosyllable realizations, whereas others do not (figure 1). As has been shown, this preference for monosyllables is presumably connected to both schwa apocopation and ge- deletion. The southern boundary of monosyllabic infrequency, in particular, represents a breaking point in the dialect continuum, as becomes evident from the distribution of the (C)V(C) and (C3)V(C3+) pattern (figure 4/A/C and figure 5). This may be because both these historical processes led to syllable reduction, which in turn affected C clustering. An example of the boundary based on a larger quantity of data is provided in the language atlas of the German Empire. On the map with the lemma “Affe” (‘ape’)—which incidentally is not represented in the PAD corpus—the region where schwa apocopation did not take place coincides almost perfectly with the region where monosyllables are less frequent in figure 1.Footnote 18
These general findings lead to a more specific result, namely, the North–South divide in terms of syllable complexity. This result has been substantiated not in the least by the probabilities of segment transition. Focusing on individual phenomena, such as schwa apocopation, the analysis has revealed a clear (categorial) separation between North and South. In addition, the models that were developed based on the stochastic application of Markov chains reveal a continuous increase in obstruent clusters in both onset and coda from North to South. The most prominent areas are the northern coastal region (that is, northern Low German and Pomeranian, see Appendix) and the southwestern region (that is, Alemannic, including Swabian) together with the southeastern region (that is, parts of Bavarian).
It must be noted that these regions represent larger areas with similar phonotactic characteristics. Specifically, the pattern of the Bavarian region seems to reach Upper Saxon, as figure 5B indicates. However, altogether, these regions define the hot spots of the North–South divide, where the northern dialects tend toward more simple syllable structures and the southern dialects toward more complex clusters. Consequently, with regard to Maddieson’s definition of syllable structure, it is assumed that northern and southern dialects could represent two different types, among which the syllable structure of Low German dialects is “moderately complex” and the syllable structure of High German dialects is “complex” (Maddieson Reference Maddieson, Dryer and Haspelmath2013).
If the zone between (C)V(C) and (C3)V(C3+) (see figure 4A/C) is assumed to be the border zone between the North and the South region, it must be stated that this geographical division does not correspond to traditional dialect classification. However, in part this finding is in line with more recent studies on the connection between speech rate and regional varieties (Hahn & Siebenhaar Reference Hahn, Siebenhaar and Jokisch2016, Siebenhaar & Hahn Reference Siebenhaar, Hahn, Calhoun, Escudero, Tabain and Warren2019). These studies also find a limited correspondence between regional borders and traditional dialect classification.Footnote 19 As has been shown, it is the deletion of schwa and other schwa-related phenomena (such as ge-deletion) that have a huge impact on the architecture of phonological systems and are thus a major issue in the regional differentiation of both lexical phonology and syllable phonology (Werner Reference Werner, Birnbaum, Durovic, Jacob, Nilsson, Sjoeberg and Worth1978, Birkenes 2014).
As a rough synopsis of the analyses, three syllable types can be highlighted: CVC, V, and (C3)V(C3+). With respect to the latter, its connection with the deletion of schwa and other schwa-related phenomena has already been established. It has also been demonstrated that with regard to coda clusters, the Bavarian language in the western region—more precisely, the contact area between Bavarian and Swabian Bavarian—primarily exhibits the most frequent occurrences of the (C3)V(C3+) type. Equally, it is in this region where the ordering of segments in the coda follows the sonority scale most consistently (expressed as a measure of sonority dispersion, figure 8B,D). This leads to the conclusion that with increasing C clustering, mechanisms that order segments according to their sonority are at work. At the same time, regional characteristics can be observed, such as the more frequent NO transitions in southwestern Germany as opposed to a sharper sonority contrast between the segments in southeastern Germany (see the Markov chains for demisyllables in figure 8D,E). In all cases, however, the Sonority Sequencing Generalization seems to apply. Exceptions, such as a putative GLV sequence in 14, resolve when the glides in question are considered as fricatives.
V-only syllables, by contrast, occur almost exclusively in the southwestern region. As they occur mainly in weak forms, such as pronouns or prepositions, further studies could investigate whether these forms have a specific function in higher level prosodic categories, such as intonational or phonological phrases. In any case, the V-only syllables contribute to the fact that the southwestern region seems to be a region of phonotactic particularities, as has also been demonstrated in other studies (see among others Nübling & Schrambke Reference Nübling, Schrambke and Glaser2004, Caro Reina Reference Caro Reina2019).
In regard to the distribution of CVC syllables, the situation is different. Even though this syllable pattern occurs in all regions, it is particularly common in northern Germany. Furthermore, as figure 3B shows, it is less frequent in those regions where schwa deletion did not take place. One possible explanation for this could be that forms with historical schwa apocopation may be realized as monosyllables, whereas forms where schwa was not deleted (or reinserted) are realized as polysyllabic, as shown in 16 (see section 3.2 for the handling of V).
From this perspective, that CVC syllables tend to occur in the absence of schwa apocopation is in line with the hypothesis of different syllable structures inside and outside the region of schwa apocopation (see Lameli Reference Lameli, Nevaci, Floarea and Farcaş2021). However, the stark difference between North and South in terms of C clustering suggests that there must be other factors influencing the particular architecture of syllables in different regions. In this regard, it should be considered that Low German schwa apocopation is a more recent process (documented around the second half of the 16th century; see Foerste Reference Foerste and Stammler1966) than High German apocopation (documented around the middle of the 13th century; see Lindgren Reference Lindgren1953), so that the much higher frequency of CVC in the coastal region is due to a different, albeit also schwa-related development. Furthermore, as this region forms part of the linguistic continuum between the Low German-speaking area and northern Europe (see Höder Reference Höder2016), further studies should investigate the extent to which this pattern is related to the regional characteristics of the syllable structures of neighboring languages, such as Danish.
In addition, 16c exemplifies morphological conditions that were not addressed in this study: It represents a relic of weak inflection, typical of Bavarian, where the Early New High German -en ending (NOM.SG.F.) underwent schwa syncopation in more recent times. As a result, /n/ became the new—now consonantal—nucleus. The phonology– morphology interface has been subject of recent studies on Standard German (Bergmann Reference Bergmann2018 among others). Examples such as 16c suggest that it would be worthwhile extending this research to German dialects (see also Birkenes 2014). It thus becomes evident that the phonotactics of dialects contributes not only to a more comprehensive description of language geography but also to a more comprehensive understanding of German as a variative language.
7.2. Methodological Reflection
With its quantifying approach to regional phonotactics, the present study has entered a field that has been underexplored so far. Different approaches, such as the measure of sonority dispersion and the modeling of Markov chains, were adopted for the analysis in order to gain different perspectives on the subject. It goes without saying that each of these approaches is able to yield only an approximation to the phonotactic structure of dialects, as the results depend on a particular model, which, in turn, can be questioned. The representativeness of data is a crucial point. The analysis focuses on the phonotactic structure of a sample of 201 monosyllabic words (realized as 13,492 tokens) representing different parts of speech. In total, approximately half of the Wenker sentences were studied. The question is whether the results can be considered to be generally representative. To answer this question the potential phonotactic possibilities in dialects needs to be clarified. However, since no comparative studies are available in this area, such an answer is hardly possible. An alternative would be to examine a selection of different phonological processes known from the standard language; but such an approach would very likely give rise to problems associated with a lack of regional balancing, as Standard German is affected by different dialects, especially by East Middle German dialect variants.
Against this background, an explorative approach based on the PAD data seemed to be a good starting point in order to construct a big picture of the phonotactics across regions in the first place. More importantly, the words analyzed in this study are representative of the Wenker sentences. This is not a small claim, considering that knowledge about the dialects in Germany is substantially shaped by translations of the Wenker sentences. Thus, this material serves as common ground between this study and previous research. For example, the fact that the phonological processes discussed by Hall (Reference Hall2020) are documented in the PAD corpus points to the fundamental suitability of the PAD data for phonotactic analysis. At the same time, it is also clear that some prosodic phenomena (such as tonal accents) are underrepresented in the PAD. It would therefore be important to further expand the corpus in the future.
Furthermore, the use of Markov chains must be addressed. In the present study, First Order Markov Chains were used to develop a memoryless transitional model consisting of known states (for example, C, V, start, and end). There are certainly more powerful techniques, such as Hidden Markov Models, which are also suited for modeling latent effects related to unknown states (see, among others, Tjong-Kim-Sang Reference Tjong-Kim-Sang1998). These techniques could be used in future work when more information on the underlying phonological systems is available. For the present exploratory purposes, however, the chosen method was not only appropriate but also very powerful.
In regard to the use of sonority dispersion, this approach has provided a new perspective on regional phonotactics. In the present study, Clements’s (1990) simplified sonority scale was used. This scale is advantageous in an exploratory study because it allows to effectively capture rough structures. By contrast, a more detailed sonority scale would be advantageous, for example, to obtain a more fine-grained picture of the southern regions, where more C clusters clearly occur. This would be a worthwhile task for future work.
8. Conclusion
The primary goal of this article was to explore the areal variation of syllable structure in German dialects, in order to take a step toward a more comprehensive view of areal phonotactics. To this end, 13,492 tokens of monosyllables across 182 locations were analyzed. By focusing on the structure of monosyllables, new insights into the phonological micro-variation of German were gained. In particular, new information on dialectal variation came to light, which concerns the structure of syllables as well as specific arrangements of individual segments, both of which vary between northern and southern Germany. The study reveals that monosyllables are quite frequent in both the North and the South. The most typical monosyllable in northern German dialects is CVC, while the dialects in southern Germany tend toward more complex monosyllables, with obstruent clusters. An analysis of sonority dispersion yields two major findings: On the one hand, final demisyllables in southern German tend to be made up of segments more closely arranged on the sonority scale. On the other hand, initial demisyllables in middle German are often comprised of segments that are far apart on the sonority scale. This study thus constitutes a good starting point for further crossregional, in-depth analysis of micro-variation in syllable structure.
APPENDIX