Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T04:10:03.422Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Singular Agreement in Special Partitive Constructions in Dutch

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 February 2019

Lotte Hogeweg*
Affiliation:
Radboud University Nijmegen
Stefanie Ramachers*
Affiliation:
Radboud University Nijmegen
Helen de Hoop*
Affiliation:
Radboud University Nijmegen
*
Radboud University Nijmegen, Centre for Language Studies, Postbus 9103, 6500 HD Nijmegen, Netherlands [[email protected]] [[email protected]] [[email protected]]
Radboud University Nijmegen, Centre for Language Studies, Postbus 9103, 6500 HD Nijmegen, Netherlands [[email protected]] [[email protected]] [[email protected]]
Radboud University Nijmegen, Centre for Language Studies, Postbus 9103, 6500 HD Nijmegen, Netherlands [[email protected]] [[email protected]] [[email protected]]

Abstract

Dutch partitive constructions of the type one of the (few) X who Y show a striking pattern of singular subject-verb agreement in their relative clause. This paper presents a corpus study showing that the prescriptively “incorrect” singular agreement pattern is in fact the dominant pattern in Dutch. In order to explain this, we argue that this type of partitive construction often has a specific function in context, namely, to point out that the subject is special or extraordinary, usually for the reason presented by the relative clause. We apply a usage-based approach to this construction within the framework of Construction Grammar, arguing that the prevalent implicature of the subject’s specialness has become a conventionalized part of the meaning of the construction. This analysis then can be used to explain the syntactic pattern of singular agreement within the relative clause. A similar albeit less pronounced pattern can be found in German.*

Type
Articles
Copyright
© Society for Germanic Linguistics 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

We are most grateful to Roeland van Hout for his help with the statistical analyses of the data, and to Lukas Reinarz, who collected and annotated the German data. We would like to thank Sebastian Collin, Geertje van Bergen, Ad Foolen, Johan Kobben, Sander Lestrade, Vera van Mulken, Kees de Schepper, Wessel Stoop, Peter de Swart, Thijs Trompenaars, Ruti Vardi, Puck Wildschut, and Martine Zwets for helpful discussions and comments. A special word of thanks goes to Johan Kobben and Vera van Mulken for translating the Dutch fragments. Versions of this paper were presented at the conferences Structural alternations: Speaker and hearer perspectives (Groningen, August 2011), TIN-dag 2012 (Utrecht, February 2012), and New ways of analyzing syntactic variation (Nijmegen, November 2012). We thank the audiences for their questions and comments. Finally, we thank two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier version of this paper.

References

DATA SOURCES

Abrahams, Frits. Geboren columnist [Native columnist]. NRC Handelsblad, April 5, 2012.Google Scholar
Algemene Nederlandse Spraakkunst. 1997, ed. by Walter Haeseryn, Kirstin Romijn, Guido Geerts, Jaap de Rooij, & Maarten C. van den Toorn. Groningen: Martinus Nijhoff. Available at http://ans.ruhosting.nl.Google Scholar
CGN (Corpus Gesproken Nederlands [Corpus of Spoken Dutch]). Available at http://lands.let.ru.nl/cgn/.Google Scholar
Chip forum. Available at http://forum.chip.de/.Google Scholar
de Volkskrant. Available at https://www.volkskrant.nl/Google Scholar
Fitness com: The global fitness community. Available at http://de.fitness.com.Google Scholar
Fok! Available at forum.fok.nl.Google Scholar
Gutefrage. Available at www.gutefrage.net.Google Scholar
Studis Online: Die schlauen Seiten rund ums Studium [Studis Online: The smart pages for studying]. Available at www.studis-online.de.Google Scholar
WordReference. Available at www.wordreference.com.Google Scholar

REFERENCES

Bergen, Geertje van. 2011. Who’s first and what’s next. Animacy and word order variation in Dutch language production. Nijmegen, the Netherlands: Radboud University Nijmegen dissertation.Google Scholar
Bock, Kathryn, & Miller, Carol A.. 1991. Broken agreement. Cognitive Psychology 23. 4593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gessel, Han van, Hulsbosch, Jan Kees, Huurdeman, Henk, Kleef, Bas van, Los, Kees, & Vuijsje, Bert. 1992. De Volkskrant stijlboek. Den Haag: Sdu Uitgeverij.Google Scholar
Goldberg, Adele E. 2003. Constructions: A new theoretical approach to language. TRENDS in Cognitive Science 7. 219224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, Adele E. 2006. Constructions at work. The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Grice, H. Paul. 1975. Logic and conversation. Speech acts, ed. by Cole, Peter & Morgan, Jerry L., 4158. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Grondelaers, Stefan, & Roeland, van Hout. 2011. The standard language situation in the low countries: Top-down and bottom-up variations on a diaglossic theme. Journal of Germanic Linguistics 23. 199243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herring, Susan C. 2010. Computer-mediated conversation: Introduction and overview. Language@Internet 7, article 2.Google Scholar
Hinrichs, Lars. 2016. Modular repertoires in English-using social networks: A study of language choice in the networks of adult Facebook users. English in computer-mediated communication. Variation, representation, and change, ed. by Lauren, Squires, 1742. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Hogeweg, Lotte. 2009. Word in process. On the interpretation, acquisition, and production of words. Ph.D. dissertation, Radboud University Nijmegen/ Utrecht: LOT publications.Google Scholar
Hogeweg, Lotte, Ramachers, Stefanie, & Wottrich, Verena. 2011. Doch, toch and wel on the table. Linguistics in the Netherlands 2011, ed. by Rick, Nouwen & Elenbaas, Marion, 5060. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Kilgarriff, Adam. 2007. Googleology is bad science. Computational Linguistics 33. 147151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladusaw, William. 1982. Semantic constraints on the English partitive construction. Proceedings of the First West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, ed. by Flickinger, Daniel P., Macken, Marlys, & Wiegand, Nancy, 231242. Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Pearlmutter, Neal J., Garnsey, Susan M., & Bock, Kathryn. 1999. Agreement processes in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language 41. 427456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reed, Ann. 1996. Partitives, existentials, and partitive determiners. Partitives. Studies on the syntax and semantics of partitive and related constructions, ed. by Hoeksema, Jacob, 143178. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Sanford, Anthony J., Moxey, Linda M., & Paterson, Kevin. 1994. Psychological studies of quantifiers. Journal of Semantics 10. 153170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C. 2007. The concepts of constructional mismatch and type-shifting from the perspective of grammaticalization. Cognitive Linguistics 18. 523557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veenstra, Alma. 2014. Semantic and syntactic constraints on the production of subject-verb agreement. Nijmegen, the Netherlands: Radboud University Nijmegen dissertation.Google Scholar
Verhagen, Arie. 2002. From parts to wholes and back again. Cognitive Linguistics 13. 403439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar