Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-21T18:57:52.810Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Are Goodwill Ambassadors Good for Business? The Impact of Celebrities on International Organization Fundraising

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 August 2024

Rabia Malik*
Affiliation:
University of Essex, Colchester, UK
Svanhildur Thorvaldsdottir
Affiliation:
University of Iceland, Reykjavik, Iceland
*
Corresponding author: Rabia Malik; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Many international organizations (IOs) rely on voluntary contributions from member states and private actors to fund their operations. Donations from individuals are a significant and increasing income source for these IOs, who rely on marketing strategies such as celebrity endorsement, in the form of Goodwill Ambassadors, to help raise funds. Little is known, however, about the effectiveness of this strategy in the context of IOs although intuition from literatures in marketing and psychology suggests that celebrity endorsement should be effective. We conduct a survey experiment to investigate the effectiveness of Goodwill Ambassadors and, contrary to expectations, find no average effect of celebrity endorsement on donations to, and interest in, IOs and only limited effects among certain sub-groups. We speculate that the context of IOs makes it harder to generate the type of connection between celebrity and cause necessary to make endorsement effective and suggest that further investigation is needed.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of American Political Science Association

Introduction

United Nations (UN) agencies rely almost exclusively on voluntary contributions to fund global development and humanitarian operations. For some, a significant part of these contributions comes from individuals, whose collective contributions can add up to hundreds of millions of dollars annually. Footnote 1 One way in which many international organizations (IOs) raise awareness and funds is by recruiting celebrities – so-called Goodwill Ambassadors – who volunteer their time and effort to raise the agency’s profile. Footnote 2 In many ways, Goodwill Ambassadors are comparable to brand or product endorsers who appear on behalf of the organization or talk about it on social media, using their name recognition to promote the organization to the general population.

Despite the popularity of this fundraising strategy among many IOs, we know little about its effectiveness. Although many factors identified in scholarship on celebrity endorsements might hold (cf. Bergkvist and Zhou, Reference Bergkvist and Zhou2016; Erdogan, Reference Erdogan1999; Halder, Pradhan and Roy Chaudhuri, Reference Halder, Pradhan and Roy Chaudhuri2021), there are contextual differences that make celebrity endorsement of IOs important to examine specifically. Unlike product endorsement, donors do not receive products in exchange for their “purchase,” but rather the knowledge of having contributed to a worthy cause. Additionally, IOs are further removed from donors’ daily lives than domestic charitable organizations, making donors unlikely to personally see the effectiveness of their donations.

We conduct what we believe is the first survey experiment to directly test the effect of celebrity endorsement on donations to, and interest in, IOs. We randomly assigned respondents to read a brief description of UNICEF’s work on one of two issues, highlighted by either a celebrity endorser or non-celebrity expert. Respondents then answered questions about their willingness to donate and interest in UNICEF. Counter to much of the extant literature and our pre-registered expectations, we find little support for the effectiveness of celebrity endorsement. We find no significant average treatment effect (ATE) (substantively or statistically) of Goodwill Ambassador endorsement; however, we do find that donors sharing an ethnicity with the endorser donate more in the treatment condition and that although men donate, on average, significantly less than women, treatment renders that difference insignificant. We speculate that, in the case of IOs, celebrities are less likely to have a personal connection to the causes they promote, weakening the tie between endorser and brand. We further conjecture that any “warm-glow” feeling that people get from donating to a charity (Andreoni, Reference Andreoni1989) may not be enhanced by celebrity endorsement. These findings suggest that IOs should carefully evaluate the potential impact of celebrity endorsers and closely consider which populations they seek to influence.

Celebrity endorsement and IOs

Psychology and communications scholars have long studied sources of celebrity influence (Giles, Reference Giles2002; Brown, Reference Brown2018). This literature argues that people can form so-called parasocial relationships with celebrities, where individuals develop a pseudo-relationship with a celebrity through media consumption. These relationships can evolve into celebrity identification, where individuals start to see celebrities as role models for their own behavior (Brown, Reference Brown2018), and it is generally through this identification mechanism that celebrities are expected to be influential. In fact, IOs recruit celebrities as Goodwill Ambassadors, in part, because of their perceived ability to influence their followers and act as role models, thus raising both awareness and funds for the organization. Footnote 3

Research in psychology and marketing suggests additional mechanisms of influence, such as the “match-up” hypothesis (Kamins, Reference Kamins1990), where an endorsement is only effective if a stimulus matches the endorser well (e.g., attractive celebrities endorsing beauty-enhancing products), and associative learning (Till, Reference Till1998), where repeated exposure to celebrity and product together creates an association between the two in the minds of individuals. Consequently, celebrity status can be a persuasive force on consumers (Kumru and Vesterlund, Reference Kumru and Vesterlund2010).

Factors that make celebrity endorsement most effective can largely be split into two groups: features of the celebrities themselves (source effects) and features of the connection between celebrity and brand (fit effects) (Bergkvist and Zhou, Reference Bergkvist and Zhou2016). Source effects include credibility, attractiveness, and likeability. Fit effects focus on the match between celebrity and brand, either as a main effect or a moderator of source effects (Bergkvist and Zhou, Reference Bergkvist and Zhou2016) and can also refer to fit between celebrity and consumer, such as shared ethnicity (Deshpande and Stayman, Reference Deshpande and Stayman1994).

Being perceived as a credible sponsor of a brand (a good fit) is perhaps of paramount importance (cf. Halder, Pradhan and Roy Chaudhuri, Reference Halder, Pradhan and Roy Chaudhuri2021; Ohanian, Reference Ohanian1991; Wheeler, Reference Wheeler2009; Park, Reference Park2017; Ilicic and Webster, Reference Ilicic and Webster2013). For example, Ilicic and Baxter (Reference Ilicic and Baxter2014) note that the late actor Christopher Reeve’s collaboration with the American Paralysis Association following his horse-riding accident led to a doubling of the association’s revenue within a few years and Wheeler (Reference Wheeler2009) finds that Michael J. Fox, who suffers from Parkinson’s, outperforms Harrison Ford as an endorser of a (fictitious) Parkinson’s disease-related organization.

Celebrity endorsement is also commonly used by non-profit organizations to promote charitable giving (Wheeler, Reference Wheeler2009), although celebrity effectiveness in charity advertisement has received less scholarly attention (Ilicic and Baxter, Reference Ilicic and Baxter2014). Important contextual differences exist between endorsement for charities and product branding (Wymer and Drollinger, Reference Wymer and Drollinger2015); e.g., people donate to charities for various reasons, such as awareness of need, altruism, values, a “warm-glow” effect, and simply being asked (c.f. Bekkers and Wiepking, Reference Bekkers and Wiepking2011; Andreoni, Reference Andreoni1989). A celebrity endorser of a charity must persuade potential donors that the charity is important and needs help and that their contribution can make a difference (Wymer and Drollinger, Reference Wymer and Drollinger2015). Therefore, a celebrity’s ability to credibly advertise the cause – e.g., through a personal connection or experience, or continued engagement with the charity – is likely to matter more. Thus, it is notable that much of the extant experimental literature uses either fictional celebrities (Park, Reference Park2017) or fictional charities (Wheeler, Reference Wheeler2009; Wymer and Drollinger, Reference Wymer and Drollinger2015), making it harder to directly test fit perceptions. Exceptions are Karlan and List (Reference Karlan and List2012) and Ilicic and Baxter (Reference Ilicic and Baxter2014), but both use little-known nonprofits.

By contrast, we use a well-known IO, UNICEF, which is, in many ways, generally representative of UN agencies in terms of both its funding structure and its use of celebrity endorsements. Operational activities of UN development and humanitarian agencies are almost entirely funded through voluntary contributions (Goetz and Patz, Reference Goetz and Patz2017; Graham, Reference Graham2015). These contributions largely come from member states, who can (and do) earmark their contributions for specific recipients or purposes, but significant sums also come from private donors.

Several organizations therefore have fundraising strategies targeted toward individuals. UNICEF appeals directly to individuals for donations, e.g., through their Inspired Gifts concept. Footnote 4 Other UN agencies have similar strategies. For example, certain other mandate-driven UN funds and programs, such as UNHCR and the World Food Programme (WFP), list exactly what a specified donation amount will purchase, thus making contributions more concrete to individual donors; the WFP is working to build its individual donor base after the success of its Share the Meal app, and UN Development Programme, in addition to soliciting general-purpose donations from individuals, also appeals for funding from individuals through a recently developed crowdfunding initiative. Thus, contributions from individuals constitute both an important and growing source of revenue for a large number of UN agencies.

Fourteen UN agencies make use of Goodwill Ambassadors to help increase awareness and increase revenue. Footnote 5 These are celebrities who generally work with the organization for multiple years, make appearances across the globe on its behalf, and highlight its causes on social media and other venues. The organizations often have designated staff to manage Goodwill Ambassadors and some have developed handbooks to regulate how the organization is promoted through this mechanism (cf. UNICEF, 2010; UNFPA, 2021).

To maximize the ecological validity of our experiment, we use a real celebrity endorser (the singer Shakira) alongside UNICEF as a representative organization. Shakira was selected due to her global recognition and popularity and her extended tenure as a UNICEF Goodwill Ambassador. She is thus likely to be as credible an endorser as possible for an IO. Footnote 6

Due to the dearth of research on Goodwill Ambassador effectiveness, the experiment was designed to maximize the potential for observing possible impacts of endorsement. In addition to the ecological validity reasons above, we also selected an organization that helps children and is widely known for its good work, i.e., one that is unlikely to be perceived as controversial by respondents. Furthermore, the outcome questions were about hypothetical donations or willingness to learn, which have low activation barriers. Thus, if celebrity endorsement is even somewhat effective, our design should capture it.

The paper tests two pre-registered hypotheses. The former tests whether respondents receiving the celebrity treatment are, on average, more willing to donate to the organization or the topic they saw discussed, whether they donate more money to the organization, or whether they are more willing to learn more about the organization. The latter hypothesis tests the same dependent variables, but additionally tests whether respondents who are co-ethnics of the celebrity endorser are more affected by the treatment than others.

Research design

To answer our research question, we conducted an online survey experiment through Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk) in January 2019, among 1200 US residents, of which 1121 completed the survey. Footnote 7 The sample size was determined based on a combination of resource constraints and power analyses that indicated 93% power to detect differences of $5 and 0.05 in the continuous and binary dependent variables, respectively, at the 5% significance level.

All US-based MTurk workers were eligible to participate, creating a diverse sample across various age, gender, education, and regional groups. It excludes those without internet access, who are also unlikely to be donors to IOs, and very wealthy individuals who, if involved in charitable giving, are perhaps also less likely to be influenced by celebrity endorsement. Thus, the target population of the “ordinary citizens” who might sometimes donate to charitable causes is reasonably represented in our sample. MTurk surveys have generally been found to replicate the sign of effects from nationally representative samples in the United States (e.g., Mullinix et al., Reference Mullinix, Leeper, Druckman and Freese2015; Coppock, Reference Coppock2018) despite concerns that these samples are not always representative (Berinsky, Huber and Lenz, Reference Berinsky, Huber and Lenz2012; Huff and Tingley, Reference Huff and Tingley2015).

The treatment involved respondents reading a brief description of UNICEF and its work on one of two issues and then reading about either a celebrity Goodwill Ambassador (treatment) or a non-celebrity expert (control) highlighting the issue and its importance. To ensure respondents noticed the endorser, we included a small picture for both conditions. See the first part of the Appendix for vignette and outcome question details. We chose a text-based treatment for several reasons. First, to ensure comparability, the content in treatment and control conditions had to be as similar as possible, which was only possible by using text. Second, IOs such as UNICEF often do use text-based messaging from their Goodwill Ambassadors in efforts to increase awareness and donations so this mimics a strategy that is already in use by the actual organizations (e.g., UNFPA (2021)). Further, text-based prompts have been shown to have significant effects in a wide variety of situations (including on persuasion and attitudes), in many cases comparable to audio and visual cues (see, e.g., Chaiken and Eagly (Reference Chaiken and Eagly1976), Corston and Colman (Reference Corston and Colman1997), Wittenberg et al. (Reference Wittenberg, Tappin, Berinsky and Rand2021) and Yadav et al. (Reference Yadav, Phillips, Lundeberg, Koehler, Hilden and Dirkin2011)).

A secondary treatment dimension included respondents reading about either Girls’ Education or Displaced Children to provide more generalizability to the findings. Thus, each respondent was randomly assigned to one of four treatment conditions. Appendix Table A2 reports the number of respondents in each condition. Respondents answered demographic questions before the intervention, and outcome questions regarding interest in UNICEF and willingness to donate to the organization or the issue they had read about after.

We chose UNICEF for three reasons beyond its general representativeness. First, it is almost universally known, including among our respondents (1060/1121 already knew UNICEF). This ensures any treatment effect is not driven by the message increasing awareness of the IO itself. Second, UNICEF deals with children’s issues, a policy domain with fairly universal appeal. Thus, any lack of a treatment effect is unlikely due to respondents being strongly averse to the organization’s mandate. Third, UNICEF is widely known for its use of celebrity Goodwill Ambassadors. Consequently, a UNICEF celebrity endorsement is unlikely to be surprising to respondents.

Shakira was selected as our Goodwill Ambassador for several reasons. First, she is well-recognized across various age groups, genders, and ethnicities. Second, her Latin roots allow us to analyze potential heterogeneous effects of ethnic identity. Third, she has been a UNICEF Goodwill Ambassador since 2003, often endorsing the issues used in the experiment. Footnote 8

Including a UNICEF expert as the control gives us a human endorser in both conditions without which we cannot disentangle the effect of a celebrity Goodwill Ambassador from that of having a human at all. Second, this comparison is more meaningful given our interest in the effectiveness of a celebrity endorser versus someone in-house.

Table 1 summarizes the main variables. The main (continuous) dependent variable is Amount Donated (to UNICEF), where respondents were asked to (hypothetically) split $100 between themselves and UNICEF. A further robustness check to ensure that any treatment effect was not simply due to the hypothetical nature of this question was that half the respondents were randomly told that one person’s split would be randomly selected and implemented. Though respondents in this group donated less, on average, there was no difference in the treatment effect between both groups, as shown in Appendix Table A15. The remaining dependent variables are binary. Donate to cause and Donate to UNICEF indicate whether the respondent was likely to donate to the cause they had read about or to UNICEF in general, while Learn about UNICEF measures interest in learning more about UNICEF at the end of the survey. Another post-treatment measure asked if respondents had previously heard of UNICEF. Virtually all had and this variable is not used in the analyses. Given the research design, respondents should be well-balanced on demographic factors; Appendix Table A3 nonetheless presents confirmatory balance tests. Appendix Table A4 also presents more detailed summary statistics for all four dependent variables by treatment group.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics

Results

We use linear regressions with the coefficient on Goodwill Ambassador representing the ATE. Figure 1 summarizes our main results across all four dependent variables with two specifications for each, with and without covariates. The main feature across all eight models is the lack of a significant treatment effect, indicating that, counter to the expectations of our first hypothesis, an endorsement from Shakira does not significantly affect a respondent’s donation behavior toward, or interest in, UNICEF. (See Appendix Table A9 for similar results when separating the treatment into the two causes.) This lack of significance is robust to various control variables. Footnote 9

Figure 1. Celebrity endorsement and attitudes toward UNICEF.

Note: This plot summarizes the average treatment effect of a Goodwill Ambassador on four dependent variables, explained above. For each pair of regressions, the top line is the baseline result while the second adds covariates. The thicker bars around the point estimates indicate the 90% confidence intervals, while the thinner extensions denote the 95% intervals. See Table A5 in the Appendix for full results.

The lack of significance is unlikely explained by other factors. First, it is unlikely due to sample size as the ATE on the main dependent variable is substantively very small, at $1 out of a possible $100, indicating no meaningful difference in donation choices between the two groups. The same holds true for the other (binary) dependent variables where the coefficient is no larger than 0.02. Footnote 10

Second, to guard against lack of attention driving any null findings, we asked respondents, post-treatment (so the analysis can only be treated descriptively), if they had seen any celebrities in the study and, if they said yes, whom they had seen. Only 54 of 568 treated respondents did not recall a celebrity and only 12 of the remainder could not recall her name. Thus, only 10% of respondents could not accurately identify Shakira. As a robustness check, we re-ran our analyses restricting our sample to those who did pay attention. The results are virtually unchanged (see Appendix Table A10).

Third, we ensure that the lack of ATE is not due to Shakira being disliked and, therefore, ineffective. Post-treatment, respondents rated their like for several celebrities, including Shakira, on a 0–10 Likert scale, with 5 being neutral. Approximately 17% of the sample rated Shakira less than neutral, which is comparable to other celebrities on the list. Our results are robust to dropping these respondents from the analysis (see Appendix Table A11).

Lastly, it does not seem that UNICEF is already so well-known and well-liked that there is no room for celebrity endorsement to add value. As Table 1 shows, the average donation is under $38 (out of a potential $100) and almost 20% of our respondents chose to donate nothing. Thus, there is ample room for the Goodwill Ambassador to increase donations. Furthermore, given UNICEF’s extensive use of Goodwill Ambassadors to promote its causes, the organization itself likely believes that celebrities bring added value. Similarly, this finding is not driven by respondents who would never donate to the UNICEF or the given issue more generally (see Appendix Table A12).

Heterogeneous treatment effects

Next, we test the second hypothesis of whether shared ethnicity (Hispanic, in this case) with a celebrity Goodwill Ambassador can make respondents react differently. Results displayed in Figure 2 show limited evidence of this. The specifications and variables are the same as before, the only difference being an interaction between the treatment, Goodwill Ambassador, and dummy variable Hispanic. Although only significant at the 10% level, and only in one specification, we do find that Hispanics in the treatment group donate, on average, $10 more than others (left-most panel).

Figure 2. Celebrity endorsement, ethnicity, and attitudes toward UNICEF.

Note: This figure summarizes results for heterogeneous treatment effects of celebrity Goodwill Ambassadors on those who share an ethnicity with the celebrity. The four dependent variables and each specification are the same as presented in earlier results. The thicker bars around the point estimates indicate the 90% confidence intervals, while the thinner extensions denote the 95% intervals. See Table A6 in the Appendix for full results.

The base term ensures this effect is driven by Hispanic respondents in our sample being more responsive to the Goodwill Ambassador rather than them being more charitable than non-Hispanics. Table A7 (Appendix) re-weights our data to account for the lower proportion of Hispanic respondents in our sample than the national average, which strengthens the findings. Exploratory analysis finds no evidence of other minority groups being more responsive to the treatment, suggesting that our findings are not likely due to non-majority groups being more prone to celebrity influence. Footnote 11,Footnote 12

Finally, we consider possible heterogeneous treatment effects by gender based on the vast literature that finds gender differences in donation behavior (cf. Piper and Schnepf, Reference Piper and Schnepf2008; Wheeler, Reference Wheeler2009; Mesch et al., Reference Mesch, Osili, Pactor, Ackerman, Bergdoll and Dale2016; Dale et al., Reference Dale, Ackerman, Mesch, Osili and Garcia2018) and finds that men and women respond to different types of cues (cf. Meyers-Levy and Maheswaran, Reference Meyers-Levy and Maheswaran1991; Klaus and Bailey, Reference Klaus and Bailey2008; Wymer and Drollinger, Reference Wymer and Drollinger2015). These results, summarized in Figure 3, were not pre-registered so they should be taken as exploratory. We include them nonetheless as the celebrity in our experiment is a woman and widely considered attractive, which may affect men and women differently.

Figure 3. Celebrity endorsement, gender, and attitudes toward UNICEF.

Note: This table summarizes results for heterogeneous treatment effects of celebrity Goodwill Ambassadors by gender. The four dependent variables and each specification are the same as presented in earlier results. The thicker bars around the point estimates indicate the 90% confidence intervals, while the thinner extensions denote the 95% intervals. See Table A8 in the Appendix for full results.

Two results stand out. First, in line with existing literature, men have less interest in UNICEF and are less likely to donate, indicated by negative coefficients across all specifications, most of which are statistically significant and substantively meaningful. In the first sub-figure, men donate, on average, approximately $8 less than women in the control condition. Second, for Amount donated, we find a positive ATE among men, indicated by the positive significant interaction coefficient. Men in the treatment condition donate, on average, almost $7 more than men in the control condition, making their average donation statistically indistinguishable from women’s. Footnote 13 In other words, the Goodwill Ambassador endorsement attenuates the difference between men and women. As before, however, these differences do not extend to the other dependent variables.

Overall, our findings suggest a limited effect of a celebrity Goodwill Ambassador, even in a “most likely” scenario where the celebrity is well-known and well-liked, the causes and organization are credible and non-controversial, the donation does not involve giving up actual money, and the intervention is very similar to how people are likely to encounter it in the real world.

Conclusion

IOs are predicting a massive shortfall in humanitarian funding (OCHA, 2023), making it important to analyze fundraising effectiveness. In this paper, we examine a common IO fundraising tool, namely celebrity Goodwill Ambassadors. Contrary to our expectations and much extant literature, we do not find clear effects of celebrity endorsement.

Why might this be? A potential explanation is that respondents in the treatment condition “overcorrect” and donate less when they see Shakira as the endorser, thinking that this must be a trick to get them to donate (Kang and Herr, Reference Kang and Herr2006). Another possibility is that the perceived expertise of the UNICEF Director General, whose picture respondents in the control condition saw, is sufficient to make her as effective as Shakira (Wymer and Drollinger, Reference Wymer and Drollinger2015). If this is the case, it is unlikely that many organization officials would command the same perception, meaning that Goodwill Ambassadors may still be effective in general. Lastly, the plight of the children in the vignettes may seem farther removed from the everyday lives of potential donors in the United States than a domestic charity appeal would. The hundreds of millions of dollars donated each year by individuals through the US National Committee for UNICEF, however, suggest otherwise. Further, an observational study in the UK showed no material differences in the types of individuals who donate nationally vs internationally (Micklewright and Schnepf, Reference Micklewright and Schnepf2009).

These findings are not without limitations. Different endorsers, organizations, or modes of appeal could have different effects, and our findings only speak to a certain type of organization. Specifically, the parameters of our experiment were chosen to maximize the likelihood of finding a treatment effect within an IO that is representative of a broad range of IOs that rely on voluntary contributions, fund-raise from individuals using text-based appeals, and focus primarily on humanitarian and development issues that many around the world care about. Despite this, we find limited effects, suggesting that further research is needed to determine the overall effectiveness of celebrity endorsement in the context of IOs.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/XPS.2024.6

Data availability

Support for this research was provided by the Social Science Mentoring Programme at the Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich. The data, code, and any additional materials required to replicate all analyses in this article and its supplementary appendix are available through the Journal of Experimental Political Science Dataverse within the Harvard Dataverse Network at: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/CKBFMB.

Acknowledgments

We thank Manu Munoz Herrera, Rebecca Morton, Daniel Nielson, Blaine Robbins, participants at the NYUAD PostDoc Workshop, the panel on “Peddling Influence” at EPSA in 2019, the IR Colloquium at LMU Munich in 2020, the ECPR Joint Session on “Resourcing International Organizations” in 2023, and the panel on “Governing International Organizations” at EPSA in 2023 for comments and suggestions on the research design and earlier versions of this paper.

Competing interests

Neither author has any conflicts of interest to declare.

Ethics statement

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at New York University Abu Dhabi with approval ID #114-2018, where one of the authors was based at the time of conducting the experiment. The article fulfills reporting standards recommended by the APSA Organized Section on Experimental Research and adheres to APSA’s Principles and Guidance for Human Subjects Research. Further details on the design can be found in the supplementary appendix.

Footnotes

This article has earned badges for transparent research practices: Open data and Open materials. For details see the Data Availability Statement.

The study was pre-registered and the pre-registration plan can be accessed at https://osf.io/k3bj9.

1 Per annual reports, UNICEF has received around $1.5B in total private sector revenue, with $$\sim $$ 60-65% coming from 8-10 million individuals.

3 For example, UNICEF’s Guide to Working with Goodwill Ambassadors notes that celebrities have helped raise funds and that they are recruited in part because of their “ability to reach specific audiences” (UNICEF, 2010, 4-5). The UN Population Fund (UNFPA) has a similar, but more recent report, that highlights sharing messages about the organization on social media as one of the roles of a Goodwill Ambassador (UNFPA, 2021).

4 UNICEF’s Inspired Gifts allow individuals to ‘gift’ a donation to a friend or loved one. The gift is in the form of specific items—e.g., mosquito nets or school supplies—which are then distributed by UNICEF.

5 For details on UN Goodwill Ambassadors, see https://www.un.org/en/isotope-articles/9189

6 The survey was conducted in 2019, well before Shakira’s recent Spanish tax fraud accusations, which might otherwise have affected her effectiveness (although findings on negative implications of endorser transgressions are mixed (Bergkvist and Zhou, Reference Bergkvist and Zhou2016)).

7 For data and supporting files, see Malik and Thorvaldsdottir (2024). Also note that the authors only have access to the 1121 completed surveys so it is not possible for us to know whether the 79 respondents who did not complete the survey dropped out before or after treatment. However, given the low rate of attrition and the short nature of the experiment, we expect this to not have affected the findings in any meaningful way.

8 For further information, see https://www.unicef.org/people/people_47895.html.

9 We also conducted ANOVA tests to compare the presented models with restricted versions that exclude the treatment dummy variable. The ‘full’ models, presented here, do not have more explanatory power than the ‘restricted’ ones; full results available on request.

10 Though the substantive differences here are even smaller than what was discussed in the power analyses, we would argue that even if the coefficients had statistical significance, they would essentially be precisely estimated zeroes.

11 With Hispanics, the largest minority groups in our data self-identify as Black or Asian; neither group is significantly affected by the treatment.

12 Appendix Tables A13 and A14 present remaining pre-registered analyses on heterogeneous effects of political leanings and do not find significant differences.

13 The composite coefficient of −1.4 on Male has a standard error of 2.62, indicating a lack of statistical significance.

References

Andreoni, James. 1989. “Giving with Impure Altruism: Applications to Charity and Ricardian Equivalence.” Journal of Political Economy 97: 14471458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bekkers, René, and Wiepking, Pamala. 2011. “A Literature Review of Empirical Studies of Philanthropy: Eight Mechanisms That Drive Charitable Giving.” A Literature Review of Empirical Studies of Philanthropy: Eight Mechanisms That Drive Charitable Giving 40: 924973.Google Scholar
Bergkvist, Lars, and Zhou, Kris Qiang. 2016. “Celebrity Endorsements: A Literature Review and Research Agenda.” International Journal of Advertising 35: 642663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berinsky, Adam J, Huber, Gregory A., and Lenz, Gabriel S.. 2012. “Evaluating Online Labor Markets for Experimental Research: Amazon. com’s Mechanical Turk.” Political analysis 20: 351368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, William J. 2018. “Celebrity Involvement: Parasocial Interaction, Identification and Worship.” In Routledge Handbook of Celebrity Studies. Routledge, 16.Google Scholar
Chaiken, Shelly, and Eagly, Alice H.. 1976. “Communication Modality as a Determinant of Message Persuasiveness and Message Comprehensibility.” Journal of personality and social psychology 34: 605614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coppock, Alexander. 2018. “Generalizing From Survey Experiments Conducted on Mechanical Turk: A Replication Approach.” Political Science Research and Methods, Cambridge University Press, 116. Google Scholar
Corston, Rod, and Colman, Andrew M.. 1997. “Modality of Communication and Recall of Health-Related Information.” Journal of Health Psychology 2: 185194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, Elizabeth J., Ackerman, Jacqueline, Mesch, Debra J., Osili, Una Okonkwo, and Garcia, Silvia. 2018. “Giving to Women and Girls: An Emerging Area of Philanthropy.” Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Quarterly 47: 241261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deshpande, Rohit, and Stayman, Douglas M.. 1994. “A Tale of Two Cities: Distinctiveness Theory and Advertising Effectiveness.” Journal of Marketing Research 31: 5764.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erdogan, B. Zafer. 1999. “Celebrity Endorsement: A Literature Review.” Journal of marketing management 15: 291314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giles, David C. 2002. “Parasocial Interaction: A Review of the Literature and a Model for Future Research.” Media Psychology 4: 279305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goetz, Klaus H., and Patz, Ronny. 2017. “Resourcing International Organizations: Resource Diversification, Organizational Differentiation, and Administrative Governance.” Global Policy 8: 514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, Erin R. 2015. “Money and Multilateralism: How Funding Rules Constitute IO Governance.” International Theory 7: 162194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halder, Deepa, Pradhan, Debasis, and Roy Chaudhuri, Himadri. 2021. “Forty-five Years of Celebrity Credibility and Endorsement Literature: Review and Learnings.” Journal of Business Research 125: 397415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huff, Connor, and Tingley, Dustin. 2015. ““Who are These People?” Evaluating the Demographic Characteristics and Political Preferences of MTurk Survey Respondents.” Research & Politics 2: 2053168015604648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ilicic, Jasmina, and Webster, Cynthia M.. 2013. “Celebrity Co-branding Partners as Irrelevant Brand Information in Advertisements.” Journal of Business Research 66: 941947.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ilicic, Jasmina, and Baxter, Stacey. 2014. “Fit in Celebrity–Charity Alliances: When Perceived Celanthropy Benefits Nonprofit Organisations.” International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing 19: 200208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamins, Michael A. 1990. “An Investigation into the “Match-up” Hypothesis in Celebrity Advertising: When Beauty May Be Only Skin Deep.” Journal of Advertising 19: 413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kang, Yong-Soon, and Herr, Paul M.. 2006. “Beauty and the Beholder: Toward an Integrative Model of Communication Source Effects.” Journal of Consumer Research 33: 123130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karlan, Dean, and List, John A.. 2012. How Can Bill and Melinda Gates Increase Other People’s Donations to Fund Public Goods? Working Paper 17954 National Bureau of Economic Research. Series: Working Paper Series tex.urldate: 2019-06-08TZ. URL: http://www.nber.org/papers/w17954 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klaus, Nathan, and Bailey, Ainsworth Anthony. 2008. “Celebrity Endorsements: An Examination of Gender and Consumers’ Attitudes.” American Journal of Business 23: 5362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kumru, Cagri S., and Vesterlund, Lise. 2010. “The Effect of Status on Charitable Giving.” Journal of Public Economic Theory 12: 709735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malik, Rabia, and Thorvaldsdottir, Svanhildur. 2024. “Replication Data for: Are Goodwill Ambassadors Good for Business? The Impact of Celebrities on IO Fundraising.” URL: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/CKBFMB CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesch, Debra, Osili, Una, Pactor, Andrea, Ackerman, Jacqueline, Bergdoll, Jonathan, and Dale, Elizabeth. 2016. “Giving to women and girls: Who gives, and why?” IPUI Women’s Philanthropy Institute.Google Scholar
Meyers-Levy, Joan, and Maheswaran, Durairaj. 1991. “Exploring Differences in Males’ and Females’ Processing Strategies.” Journal of Consumer Research 18: 6370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Micklewright, John, and Schnepf, Sylke V.. 2009. “Who Gives Charitable Donations for Overseas Development?” Journal of Social Policy 38: 317341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mullinix, Kevin J., Leeper, Thomas J., Druckman, James N., and Freese, Jeremy. 2015. “The Generalizability of Survey Experiments.” Journal of Experimental Political Science 2: 109138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
OCHA. 2023. Global Humanitarian Overview 2024: Abridged Report. Technical report. URL: https://reliefweb.int/report/world/global-humanitarian-overview-2024-enarfres Google Scholar
Ohanian, Roobina. 1991. “The Impact of Celebrity Spokespersons’ Perceived Image on Consumers’ Intention to Purchase.” Journal of Advertising Research 31: 4654.Google Scholar
Park, Sun-Young. 2017. “Celebrity Endorsement for Nonprofit Organizations: The Role of Experience-based Fit between Celebrity and Cause.” International Business Research 10: 821.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piper, Greg, and Schnepf, Sylke V.. 2008. “Gender Differences in Charitable Giving in Great Britain.” Voluntas : international journal of voluntary and nonprofit organizations 19: 103124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Till, Brian D. 1998. “Using Celebrity Endorsers Effectively: Lessons from Associative Learning.” Journal of Product & Brand Management 7: 400409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
UNFPA. 2021. Policies and Procedures Manual: Policy and Procedures for UNFPA’s Work with Goodwill Ambassadors and other Celebrity Spokespersons. manual.Google Scholar
UNICEF. 2010. A Guide to Working with Goodwill Ambassadors. manual.Google Scholar
Wheeler, Robert T. 2009. “Nonprofit Advertising: Impact of Celebrity Connection, Involvement and Gender on Source Credibility and Intention to Volunteer Time or Donate Money.” Journal of Nonprofit & Public Sector Marketing 21: 80107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittenberg, Chloe, Tappin, Ben M., Berinsky, Adam J., and Rand, David G.. 2021. “The (minimal) persuasive advantage of political video over text.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 118: 17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wymer, Walter, and Drollinger, Tanya. 2015. “Charity Appeals Using Celebrity Endorsers: Celebrity Attributes Most Predictive of Audience Donation Intentions.” VOLUNTAS: International Journal of Voluntary and Nonprofit Organizations 26: 26942717.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yadav, Aman, Phillips, Michael, Lundeberg, Mary, Koehler, Matthew, Hilden, Katherine, and Dirkin, Kathryn. 2011. “If a Picture is Worth a Thousand Words is Video Worth a Million? Differences in Affective and Cognitive Processing of Video and Text Cases.” J. Computing in Higher Education 23: 1537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics

Figure 1

Figure 1. Celebrity endorsement and attitudes toward UNICEF.Note: This plot summarizes the average treatment effect of a Goodwill Ambassador on four dependent variables, explained above. For each pair of regressions, the top line is the baseline result while the second adds covariates. The thicker bars around the point estimates indicate the 90% confidence intervals, while the thinner extensions denote the 95% intervals. See Table A5 in the Appendix for full results.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Celebrity endorsement, ethnicity, and attitudes toward UNICEF.Note: This figure summarizes results for heterogeneous treatment effects of celebrity Goodwill Ambassadors on those who share an ethnicity with the celebrity. The four dependent variables and each specification are the same as presented in earlier results. The thicker bars around the point estimates indicate the 90% confidence intervals, while the thinner extensions denote the 95% intervals. See Table A6 in the Appendix for full results.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Celebrity endorsement, gender, and attitudes toward UNICEF.Note: This table summarizes results for heterogeneous treatment effects of celebrity Goodwill Ambassadors by gender. The four dependent variables and each specification are the same as presented in earlier results. The thicker bars around the point estimates indicate the 90% confidence intervals, while the thinner extensions denote the 95% intervals. See Table A8 in the Appendix for full results.

Supplementary material: File

Malik et al. supplementary material 1

Malik et al. supplementary material
Download Malik et al. supplementary material 1(File)
File 296.2 KB
Supplementary material: File

Malik et al. supplementary material 2

Malik et al. supplementary material
Download Malik et al. supplementary material 2(File)
File 336.7 KB
Supplementary material: Link

Malik and Thorvaldsdottir Dataset

Link