Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T12:39:04.418Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

492. The importance of psychophysical errors in subjective judgements of firmness of Cheshire cheese

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 June 2009

D. Sheppard
Affiliation:
National Institute for Research in Dairying, University of Reading

Extract

1. Ratings for firmness which were carried out on 150 Cheshire cheese at 2 weeks old, by three judges, were examined so as to extract the expected psychophysical errors.

2. It was found that the value for firmness given to any particular cheese tended to be a function of the average firmness of the group of cheese in which it was judged, so that a cheese of a given objective value would be given a different rating according to its position in the series being judged.

3. A cheese of a given value presented after a firm one is judged softer than if it had been presented after a soft one. This contrast effect was masked by the influence of the group as described in (2). Nevertheless, it seems probable that contrast effects influence the judgements.

4. The frequency distributions of the subjective judgements were examined, and it was noted that the ‘semi-expert’ judge gave subjective values which approximated very closely to a normal distribution, more nearly so than the judgements of the other two judges.

5. Reasons are suggested for the differences in the values allotted to the cheese as judged by the different judges.

6. Some suggestions are made for eliminating the effects described.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Proprietors of Journal of Dairy Research 1952

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

(1)Baron, M. & Harper, R. (1951). Dairy Industr. 16, 45.Google Scholar
(2)Hamper, R. & Baron, M. (1950). J. Dairy Res. 17, 329.Google Scholar
(3)Harper, R. & Baron, M. (1949). J. Dairy Res. 16, 363.Google Scholar
(4)Tresselt, M. E. & Volkmann, J. (1942). J. abnorm. (soc.) Psychol. 37, 234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
(5)Tresselt, M. E. (1947). J. exp. Psychol. 37, 251.Google Scholar
(6)Tresselt, M. E. (1948). J. soc. Psychol. 27, 209.Google Scholar
(7)Turner, W. D. (1931). Amer. J. Psychol. 43, 1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
(8)Long, L. (1937). Arch. Psychol. 209.Google Scholar
(9)Cohen, N. E. (1937). Amer. J. Psychol. 49, 93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
(10)Boggs, M. M. & Hanson, H. L. (1949). Advances in Food Research, 2, 219–58. New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
(11)Sheppard, D. (1952). Dairy Industr. 17, 707.Google Scholar