Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:20:20.100Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Verbal and nonverbal aspects of mothers' directives*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

H. R. Schaffer
Affiliation:
University of Strathclyde
Anne Hepburn
Affiliation:
University of Strathclyde
G. M. Collis
Affiliation:
University of Strathclyde

Abstract

Maternal directives to children aged 10 and 18 months were analysed with respect to both verbal and concomitant nonverbal aspects. The findings emphasize the multimodal nature of mothers' communicative messages and draw attention to the way in which language occurs in an action context and not as an isolated output. No indication was found for this age range that verbal devices come to replace nonverbal ones. At both ages most utterances were accompanied by some form of nonverbal behaviour; the precise nature of the association, however, gives little support to the idea of parallel coding. The child's own behaviour is also shown to form part of the context in which maternal speech takes place.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1983

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was supported by a grant from the Social Science Research Council. The authors are grateful for the help and advice given by Margaret Hunter, Christine Liddell, Rhian Powell, Norman Sharp and Charmaine Whitehouse. Address for correspondence: H. R. Schaffer, Department of Psychology, University of Strathclyde, Turnbull Building, 155 George Street, Glasgow, G1 1RD, Scotland.

References

REFERENCES

Argyle, M. (1975). Bodily communication. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Bellinger, D. (1979). Changes in the explicitness of mothers' directives as children age. JChLang 6. 443–58.Google ScholarPubMed
Bishop, Y. M. M., Fienberg, S. E. & Holland, P. W. (1975). Discrete multivariate analysis: theory and practice. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1973). Talking, understanding and thinking. In Schiefelbush, R. & Lloyd, L. (eds), Language perspectives: acquisition, retardation and intervention. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Bridges, A. (1979). Directing two-year-olds' attention: some clues to understanding. JChLang 6. 211–26.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1977). Early social interaction and language acquisition. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Collis, G. M. (1977). Visual co-orientation and maternal speech. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Collis, G. M. & Schaffer, H. R. (1975). Synchronisation of visual attention in mother–infant pairs. JChPsycholPsychiat 16. 315–20.Google ScholarPubMed
Fleiss, J. L. (1973). Statistical methods for rates and proportions. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Gentner, D. (1978). On relational meaning: the acquisition of verb meaning. ChDev 49. 988–98.Google Scholar
Gutmann, A. J. & Turnure, J. E. (1979). Mothers' production of hand gestures while communicating with their preschool children under various task conditions. DevPsych 15. 197203.Google Scholar
Harper, R. G., Wiens, A. N. & Matarazzo, J. D. (1978). Nonverbal communication: the state of the art. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
McCall, R. B., Parke, R. D. & Kavanagh, R. D. (1977). Imitation of live and televised models by children one to three years of age. Monogr.Soc.Res.Ch.Devel. 42 (No. 5), Serial No. 173.Google ScholarPubMed
Macnamara, J. (1972). Cognitive basis of language learning in infants. PsychRev 79. 113.Google ScholarPubMed
Macnamara, J. (1977). From sign to language. In Macnamara, J. (ed.), Language, learning and thought. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Messer, D. J. (1978). The integration of mother's referential speech with joint play. ChDev 49. 781–7.Google Scholar
Messer, D. J. (1981). Non-linguistic information facilitating the young child's comprehension of adult speech. In Robinson, W. P. (ed.), Communication in development. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Murphy, C. (1978). Pointing in the context of a shared activity. ChDev 49. 371–80.Google Scholar
Oviatt, S. L. (1980). The emerging ability to comprehend language: an experimental approach. ChDev 51. 97106.Google ScholarPubMed
Phillips, J. R. (1973). Syntax and vocabulary of mothers' speech to young children: age and sex comparisons. ChDev 44. 182–5.Google Scholar
Rodgon, M. M. & Kauffman, A. (1979). Mothers' gestures to children acquiring language. Paper presented to Society for Research in Child Development,San Francisco.Google Scholar
Schaffer, H. R. (1977). Early interactive development. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Schaffer, H. R. & Crook, C. K. (1979). Maternal control techniques in a directed play situation. ChDev 50. 989–98.Google Scholar
Schaffer, H. R. (1980). Child compliance and maternal control techniques. DevPsych 16. 5461.Google Scholar
Schneiderman, M. H. (1983). ‘Do what I mean, not what I say!’. Changes in mothers' action-directives to young children. JChLang 10. 357–67.Google Scholar
Shatz, M. (1978). Children's comprehension of their mothers' question-directives. JChLang 5. 3946.Google Scholar
Shatz, M. (1982). On mechanisms of language acquisition: can features of the communicative environment account for development? In Gleitman, L. & Wanner, E. (eds), Language acquisition: the state of the art. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. JChLang 4. 122.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds). (1977). Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar