Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:56:08.150Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The use of props in a syntactic awareness task*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Amanda Marie Blackmore*
Affiliation:
Edith Cowan University
Chris Pratt
Affiliation:
The University of Tasmania
Alison Dewsbury
Affiliation:
The Western Australian Ministry of Education
*
Edith Cowan University, Joondalup Campus, Joondalup Drive, Joondalup, Western Australia, 6027.

Abstract

The study investigated the effects of using props to illustrate the meaning of sentences in a syntactic awareness task in which subjects were required to correct ungrammatical sentences. Forty preschool children (aged 4;11 to 5;11) were asked to correct 20 sentences, 10 of which were illustrated using props, and 10 of which were not. Children scored significantly higher in the props condition though the proportion of meaning-changing errors to total errors was not significantly lower in this condition. It is argued that the use of props, by providing contextual support, reduces the processing capacity required to complete the task.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

We gratefully acknowledge the co-operation of the children who participated in this study and their teachers. While this research was being conducted, the first author was receiving a Commonwealth Postgraduate Scholarship and this research was part of the work submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy of the University of Western Australia, Department of Psychology.

References

REFERENCES

Bialystok, E. (1986). Factors in the growth of linguistic awareness. Child Development 57, 498510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1988). Aspects of linguistic awareness in reading comprehension. Applied Psycholinguistics 9, 123–39.Google Scholar
Bohannon, J. N. (1975). The relationship between syntax discrimination and sentence imitation in children. Child Development 46, 444–51.Google Scholar
Bohannon, J. N. (1976). Normal and scrambled grammar in discrimination, imitation, and comprehension. Child Development 47, 669–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohannon, J. N., Warren-Leubecker, A. & Hepler, N. (1984). Word order awareness and early reading. Child Development 55, 1541–8.Google Scholar
Bowey, J. A. (1986 a). Syntactic awareness in relation to reading skill and ongoing reading comprehension monitoring. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 41, 282–99.Google Scholar
Bowey, J. A. (1986 b). Syntactic awareness and verbal performance from preschool to fifth grade. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 15, 285308.Google Scholar
Bowey, J. A. & Patel, P. K. (1988). Metalinguistic ability and early reading achievement. Applied Psycholinguistics 9, 367–83.Google Scholar
de Villiers, P. A. & de Villiers, J. G. (1972). Early judgements of semantic and syntactic acceptability by children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 1, 299310.Google Scholar
Fowler, A. E. (1988). Grammaticality judgements and reading skill. Annals of Dyslexia 38, 7394.Google Scholar
Gleitman, L. R., Gleitman, H. & Shipley, E. F. (1972). The emergence of the child as grammarian. Cognition 1, 137–64.Google Scholar
Hakes, D. T., Evans, J. S. & Tunmer, W. (1980). The development of metalinguistic abilities in children. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pratt, C., Tunmer, W. E. & Bowey, J. A. (1984). Children's capacity to correct grammatical violations in sentences. Journal of Child Language 11, 129–41.Google Scholar
Ryan, E. B. & Ledger, G. W. (1979). Grammaticality judgements, sentence repetitions and sentence corrections of children learning to read. International Journal of Psycholinguistics 6(4), 2340.Google Scholar
Scholl, D. M. & Ryan, E. B. (1975). Child judgements of sentences varying in grammatical complexity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 20, 274–85.Google Scholar
Scholl, D. M. & Ryan, E. B. (1980). Development of metalinguistic performance in the early school years. Language and Speech 23, 199211.Google Scholar
Siegel, L. S. & Ryan, E. B. (1988). Development of grammatical-sensitivity, phonological, and short-term memory skills in normally achieving learning disabled children. Developmental Psychology 24, 2837.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. L. & Tager-Flusberg, H. (1982). Metalinguistic awareness and language development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 34, 449–68.Google Scholar
Tunmer, W. E., Nesdale, A. R. & Wright, A. D. (1987). Syntactic awareness and reading acquisition. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 5, 2534.Google Scholar
Warren-Leubecker, A. (1987). Competence and performance factors in word order awareness and early reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 43, 6280.Google Scholar
Warren-Leubecker, A. & Carter, B. W. (1988). Reading and growth in metalinguistic awareness: relations to socioeconomic status and reading readiness skills. Child Development 59, 728–42.Google Scholar