Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T15:17:18.387Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Towards a theory of definition*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Rita Watson
Affiliation:
University of British Columbia

Abstract

A brief theoretical statement on definition is advanced, followed by a study of the development of definition in children aged 5;0–10;0. The empirical findings suggest that the emergence of superordinate terms in definition cannot be adequately accounted for simply by appeal to changes in underlying knowledge, or to selectional restrictions on the structure of the definiens. Rather, the development of definition can best be characterized as the gradual articulation of a conventional definitional form out of the more general forms of ordinary oral discourse. It is suggested that definitions are expressions of word meaning that reflect the unique requirements of a literate register, and that the increasing dominance of superordinate terms in definitions reflects in part the adoption of this conventional form of expression.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1985

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Al-Issa, I. (1969). The development of word definitions in children. JGenetPsychol 114. 25–8.Google Scholar
Anglin, J. (1977). Word, object and conceptual development. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Bierwisch, M. & Kiefer, F. (1969). Remarks on definitions in natural languages. In Kiefer, F., (ed.), Studies in syntax and semantics. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Binet, A. & Simon, T. (1916). The development of intelligence in children. Trans. Kite, E.. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.Google Scholar
Bolinger, D. (1975). Meaning and memory. Forum Linguisticum 1. 114.Google Scholar
Bruner, J., Goodnow, J. & Austin, G. (1956). A study of thinking. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Casagrande, J. B. & Hale, K. (1967). Semantic relationships in Papago folk definitions. In Hymes, D. & Bittle, W. (eds), Studies in southwestern ethnolinguistics. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Cole, M. & Scribner, S. (1973). Culture and thought. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Eisenstein, E. (1981). The printing press as an agent of change. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Feifel, H. & Lorge, I. (1950). Qualitative differences in the vocabulary responses of children. JEdPsychol 41. 118.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J. (1974). The origins of language comprehension. In Solso, R. L.(ed.), Theories in cognitive psychology. Potomac, Md.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Inhelder, B. & Piaget, J. (1964). The early growth of logic in the child: classification and seriation. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1960). Linguistics and poetics. In Sebeok, T. A. (ed.), Style in language. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Johnson, S. (1755). A dictionary of the English language (2 vols.). London: W. Strahan.Google Scholar
Krauss, R. (1952). A hole is to dig. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Kripke, S. (1972). Naming and necessity. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Levi-Strauss, C. (1962). The savage mind. London: Weidenfeld & Nicholson.Google Scholar
Litowitz, B. (1977). Learning to make definitions. JChLang 4. 289304.Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1976). Cognitive development: its cultural and social foundations. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Macnamara, J. (1982). Names for things. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
McLuhan, M. (1962). The Gutenberg galaxy. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1977). The syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift revisited: a review of research and theory. PsycholBull 84. 93116.Google ScholarPubMed
Nelson, K. (1978). Semantic development and the development of semantic memory. In Nelson, K. E. (ed.), Children's language, Vol. 1. New York: Gardner Press.Google Scholar
Norlin, P. F. (1981). The development of relational arcs in the lexical semantic memory structures of young children. JChLang 8. 385402.Google Scholar
Olson, D. R. (1977). From utterance to text: the bias of language in speech and writing. HarvEdRev 47. 257–81.Google Scholar
Olson, D. R. (1982). The language of schooling. Topics in Language Disorders 2. 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ong, W. (1967). The presence of the word. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (1975). Mind, language and reality. Philosophical papers, Vol. 2. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Rinsland, H. D. (1945). A basic vocabulary of elementary school children. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Robinson, R. (1950). Definition. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Scribner, S. & Cole, M. (1981). The Psychology of Literacy. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1975). What is an intentional state? In Gunderson, K. (ed.), Language, mind and knowledge. Minnesota studies in the philosophy of Science, 7, 344–69.Google Scholar
Starnes, D. T. & Noyes, E. (1946). The English dictionary from Cawdrey to Johnson. Chapel Hill, N.C.: University of North Carolina Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1962). Thought and language. Trans. Hanfmann, E. & Vakar, E.. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Watson, R. & Olson, D. R. (in press). From meaning to definition: a literate bias on the structure of word meaning. In Horowitz, R. & Samuels, J. (eds), Comprehending oral and written language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1974). The Wechsler intelligence scale for children (Revised). New York: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wehren, A., De Lisi, R. & Arnold, M. (1981). The development of noun definition. JChLang 8. 165–75.Google ScholarPubMed
Wolman, R. N. & Barker, E. N. (1969). A developmental study of word definitions. JGenetPsychol 107. 159–66.Google Scholar