Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T23:03:48.889Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Timing in late babbling*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Bénédicte De Boysson-Bardies
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Psychologie, C.N.R.S.
Nicole Bacri
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Psychologie, C.N.R.S.
Laurent Sagart
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Psychologie, C.N.R.S.
Michel Poizat
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Psychologie, C.N.R.S.

Abstract

The temporal organization of the late babbling of a French child during the pivotal period between babbling and the production of meaningful utterances is studied. Two points are analysed: the segmental timing in terms of syllabic durations and the durations of sequences according to their intonational contours. Articulation rate in late babbling is fairly rapid and stable and does not vary with length of sequences. However, at the end of the period studied, longer durations are found for bi- and tri-syllabic sequences. This lengthening can be seen to reflect changes in the child's target programming and the constraints on co-articulation that it implies. Additional changes occur also as a function of intonational contours.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1981

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Allen, G. D. (1979). Formal and statistical models of speech timing: past, present and future. Proc. IX Int. Cong. Phon. Sci., Copenhagen. Vol. 11. 245–52.Google Scholar
Allen, G. D. & Hawkins, S. (1978). The development of phonological rhythm. In Bell, A. & Hooper, J. B. (eds), Syllables and segments. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
de Boysson-Bardies, B., Sagart, L. & Bacri, N. (1981). Phonetic analysis of late babbling: a case study of a French child. JChLang 8. 511524.Google ScholarPubMed
Brannigan, G. (1976). Syllabic structure and the acquisition of consonants: the great conspiracy in word formation. JPsycholingRes 5. 117–33.Google Scholar
Crystal, D. (1973). Non-segmental phonology in language acquisition: a review of the issues. Lingua 32. 145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delattre, P. (1938). L'accent final en français: accent d'intensité, accent de hauteur, accent de durée. French Review 12. 141–5.Google Scholar
Delattre, P. (1966). A comparison of syllable length conditioning among languages. IRAL 4. 183–98.Google Scholar
Dore, J., Franklin, M. B., Miller, R. T. & Ramer, A. L. H. (1976). Transitional phenomena in early language acquisition. JChLang 3. 1328.Google Scholar
Eguchi, S. & Hirsch, I. J. (1969). Development of speech sounds in children. AcOtolaryng 257. 751.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. & Farwell, C. B. (1975). Words and sounds in early language acquisition. Lg 51. 419–39.Google Scholar
Gilbert, J. H. V. & Purves, B. A. (1977). Temporal constraints on consonant clusters in child speech production. JChLang 4. 417–32.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. & Deschamps, A. (1972). Analyse des variables temporelles du francais spontané. Phonetica 26. 129–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grundstrom, A. (1973). L'intonation des questions en français standard. StudPhon 8. Didier.Google Scholar
Hawkins, S. (1979). The control of timing in children's speech. Proc. IX Int. Cong. Phon. Sci., Copenhagen. Vol. 11. 142–9.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1941). Kindersprache, Aphasie, und allgemeine Lautgesetze. Uppsala: Almqvist & Wiksell.Google Scholar
Keating, P. & Kubaska, C. (1978). Variation in the duration of words. JAcS 63. Suppl. 1. 56(A).Google Scholar
Kewley-Port, D. & Preston, M. (1974). Early apical stop production: a voice onset time analysis. JPhon 2. 195210.Google Scholar
Konopczynski, G. (1976). Etude expérimentale de quelques structures prosodiques employées par les enfants français entre 7 et 22 mois. Travaux de l'lnstitut de Phonetique de Strasbourg 7. 171205.Google Scholar
Naeser, M. A. (1970). The American child's acquisition of differential vowel duration. University of Wisconsin: Tech. Report 144.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1975). The nominal shift in semantic-syntactic development. CogPsych 7. 461–79.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. E. (1978). Children's language, Vol. 1. New York: Gardner Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. E. (1979). Children's language, Vol. II. New York: Gardner Press.Google Scholar
Oiler, D. K. & Smith, B. L. (1977). The effect of final-syllable position on vowel duration in infant babbling. JAcS 62. 994–7.Google Scholar
Oiler, D. K., Wieman, L. A., Doyle, W. J. & Ross, C. (1976). Infant babbling and speech. JChLang 3. 111.Google Scholar
Peters, A. (1977). Language learning strategies: does the whole equal the sum of the parts? Lg 3. 4962.Google Scholar
Rigault, A. (1962). Rôle de la fréquence, de l'intensité et de la durée vocalique dans la perception de l'accent en français. Proc. IV Int. Cong. Phon. Sci., Helsinki, 735–48.Google Scholar
Shvachkin, N. K. (1948). The development of phoneme speech perception in early childhood. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (eds), Studies in child language development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Smith, B. L. (1978). Temporal aspects of English speech production: a developmental perspective. JPhon 6. 137–67.Google Scholar
Waterson, N. (1971). Child phonology, a prosodic view. JL 6. 179211.Google Scholar
Weir, R. (1966). Some questions on the child's learning of phonology. In Smith, F. & Miller, G. (eds), The genesis of language, Cambridge, Mass: M.I.T.Google Scholar