Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T22:54:10.281Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Testing language acquisition models: null and overt topics in Mandarin

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 April 2019

Jingtao ZHU*
Affiliation:
Departament de Filologia Catalana, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain ClicAsia, Centre d'Estudis Orientals, Barcelona, Spain
Anna GAVARRÓ
Affiliation:
Departament de Filologia Catalana, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain
*
*Corresponding author. ClicAsia, Centre d'Estudis Orientals, Carrer de València, 359, 5è 2a– 6è2a, 08009, Barcelona, Spain. Tel.: (+34 )935143379; E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Parameter setting is either precipitous (Gibson & Wexler, 1994) or it is gradual in response to input frequency (Yang, 2002, 2004). In this study, we compare these models against the empirical domain of subject and (direct) object drop in Mandarin. We conducted a corpus-based study of the speech of 47 Mandarin-speaking children aged 1;2–6;5, and their caregivers, from the CHILDES database. The results show that before age 1;8 all the children used null subjects and null objects in a target-like fashion, which reveals that the parameter that governs null topics is set from very early on, even if the presence of disambiguating evidence for [+Null Topic] patterns is low. Besides, children's ba constructions, which require an overt object, reliably included this object from the first occurrence although its frequency was scarce in the input. Our results indicate that the setting of certain parameters occurred early independently of the input.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abalada, S. (2013). Acquisition of the left and right peripheries in European Portuguese. In Stavrakaki, S., Konstatinopoulou, P., & Lalioti, M. (Eds.), Advances in language acquisition: Proceedings of GALA 2011 (pp. 413). Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
Adger, D. (2014). Variability and grammatical architecture. In Picallo, M. C. (Ed.), Linguistic variation in the minimalist framework (pp. 179–96). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Badan, L. (2008). Preposed object and low periphery in Mandarin Chinese. Working Papers on Language and Cognition (StiL CISCL) 2, 1841.Google Scholar
Baker, M. (2008). The macroparameter in a microparametric world. In Biberauer, T. (Ed.) The limits of variation (pp. 351–374). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Belletti, A. (2001). ‘Inversion’ as Focalization. In Hulk, A. & Pollock, J. Y. (Eds.), Inversion in Romance and the theory of Universal Grammar (pp. 6090). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Belletti, A. (2004). Aspects of the low IP area. In Rizzi, L. (Ed.), The structure of IP and CP: the cartography of syntactic structures, Vol. 2 (pp. 1651). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Biberauer, T., & Roberts, I. (2016). Emergent parameters and syntactic complexity: new perspectives. Paper presented at the 49th Annual Meeting of the Societas Linguistica Europaea, Università degli Studi di Napoli Federico II, Naples.Google Scholar
Boeckx, C. (2014). What principles and parameters got wrong. In Picallo, M. C. (Ed.), Linguistic variation in the minimalist framework (pp. 155–78). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Borer, H. (1984). Parametric syntax: case studies in Semitic and Romance languages. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Chang, C. (1998). The development of autonomy in preschool Mandarin Chinese-speaking children's play narratives. Narrative Inquiry, 8(1), 77111.Google Scholar
Chao, Y. R. (1968). A grammar of spoken Chinese. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Chen, J. (2008). The acquisition of verb compounding in Mandarin. Doctoral dissertation. Nijmegen & Amsterdam: Max-Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics & Free University of Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Cheung, H., Chang, C.-J., Ko, H.-W., & Tsay, J. (2011). Taiwan Corpus of Child Mandarin (TCCM). (NSC 96-2420-H-002-030).Google Scholar
Cheung, L. Y.-L. (2008). First language acquisition of elliptical structures in Cantonese. In H. Chan, E. Kapia, & H. Jacob (Eds.), BUCLD 32 Online Proceedings Supplement. Retrieved from <http://www.bu.edu/bucld/proceedings/supplement/vol32/>..>Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on government and binding: the Pisa lectures. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
De Cat, C. (2000). Structure building and the acquisition of dislocations in child French. In Howell, C., Fish, S. A., & Keith-Lucas, T. (Eds.), Language acquisition and development: Proceedings of BUCLD 24 (pp. 242–52). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
De Cat, C. (2008). Experimental evidence for preschoolers’ mastery of topics. In Gavarró, A., & Freitas, M. J. (Eds.), Proceedings of GALA 2007 (pp. 155–65). Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
Demuth, K. (1996). Collecting spontaneous production data. In McDaniel, D., McKee, C., & Cairns, H. S. (Eds.), Methods for assessing children's syntax (pp. 322). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Deng, X., & Yip, V. (2018). A multimedia corpus of child Mandarin: the Tong corpus. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 46(1), 6992.Google Scholar
Friedmann, N., & Costa, J. (2011). Acquisition of SV and VS order in Hebrew, European Portuguese, Palestinian Arabic, and Spanish. Language Acquisition, 18(1), 138.Google Scholar
Gavarró, A., & Cabré, Y. (2009). Subjects, verb classes and word order in child Catalan. In Grinstead, J. (Ed.), Perspectives on typical and atypical Hispanic language development (pp. 175–94). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Gavarró, A., Lewandowski, W., & Markova, A. (2010). An approach to multiple interrogatives in child Bulgarian and Polish. In Costa, J., Castro, A., Lobo, M., & Pratas, F. (Eds.), Language acquisition and development: Proceedings of Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition 2009 (pp. 170–81). Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
Gibson, E., & Wexler, K. (1994). Triggers. Linguistic Inquiry, 25, 407–54.Google Scholar
Guasti, M. T., Gavarró, A., De Lange, J., & Caprin, C. (2008). Article omission across child languages. Language Acquisition, 15(2), 89119.Google Scholar
Holmberg, A., & Roberts, I. (2014). Parameters and the three factors of language design. In Picallo, M. C. (Ed.), Linguistic variation in the minimalist framework (pp. 6181). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Huang, J. (1984a). On the distribution and reference of empty pronouns. Linguistic Inquiry, 15(4), 531–74.Google Scholar
Huang, J. (1984b). Phrase structure, lexical integrity, and Chinese compounds. Journal of Chinese Language Teachers Association, 19, 5378.Google Scholar
Huang, J. (1989). Pro-drop in Chinese: a generalized control theory. In Jaeggli, O. & Safir, K. (Eds.), The null subject parameter (pp. 185214). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Huang, J., Li, A., & Li, Y. F. (2009). The syntax of Chinese. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jones, B. M. (1999). The Welsh answering system. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. (2010). Brain mechanisms in early language acquisition. Neuron Review, 67(5), 713–27.Google Scholar
Labov, W. (1981). Resolving the neogrammarian controversy. Language, 57, 267308.Google Scholar
Lacerda, R. (2017). Information structure in child English: contrastive topicalization and the dative alternation. Paper presented at the Boston University Conference on Language Development 41, Boston.Google Scholar
Lassotta, R., Omaki, A., & Franck, J. (2015). Abstract knowledge of non-canonical word order by age 2. Paper presented at the Boston University Conference on Language Development 39, Boston.Google Scholar
Lee, H. C. (2005). On Chinese focus and cleft constructions. Doctoral dissertation, Taiwan: National Tsing Hua University.Google Scholar
Lee, C., & Cho, S. W. (2009). Acquisition of the subject and topic nominals and markers in the spontaneous speech of young children in Korean. In Lee, C., Simpson, G. B. & Kim, Y. (Eds.), The handbook of East Asian psycholinguistics, vol. III (pp. 2333). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Legate, J. A., & Yang, C. (2007). Morphosyntactic learning and the development of tense. Language Acquisition, 14(3), 315–44.Google Scholar
Li, C., & Thompson, S. (1981). Mandarin Chinese: a functional reference grammar. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Li, Y. H. (2006). Chinese ba. In Everaert, M. & Van Riemsdijk, H. (Eds.), The Blackwell companion to syntax, vol. I (pp. 374468). Malden: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Liu, H. Y. (2016). Also introduing arguments: the Mandarin ba construction. Paper presented at the Meeting of the Canadian Linguistic Association, Calgary.Google Scholar
Lorusso, P., Caprin, C., & Guasti, M.-T. (2005). Overt subject distribution in early Italian children. In Brugos, A., Clark-Cotton, M. R., & Ha, S. (Eds.), Supplement: Proceedings of the 29th BUCLD. Retrieved from <http://www.bu.edu/bucld/files/2011/05/29-LorussoBUCLD2004.pdf>..>Google Scholar
, S. X. (1980). Xiandai Hanyu Babai Ci [800 words of contemporary Chinese]. Beijing: The Commercial Press.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2015). The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk. Retrieved from <https://talkbank.org>..>Google Scholar
Packard, J. (2000). The morphology of Chinese: a linguistic and cognitive approach. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Phillips, B. (1984). Word frequency and the actuation of sound change. Language, 60(2), 320–42.Google Scholar
Pierce, A. (1989). On the emergence of syntax: a crosslinguistic study. Doctoral dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Reinhart, T. (1982). Pragmatics and linguistics: an analysis of sentence topics. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
Rische, J., & Komarova, N. (2015). Regularization of languages by adults and children: a mathematical framework. Cognitive Psychology, 84, 130.Google Scholar
Roberts, I. (2016). Null subject parameters and determiner parameters. Talk presented at the 1st SynCart workshop: From maps to principles: Syntactic cartography and locality. Chiusi.Google Scholar
Roberts, I., & Holmberg, A. (2010). Introduction. In Biberauer, T., Holmberg, A., Roberts, I., & Sheehan, M. (Eds.), Parametric variation: null subjects in minimalist theory (pp. 156). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Roeper, T. (2007). What frequency can do and what it can't. In Gülzow, I. & Gagarina, N. (Eds.), Frequency effects in language acquisition: defining the limits of frequency as an explanatory concept (pp. 2350). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Sakas, W. G. (2016). Computational approaches to parameter setting in generative linguistics. In Lidz, J., Snyder, W., & Pater, J. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of developmental linguistics (pp. 696724). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Santos, A. L. (2004). How do children say ‘yes’ in European Portuguese? In Van Kampen, J. & Baauw, S. (Eds.), Proceedings of Generative Approaches for Language Acquisition 2003, vol. 2. (pp. 433–43). Utrecht: LOT Occasional Series.Google Scholar
Santos, A. L. (2009) Minimal answers: ellipsis, syntax and discourse in the acquisition of Portuguese. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Sugisaki, K. (2009). The acquisition of argument ellipsis in Japanese: a preliminary study. Nanzan Linguistics, 5, 6173.Google Scholar
Teng, S. H. (1979). Remarks on cleft sentences in Chinese. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 7, 101–12.Google Scholar
Thornton, R., & Tesan, G. (2007). Categorical acquisition: parameter-setting in Universal Grammar. Biolinguistics, 1, 4998.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tsao, F. F. (1987). A topic–comment approach to the ba construction. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 15, 154.Google Scholar
Wang, Q., Lillo-Martin, D., Best, C., & Levitt, A. (1992). Null subject versus null object: some evidence from the acquisition of Chinese and English. Language Acquisition, 2(3), 221–54.Google Scholar
Westergaard, M. (2009). The acquisition of word order: micro-cues, information structure and economy. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Wexler, K. (2011). Grammatical computation in the optional infinitive stage. In De Villiers, J. & Roeper, T. (Eds.), Handbook of generative approaches to language acquisition (pp. 53118). Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Yang, C. (2002). Knowledge and learning in natural language. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Yang, C. (2004). Universal grammar, statistics, or both? Trends in Cognitive Science, 8(10), 451–6.Google Scholar
Yang, C. (2011). Computational models of language acquisition. In De Villiers, J. & Roeper, T. (Eds.), Handbook of generative approaches to language acquisition (pp. 119–54). Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Yang, C. (2012). Computational models of syntactic acquisition. Cognitive Science, 3, 205–13.Google Scholar
Yang, C. (2013). Who's afraid of George Kingsley Zipf? Or: Do children and chimps have language? Significance: Magazine of the The Royal Statistical Society, December, 29–34.Google Scholar
Yang, C. (2016). The price of productivity. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Zhou, J. (2001). Pragmatic development of Mandarin speaking young children: from 14 months to 32 months. Doctoral dissertation, The University of Hong Kong. Available from HKU Thesis Online (HKUTO).Google Scholar
Zhou, P. (2014). Children's knowledge of ellipsis constructions in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 43, 421–45.Google Scholar
Zhuo, J. S. (2005). Dramatized discourse: the Mandarin Chinese ba-construction. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar