Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-29T20:19:49.089Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Segmental distribution patterns of English infant- and adult-directed speech*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 July 2009

SUE ANN S. LEE*
Affiliation:
Oklahoma State University
BARBARA L. DAVIS
Affiliation:
University of Texas
*
Address for correspondence: Sue Ann S. Lee, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee – Communication Sciences and Disorders, PO BOX 413, Milwaukee, Wisconsin 53201, United States. e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This study compared segmental distribution patterns for consonants and vowels in English infant-directed speech (IDS) and adult-directed speech (ADS). A previous study of Korean indicated that segmental patterns of IDS differed from ADS patterns (Lee, Davis & MacNeilage, 2008). The aim of the current study was to determine whether such differences in Korean are universal or language-specific. Results indicate that consonant distribution patterns of English IDS were significantly different from English ADS. Speakers who used IDS produced fewer fricatives, affricates, nasals and liquids, but more stops and glides, than speakers who used ADS. In terms of vowels, IDS speakers produced more high-back vowels /u Ʊ/ and /ɔI/ diphthongs than ADS speakers. These results indicate both general trends and language-specific segmental distribution patterns in IDS. When compared to previous findings on ADS and IDS in Korean, these results for English give support to a more general assertion that segmental distribution patterns in IDS seem to be mediated by linguistic and cultural factors across languages.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

We would like to thank Dr Nadya Foaud and Dr Susan Cashin, who provided valuable input on this article. We also thank Kendra Gefke, Ali Dammeir and Jaime Moore for their assistance with data collection and data analysis.

References

REFERENCES

Bernstein Ratner, N. (1984). Phonological rule usage in mother-child speech. Journal of Phonetics 12, 245–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Tal, T., Rahn, C., Galperin, C. Z., Pecheux, M., Lamour, M., Toda, S., Azuma, H., Orino, M. & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (1992). Functional analysis of the contents of maternal speech to infants of 5 and 13 months in four cultures: Argentina, France, Japan, and the United States. Developmental Psychology 28, 593603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boysson-Bardies, B. & Vihman, M. M. (1991). Adaptation to language: Evidence from babbling and first words in four languages. Language 67(2), 297319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cross, T. G. (1977). Mothers' speech adjustment: The contributions of selected child listener variable. In Snow, K. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition, 151–88. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Davis, B. L. & MacNeilage, P. F. (1995). The articulatory basis of babbling. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 38, 1199–211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dinnsen, D. A., Chin, S. B., Elbert, M. & Powell, T. (1990). Some constraints on functionally disordered phonologies: Phonetic inventories and phonotactics. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 33, 2837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dyson, A. T. (1988). Phonetic inventories of 2- and 3-year-old children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 53, 8993.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferguson, C. A. (1977). Baby talk as a simplified register. In Snow, K. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition, 219–36. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. & Morikawa, H. (1993). Common themes and cultural variations in Japanese and American mothers' speech to infants. Child Development 64(3), 637–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerometta, J. & Shafer, V. (2008). Frequent word analysis of infant directed speech across communication contexts. Poster session presented at the 2008 American Speech Language and Hearing Association Convention, Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
Hare, G. (1983). Development at 2 years. In Irwin, J. V. & Wong, S. P. (eds), Phonological development in children: 18 to 72 months, 5588. Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Kent, R. D. (1992). The biology of phonological development. In Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L. & Stoel-Gammon, C. (eds), Phonological development: Model, research, implications, 6590. Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Kent, R. D. & Bauer, H. R. (1985). Vocalizations of one-year olds. Journal of Child Language 12, 491526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, S. (2003). The phonetic basis of early speech acquisition. Unpublished doctoral Dissertation, The University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
Lee, S. & Nakayama, M. (2000). Characteristics of maternal speech in Korean: Do Korean and Japanese maternal speech show similar characteristics? Proceedings of the Annual Boston University Conference of Language Development 24, 486–97.Google Scholar
Lee, S., Davis, B. & MacNeilage, P. (2008). Segmental properties of input to infants: A study of Korean. Journal of Child Language 35, 591617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, S., Davis, B. & MacNeilage, P. (in press). Universal production patterns and ambient language influences in babbling: A cross-linguistic study of Korean and English-learning infants. Journal of Child Language.Google Scholar
Levitt, A. G. & Aydelott Utman, J. G. (1992). From babbling towards the sound systems of English and French: A longitudinal two-case studies. Journal of Child Language 19, 1949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, J. L. (1983). Phonological acquisition and change. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
MacKen, M. & Barton, D. (1980). The acquisition of the voicing contrast in English: A study of Voice Onset Time in word-initial stop consonants. Journal of Child Language 7, 4174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ménard, L., Schwartz, J. & Boë, J. (2004). Role of vocal tract morphology in speech development: Perceptual targets and sensorimotor maps for synthesized French vowels for birth to adulthood. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 47, 1059–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paschall, L. (1983). Development at 18 months. In Irwin, J. V. & Wong, S. P. (eds), Phonological development in children: 18 to 72 months, 2754. Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Phillips, J. (1973). Syntax and vocabulary of mothers' speech to young children: age and sex comparison. Child Development 44, 182–85.Google Scholar
Pine, J. (1994). The language of primary caregivers. In Gallaway, C. & Richards, B. J. (eds), Input and interaction in language acquisition, 1537. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rvachew, S., Alhaidary, A., Mattock, K. & Polka, L. (2008). Emergence of the corner vowels in the babble produced infants exposed to Canadian English or Canadian French. Journal of Phonetics 36, 564–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sachs, J. (1977). The adaptive significance of linguistic input to prelinguistic infants. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition, 5161. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N. & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science 274, 1926–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shockey, L. & Bond, Z. S. (1980). Phonological processes in speech addressed to children. Phonetica 37, 267–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. (1972). Mothers' speech to children learning language. Child Development 43, 549–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. (1984). Phonological variability in mother–child speech. Phonetica 41, 208214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. (1985). Phonetic inventories, 15–24 months: A longitudinal study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 28, 505512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoel-Gammon, C. & Harrington, P. (1990). Vowel systems of normally developing and phonologically disordered children. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 4, 145–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suh, S. (1991). Constituent structure processing in Korean. Proceedings of Eastern States Conference on Linguistics 8, 347–58.Google Scholar
Sundberg, U. (1998). Mother tongue-phonetic aspects of infant-directed speech. Doctoral dissertation, PERILUS XXI, University of Stockholm.Google Scholar
Teixeira, E. R. & Davis, B. L. (2002). Early sound patterns in the speech of two Brazilian Portuguese speakers. Language and Speech 45(2), 179204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toda, S., Fogel, A. & Kawai, M. (1990). Maternal speech to three-month-old infants in the United States and Japan. Journal of Child Language 17, 279–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vihman, M. M., Kay, E., Boysson-Bardies, B., Durand, C. & Sundberg, U. (1994). External sources of individual differences? A cross-linguistic analysis of the phonetics of mothers' speech to one-year-old children. Developmental Psychology 30(5), 651–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Interaction between Learning and Development. In Cole, M., John-Steiner, V., Scribner, S. & Souberman, E. (eds), Mind in Society: The development of higher psychological processes, 7991. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Weijer, J. van de (2002). How much does an infant hear in a day? Proceedings of the GALA2001 Conference on Language Acquisition, 279–82. Lisboa: Associação Portuguesa de Linguistíca.Google Scholar
Zamuner, T. S. (2003). Input-based phonological acquisition. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar