We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
References
REFERENCES
Hubel, D. H. & Wiesel, T. N. (1962). Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. JPhysiol160. 106–54.Google ScholarPubMed
Lettvin, J. Y., Maturana, H. R., McCulloch, W. S. & Pitts, W. H. (1959). What the frog's eye tells the frog's brain. ProcInstRadioEngr47. 1940–1951.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1978). The acquisition of morphophonology. Monogr. Soc. Res. Ch. Devel.43 (Nos. 1–2).Google Scholar
Minsky, M. & Papert, S. (1969). Perceptrons: an introduction to computational geometry. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Partee, B. H. (1981). Montague grammar, mental representations, and reality. In Kanger, S. & Öhman, S. (eds), Philosophy and grammar: papers on the occasion of the quincentennial of Uppsala University. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Richards, M. P. M. (ed.) (1974). The integration of a child into a social world. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar