Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T11:06:59.189Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Reciprocal influences between maternal language and children's language and cognitive development in low-income families*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 January 2013

LULU SONG
Affiliation:
Brooklyn College of the City University of New York, USA
ELIZABETH T. SPIER
Affiliation:
American Institutes for Research, California, USA
CATHERINE S. TAMIS-LEMONDA*
Affiliation:
New York University, USA
*
Address for correspondence: Catherine S. Tamis-LeMonda, 246 Greene Street, Floor 5E, New York, NY 10003. e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

We examined reciprocal associations between early maternal language use and children's language and cognitive development in seventy ethnically diverse, low-income families. Mother–child dyads were videotaped when children were aged 2;0 and 3;0. Video transcripts were analyzed for quantity and lexical diversity of maternal and child language. Child cognitive development was assessed at both ages and child receptive vocabulary was assessed at age 3;0. Maternal language related to children's lexical diversity at each age, and maternal language at age 2;0, was associated with children's receptive vocabulary and cognitive development at age 3;0. Furthermore, children's cognitive development at age 2;0 was associated with maternal language at age 3;0 controlling for maternal language at age 2;0, suggesting bi-directionality in mother–child associations. The quantity and diversity of the language children hear at home has developmental implications for children from low-income households. In addition, children's early cognitive skills further feed into their subsequent language experiences.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

We wish to acknowledge our colleagues in the Early Head Start (EHS) Research Consortium, including the Administration on Children, Youth and Families (ACF) and evaluation contractor (Mathematica Policy Research). We acknowledge funding by National Science Foundation Behavioral and Cognitive Sciences Grant 021859 and National Science Foundation Integrative Research Activities for Developmental Science Grant 0721383, as well as funding from New York University's Provost Office to support Lulu Song's postdoctoral fellowship at the Center for Research on Culture, Development, and Education. Lulu Song is currently at Brooklyn College, City University of New York.

References

REFERENCES

Arriaga, R. I., Fenson, L., Cronan, T. & Pethick, S. J. (1998). Scores on the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory of children from low- and middle-income families. Applied Psycholinguistics 19, 209223.Google Scholar
Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley Scales of Infant Development, 2nd ed.San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Black, M. M., Hess, C. R. & Berenson-Howard, J. (2000). Toddlers from low-income families have below normal mental, motor, and behavior scores on the revised Bayley Scales. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 21, 655–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (1989). Maternal responsiveness and cognitive development in children. New Directions for Child Development 43, 4961.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks-Gunn, J. & Duncan, G. J. (1997). The effects of poverty on children. Future of Children 7, 5571.Google Scholar
Burchinal, M. R., Campbell, F. A., Bryant, D. M., Wasik, B. H. & Ramey, C. T. (1997). Early intervention and mediating processes in cognitive performance of children of low-income African American families. Child Development 68, 935–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carle, E. (1994). The very hungry caterpillar. New York: Putnam.Google Scholar
Champion, T. B., Hyter, Y. D., McCabe, A. & Bland-Stewart, L. M. (2003). A matter of vocabulary: performances of low-income African American Head Start children on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III. Communication Disorders Quarterly 24, 121–27.Google Scholar
Clark, E. (2004). How language acquisition builds on cognitive development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8, 472–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L. M. & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – III. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. & Pethick, S. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 59, (Serial No. 242).Google Scholar
Fernald, L. C. H., Weber, A., Galasso, E. & Ratsifandrihamanana, L. (2011). Socioeconomic gradients and child development in a very low income population: evidence from Madagascar. Developmental Science 14, 832–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forget-Dubois, N., Dionne, G., Lemelin, J., Pérusse, D., Tremblay, R. E. & Boivin, M. (2009). Early child language mediates the relation between home environment and school readiness. Child Development 80, 736–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuller, B., Bridges, M., Bein, E., Jang, H., Jung, S., Rabe-Hesketh, S., Halfon, N. & Kuo, A. (2009). The health and cognitive growth of Latino toddlers: at risk or immigrant paradox? Maternal and Child Health Journal 13, 755–68.Google Scholar
Göksun, T., Hirsh-Pasek, K. & Golinkoff, R. M. (2010). Trading spaces: carving up events for learning language. Perspectives on Psychological Science 5, 3342.Google Scholar
Harlaar, N., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Dale, P. S. & Plomin, R. (2008). Why do preschool language abilities correlate with later reading? A twin study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 51, 688705.Google Scholar
Hart, B. & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Brookes.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development 74, 1368–78.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review 26, 5588.Google Scholar
Hurtado, N., Marchman, V. A. & Fernald, A. (2008). Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science 11, F31F39.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M. & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology 27, 236–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E. & Levine, S. (2002). Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology 45, 337–74.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J. & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cognitive Psychology 61, 343–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karasik, L., Celano, E., Tamis-LeMonda, C. & Adolph, K. (2012). Mothers' response to infant object sharing. Paper presented at the XVIII Biennial International Conference on Infant Studies Minneapolis, Minnesota, June.Google Scholar
Keown, L. J., Woodward, L. J. & Field, J. (2001). Language development of pre-school children born to teenage mothers. Infant and Child Development 10, 129–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landry, S. H., Miller-Loncar, C. L., Smith, K. E. & Swank, P. R. (2002). The role of early parenting in children's development of executive processes. Developmental Neuropsychology 21, 1541.Google Scholar
Lugo-Gil, J. & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2008). Family resources and parenting quality: links to children's cognitive development across the first 3 years. Child Development 79, 1065–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk, 3rd ed.Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McDonough, L., Choi, S. & Mandler, J. M. (2003). Spatial categorization: flexible infants, lexical adults. Cognitive Psychology 46, 229–59.Google Scholar
McKee, G., Malvern, D. & Richards, B. (2000). Measuring vocabulary diversity using dedicated software. Literary and Linguistic Computing 15, 223–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendelsohn, A. L., Valdez, P. T., Flynn, V., Foley, G. M., Berkule, S. B., Tomopoulos, S., Fierman, A. H., Tineo, W. & Dreyer, B. (2007). Use of videotaped interactions during pediatric well-child care: impact at 33 months on parenting and on child development. Journal of Developmental Behavioral Pediatrics 28, 206212.Google Scholar
Murray, A. D. & Hornbaker, A. V. (1997). Maternal directive and facilitative interaction styles: associations with language and cognitive development of low risk and high risk toddlers. Development and Psychopathology 9, 507516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nazzi, T. & Gopnik, A. (2001). Linguistic and cognitive abilities in infancy: When does language become a tool for categorization? Cognition 80, B11B20.Google Scholar
Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D. & Snow, C. E. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development 76, 763–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pancsofar, N. & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2010). Fathers' early contributions to children's language development in families from low-income rural communities. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 25, 450–63.Google Scholar
Plomin, R., Fulker, D. W., Corley, R. & DeFries, J. C. (1997). Nature, nurture, and cognitive development from 1 to 16 years: a parent–offspring adoption study. Psychological Science 8, 442–47.Google Scholar
Pruden, S. M., Levine, S. C. & Huttenlocher, J. (2011). Children's spatial thinking: Does talk about the spatial world matter? Developmental Science 14, 1417–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raikes, H., Luze, G., Brooks-Gunn, J., Raikes, H. A., Pan, B. A., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Constantine, J., Tarullo, L. B. & Rodriguez, E. T. (2006). Mother–child bookreading in low-income families: correlates and outcomes during the first three years of life. Child Development 77(4), 924–53.Google Scholar
Ramey, C. T. & Ramey, S. L. (2004). Early learning and school readiness: Can early intervention make a difference? Merrill Palmer Quarterly 50, 471–91.Google Scholar
Reilly, S., Wake, M., Bavin, E. L., Prior, M., Williams, J., Bretherton, L., Eadie, P., Barrett, Y. & Ukoumunne, O. C. (2007). Predicting language at 2 years of age: a prospective community study. Pediatrics 120, e1441e1449.Google Scholar
Roberts, J. E., Burchinal, M. & Durham, M. (1999). Parents' report of vocabulary and grammatical development of African American preschoolers: child and environmental associations. Child Development 70, 92106.Google Scholar
Rowe, M. L. (2008). Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language 35, 185205.Google Scholar
Sameroff, A. (2010). A unified theory of development: a dialectic integration of nature and nurture. Child Development 81, 622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shannon, J. D., Tamis-LeMonda, C., London, K. & Cabrera, N. (2002). Beyond rough and tumble: low-income fathers' interactions and children's cognitive development at 24 months. Parenting: Science, and Practice 2, 77104.Google Scholar
Shonkoff, J. & Phillips, D. (eds.) (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: the science of early childhood development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1972). Mothers' speech to children learning language. Child Development 43, 549–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language 4, 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Song, L., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Yoshikawa, H., Kahana-Kalman, R. & Wu, I. (2012). Language experiences and vocabulary development in Dominican and Mexican infants across the first 2 years. Developmental Psychology 48, 1106–123.Google Scholar
Stromswold, K. (2001). The heritability of language: a review and meta-analysis of twin, adoption, and linkage studies. Language 77, 647723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., Baumwell, L. & Damast, A. M. (1996). Responsive parenting in the second year: specific influences on children's language and play. In Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. guest editor, Parenting sensitivity: individual, contextual and cultural factors in recent conceptualizations, thematic issue of Early Development and Parenting 5, 173–83.Google Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Shannon, J. D., Cabrera, N. J. & Lamb, M. E. (2004). Fathers and mothers at play with their 2- and 3-year-olds: contributions to language and cognitive development. Child Development 75, 1806–820.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: the development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Xu, F. (2002). The role of language in acquiring object kind concepts in infancy. Cognition 85, 223–50.Google Scholar