Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:58:24.542Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Rapid learning of minimally different words in five- to six-year-old children: effects of acoustic salience and hearing impairment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 May 2015

MARCEL R. GIEZEN*
Affiliation:
Laboratory for Language and Cognitive Neuroscience, San Diego State University
PAOLA ESCUDERO
Affiliation:
MARCS Institute, University of Western Sydney
ANNE E. BAKER
Affiliation:
University of Amsterdam, Department of Linguistics
*
Address for correspondence: Marcel Giezen, Laboratory for Language and Cognitive Neuroscience, 6495 Alvarado Road #200, San Diego, CA 92120. tel.: 619-594-1355; e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This study investigates the role of acoustic salience and hearing impairment in learning phonologically minimal pairs. Picture-matching and object-matching tasks were used to investigate the learning of consonant and vowel minimal pairs in five- to six-year-old deaf children with a cochlear implant (CI), and children of the same age with normal hearing (NH). In both tasks, the CI children showed clear difficulties with learning minimal pairs. The NH children also showed some difficulties, however, particularly in the picture-matching task. Vowel minimal pairs were learned more successfully than consonant minimal pairs, particularly in the object-matching task. These results suggest that the ability to encode phonetic detail in novel words is not fully developed at age six and is affected by task demands and acoustic salience. CI children experience persistent difficulties with accurately mapping sound contrasts to novel meanings, but seem to benefit from the relative acoustic salience of vowel sounds.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J. & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 390412.Google Scholar
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C. & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language 68, 255278.Google Scholar
Bates, D. M., Maechler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. (2013). lme4: linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1–7.Google Scholar
Beckman, M. E. & Edwards, J. (2000). The ontogeny of phonological categories and the primacy of lexical learning in linguistic development. Child Development 71, 240249.Google Scholar
Bergeson, T. R., Pisoni, D. B. & Davis, R. O. A. (2005). Development of audiovisual comprehension skills in prelingually deaf children with cochlear implants. Ear and Hearing 26, 149164.Google Scholar
Bonatti, L. L., Peña, M., Nespor, M. & Mehler, J. (2005). Linguistic constraints on statistical computations: the role of consonants and vowels in continuous speech processing. Psychological Science 16, 451459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouton, S., Serniclaes, W., Bertoncini, J. & Colé, P. (2012). Perception of speech features by French-speaking children with cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 55, 139153.Google Scholar
Bowey, J. A. & Hirakis, E. (2006). Testing the protracted lexical restructuring hypothesis: the effects of position and acoustic-phonetic clarity on sensitivity to mispronunciations in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 95, 117.Google Scholar
Carreiras, M., Duñabeitia, J. A. & Molinaro, N. (2009). Consonants and vowels contribute differently to visual word recognition: ERPs of relative position priming. Cerebral Cortex 19, 26592670.Google Scholar
Curtin, S., Fennell, C. T. & Escudero, P. (2009). Weighting of vowel cues explains patterns of word–object associative learning. Developmental Science 12, 725731.Google Scholar
Cutler, A., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Soler-Vilageliu, O. & Van Ooijen, B. (2000). Constraints of vowels and consonants on lexical selection: cross-linguistic comparisons. Memory and Cognition 28, 746755.Google Scholar
Davidson, L. S., Geers, A. E., Blamey, P. J., Tobey, E. A. & Brenner, C. (2011). Factors contributing to speech perception scores in long-term pediatric cochlear implant users. Ear and Hearing 32, 1926.Google Scholar
Davidson, L. S., Geers, A. E. & Nicholas, J. G. (2014). The effects of audibility and novel word learning ability on vocabulary level in children with cochlear implants. Cochlear Implants International 15, 211221.Google Scholar
Dietrich, C., Swingley, D. & Werker, J. F. (2007). Native language governs interpretation of salient speech sound differences at 18 months. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104, 1602716031.Google Scholar
Duyck, W., Desmet, T., Verbeke, L. P. C. & Brysbaert, M. (2004). WordGen: a tool for word selection and nonword generation in Dutch, English, German, and French. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers 36, 488499.Google Scholar
Escudero, P., Mulak, K. & Vlach, H. (2015). Cross-situational learning of minimal word pairs. Cognitive Science, online: <doi:10.1111/cogs.12243>.Google Scholar
Fagan, M. K. & Pisoni, D. B. (2010). Hearing experience and receptive vocabulary development in deaf children with cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 15, 149161.Google Scholar
Garlock, V. M., Walley, A. C. & Metsala, J. L. (2001). Age-of-acquisition, word frequency, and neighborhood density effects on spoken word recognition by children and adults: implications for the development of phoneme awareness and early reading ability. Journal of Memory and Language 45, 468492.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: the nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics 27, 513543.Google Scholar
Gerken, L. A., Murphy, W. D. & Aslin, R. N. (1995). Three- and four-year-olds’ perceptual confusions for spoken words. Perception and Psychophysics 57, 475486.Google Scholar
Gerrits, E. (2001). The categorisation of speech sounds by adults and children: a study of the categorical perception hypothesis and the developmental weighting of acoustic speech cues. Utrecht: LOT Dissertation Series 42.Google Scholar
Giezen, M. R., Baker, A. E. & Escudero, P. (2014). Relationships between spoken word and sign processing in children with cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 19, 107125.Google Scholar
Giezen, M. R., Escudero, P. & Baker, A. E. (2010). Use of acoustic cues by children with cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 53, 14401457.Google Scholar
Havy, M., Bertoncini, J. & Nazzi, T. (2011). Word learning and phonetic processing in preschool-age children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 108, 2543.Google Scholar
Havy, M., Nazzi, T. & Bertoncini, J. (2013). Phonetic processing during the acquisition of new words in 3-to-6-year-old French-speaking deaf children with cochlear implants. Journal of Communication Disorders 46, 181192.Google Scholar
Hazan, V. & Barrett, S. (2000). The development of phonemic categorization in children aged 6–12. Journal of Phonetics 28, 377396.Google Scholar
Henderson, L., Weighall, A., Brown, H. & Gaskell, G. (2013). Online lexical competition during spoken word recognition and word learning in children and adults. Child Development 84, 16681685.Google Scholar
Hicks, C. B. & Ohde, R. N. (2005). Developmental role of static, dynamic, and contextual cues in speech perception. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 48, 960974.Google Scholar
Houston, D. M. & Bergeson, T. R. (2014). Hearing versus listening: attention to speech and its role in language acquisition in deaf infants with cochlear implants. Lingua 139, 1025.Google Scholar
Houston, D. M., Stewart, J., Moberly, A., Hollich, G. & Miyamoto, R. T. (2012). Word learning in deaf children with cochlear implants: effects of early auditory experience. Developmental Science 15, 448461.Google Scholar
Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 434446.Google Scholar
Jerger, S., Damian, M. F., Spence, M. J., Tye-Murray, N. & Abdi, H. (2009). Developmental shifts in children's sensitivity to visual speech: a new multimodal picture–word task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 102, 4059.Google Scholar
Kan, P. F. & Kohnert, K. (2012). A growth curve analysis of novel word learning by sequential bilingual preschool children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 15, 452469.Google Scholar
Kishon-Rabin, L., Taitelbaum, R., Muchnik, C., Gehtler, I., Kronenberg, J. & Hildesheimer, M. (2002). Development of speech perception and production in children with cochlear implants. Annals of Otology, Rhinology and Laryngology 111, 8590.Google Scholar
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B. & Bojesen, R. H. (2012). Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 2·0–20.Google Scholar
Lederberg, A. R., Spencer, P. E. & Prezbindowski, A. K. (2000). Word learning skills of deaf preschoolers: the development of novel mapping and rapid word learning strategies. Child Development 71, 15711585.Google Scholar
Mani, N. & Plunkett, K. (2008). Fourteen-month-olds pay attention to vowels in novel words. Developmental Science 11, 5359.Google Scholar
Marian, V., Bartolotti, J., Chabal, S. & Shook, A. (2012). CLEARPOND: Cross-Linguistic Easy-Access Resource for Phonological and Orthographic Neighborhood Densities. PLoS ONE 7, e43230.Google Scholar
Mayo, C. & Turk, A. (2005). The influence of spectral distinctiveness on acoustic cue weighting in children's and adults’ speech perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 118, 17301741.Google Scholar
Metsala, J. L. & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In Metsala, J. L. & Ehri, L. C. (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89120). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Nazzi, T. (2005). Use of phonetic specificity during the acquisition of new words: differences between consonants and vowels. Cognition 98, 1330.Google Scholar
Nazzi, T., Floccia, C., Moquet, B. & Butler, J. (2009). Bias for consonantal information over vocalic information in 30-month-olds: cross-linguistic evidence from French and English. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 102, 522537.Google Scholar
Nespor, M., Peña, M. & Mehler, J. (2003). On the different roles of vowels and consonants in speech processing and language acquisition. Lingue e Linguaggio ii, 221247.Google Scholar
Nittrouer, S. & Miller, M. E. (1997). Predicting developmental shifts in perceptual weighting schemes. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 101, 22532266.Google Scholar
Peterson, N. R., Pisoni, D. B. & Miyamoto, R. T. (2010). Cochlear implants and spoken language processing abilities: review and assessment of the literature. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience 28, 237250.Google Scholar
Pisoni, D. B., Cleary, M., Geers, A. E. & Tobey, E. A. (1999). Individual differences in effectiveness of cochlear implants in children who are prelingually deaf: new process measures of performance. Volta Review 101, 111164.Google Scholar
Pisoni, D. B., Conway, C. M., Kronenberger, W. G., Horn, D. L., Karpicke, J. & Henning, S. C. (2008). Efficacy and effectiveness of cochlear implants in deaf children. In Marschark, M. & Hauser, P. C. (Eds.), Deaf cognition: foundations and outcomes (pp. 52101). Oxford & New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Polka, L. & Werker, J. F. (1994). Developmental changes in perception of non-native vowel contrasts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 24, 745749.Google Scholar
R development core team (2011). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Online: < http://www.r-project.org >..>Google Scholar
Schwartz, R. G., Steinman, S., Ying, E., Mystal, E. Y. & Houston, D. M. (2013). Language processing in children with cochlear implants: a preliminary report on lexical access for production and comprehension. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 27, 264277.Google Scholar
Shatzman, K. B. & McQueen, J. M. (2006). Prosodic knowledge affects the recognition of newly acquired words. Psychological Science 17, 372377.Google Scholar
Stager, C. L. & Werker, J. F. (1997). Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word learning tasks. Nature 388, 381382.Google Scholar
Svirsky, M. A., Teoh, S.-W. & Neuburger, H. (2004). Development of language and speech perception in congenitally, profoundly deaf children as a function of age at cochlear implantation. Audiology and Neuro-Otology 9, 224233.Google Scholar
Swingley, D. & Aslin, R. N. (2002). Lexical neighborhoods and the word-form representations of 14-month-olds. Psychological Science 13, 480484.Google Scholar
Walker, E. A. & McGregor, K. K. (2013). Word learning processes in children with cochlear implants. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 56, 375387.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F. & Curtin, S. (2005). PRIMIR: a developmental framework of infant speech processing. Language Learning and Development 1, 197234.Google Scholar
Willstedt-Svensson, U., Löfqvist, A., Almqvist, B. & Sahlén, B. (2004). Is age at implant the only factor that counts? The influence of working memory on lexical and grammatical development in children with cochlear implants. International Journal of Audiology 43, 506515.Google Scholar
Woodhouse, L., Hickson, L. & Dodd, B. (2009). Review of visual speech perception by hearing and hearing-impaired people: clinical implications. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 44, 253270.Google Scholar
Xu, L. & Pfingst, B. E. (2008). Spectral and temporal cues for speech recognition: implications for auditory prostheses. Hearing Research 242, 132140.Google Scholar
Yoshida, K. A., Fennell, C. T., Swingley, D. & Werker, J. F. (2009). Fourteen-month-old infants learn similar-sounding words. Developmental Science 12, 412418.Google Scholar