Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:16:08.085Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Production as a source of input for analysis: evidence from the developmental course of a word-blend*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Loekie Elbers*
Affiliation:
Utrecht University
*
Vakgroep Psychonomie, Utrecht University, Postbus 80.140, 3508 TC Utrecht, The Netherlands.

Abstract

Theoretical arguments for considering production as a source of input for analysis (the OUTPUT-AS-INPUT hypothesis) are reviewed, and empirical evidence supporting this hypothesis is presented. The evidence consists of a longitudinal study of the developmental course of a self-created form, produced by one Dutch child. This form is the product of blending two words, wat and iets, to yield unitary wat-iets. In Dutch, independent wat and iets may each mean ‘some’ and/or ‘something’. Though wat-iets is not permitted by the language, and so does not occur in environmental input, the form stays in the child's repertoire for about 10 months (between 3;8 and 4;7) and is apparently subjected to processes of generalization: first the child treats wat-iets as a two-word frame that may be regularized, later as a unitary word that may be semantically extended. After the extension of wat-iets, independent synonymous wat and iets appear for the first time in the child's speech. It is argued that the child actually analysed his own creation.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was partly supported by a postdoctoral fellowship of the Research Institute for Language and Speech (OTS), Utrecht University. The paper is a revised and expanded version of a contribution to this Institute's 1990 yearbook (Elbers, 1991a). I am grateful to Ger de Haan for help and discussions during my stay, to the editors of the yearbook, anonymous referees and Frank Wijnen for useful comments on earlier versions of the paper, and to Anita van Loon for support and encouragement.

References

REFERENCES

Berman, R. A. (1986). A step-by-step model of language acquisition. In Levin, I. (ed.), Stage and structure: reopening the debate. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Bleile, K. M. & Tomblin, J. B. (1991). Regressions in the phonological development of two children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 20, 483–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1982). Starting to talk worse: clues to language acquisition from children's late speech errors. In Strauss, S. (ed.), U-shaped behavioral growth. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1985). Beyond communicative adequacy: from piecemeal knowledge to an integrated system in the child's acquisition of language. In Nelson, K. E. (ed.), Children's language vol. 5. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1987). Commentary: mechanisms of language acquisition. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Braine, M. D. S. (1971). On two types of models of the internalisation of grammars. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The ontogenesis of grammar. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Braine, M. D. S. (1988). Modelling the acquisition of linguistic structure. In Levy, Y., Schlesinger, I. M. & Braine, M. D. S. (eds), Categories and processes in language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cazden, C. (1968). The acquisition of noun and verb inflections. Child Development 39, 433–8.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1987). The principle of contrast: a constraint on acquisition. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Clark, R. (1977). What's the use of imitation? Journal of Child Language 4, 341–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, R. (1982). Theory and method in child language research: are we assuming too much? In Kuczaj, S. (ed.), Language development Vol. 1: Syntax and semantics. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Elbers, L. (1988 a). How children actively create their own language development: a basic cycle of cognitive operations. In Söderbergh, R. (ed.), Children's creative communication. Lund: Lund University Press.Google Scholar
Elbers, L. (1988 b). New names from old words: related aspects of children's metaphors and word compounds. Journal of Child Language 15, 591617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elbers, L. (1990). Learning language by producing language, or the synchronic relation between two- and three-word sentences. Poster presented at the 5th International Congress for the Study of Child Language, Budapest.Google Scholar
Elbers, L. (1991 a). Language acquisition: blends, overgeneralizations, and self-produced input. In Coopmans, P., Schouten, B. & Zonneveld, W. (eds), OTS yearbook 1990. Dordrecht: ICG Printing.Google Scholar
Elbers, L. (1991 b). ‘Bootstrapping’ from production: an inductive model of language acquisition. Poster presented at the Child Language Seminar, Manchester.Google Scholar
Elbers, L. & Ton, J. (1985). Play pen monologues: the interplay of words and babbles in the first words period. Journal of Child Language 12, 551–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elbers, L. & Wijnen, F. (1992). Effort, production skill, and language learning. In Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L. & Stoel-Gammon, C. (eds), Phonological development: models, research, implications. Timonium, Maryland: York Press.Google Scholar
Fay, D. (1982). Substitutions and splices: a study in sentence blends. In Cutler, A. (ed.), Slips of the tongue and language production. Berlin: Mouton.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. & Macken, M. A. (1983). The role of play in phonological development. In Nelson, K. E. (ed.), Children's languge Vol. 4. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fikkert, P. (1993). The acquisition of Dutch stress. In Verrips, M. & Wijnen, F. (eds), The acquisition of Dutch, Amsterdam Series in Child Language Development, Vol. 1, Report 60 of the Institute for General Linguistics, Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Garrett, M. F. (1980). Levels of processing in sentence production. In Butterworth, B. (ed.), Language production. Vol. 1: Speech and talk. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1983). Sound-based errors in early word use. Journal of Child Language 10, 459–64CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackendoff, R. (1987). Consciousness and the computational mind (2nd edn, 1989). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979). A functional approach to child language. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1986). From meta-processes to conscious access: evidence from children's metalinguistic and repair data. Cognition 23, 95147.Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: from intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1978). The acquisition of morphophonology. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 43, 12.Google Scholar
Marcus, G. F., Ullman, M., Pinker, S., Hollander, M., Rosen, T. J. & Xu, F. (1990). Overregularisation. Occasional Paper No. 41, MIT Center for Cognitive Science.Google Scholar
Neisser, U. (1983). Toward a skillful psychology. In Rogers, D. R. & Sloboda, J. A. (eds), The acquisition of symbolic skills. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1985). Making sense: the acquisition of shared meaning. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Peters, A. M. (1983). The units of language acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Pine, J. M. & Lieven, E. V. M. (1993). Reanalysing rote-learned phrases: individual differences in the transition to multi-word speech. Journal of Child Language 20, 551–71.Google Scholar
Platt, C. B. & MacWhinney, B. (1983). Error assimilation as a mechanism in language learning. Journal of Child Language 10, 104–14.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L. (1990). Preschoolers' QUIL: quick incidental learning of words. In Conti-Ramsden, G. & Snow, C. E. (eds), Children's language. Vol. 7. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Veneziano, E. (1992). Talking one's way out of the single-word functioning: a constructivist account of early language acquisition. Substratum 1, 79101.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. (1981). Phonology and the development of the lexicon: evidence from children's errors. Journal of Child Language 8, 239–64.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. & Miller, R. (1988). Words and babble at the threshold of language acquisition. In Smith, M. D. & Locke, J. L. (eds), The emergent lexicon: the child's development of a linguistic vocabulary. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Weir, R. H. (1962). Language in the crib. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Wexler, K. (1982). A principle theory for language acquisition. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R. (eds), Language acquisition: the state of the art. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Wijnen, F. (1990). The development of sentence planning. Journal of Child Language 17, 651–75.Google Scholar
Wong Fillmore, L. (1976). The second time around: cognitive and social strategies in second language acquisition. Doctoral dissertation, Stanford University.Google Scholar