Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-s2hrs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T06:31:57.511Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Number dissimilarities facilitate the comprehension of relative clauses in children with (Grammatical) Specific Language Impairment*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 June 2013

FLAVIA ADANI
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, University of Potsdam
MATTEO FORGIARINI
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca
MARIA TERESA GUASTI
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca
HEATHER K. J. VAN DER LELY*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Harvard University and Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université Catholique de Louvain
*
Address for correspondence: Heather K. J. van der Lely, Department of Psychology, Harvard University. William James Hall, 33 Kirkland Street, Cambridge MA, USA. e-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This study investigates whether number dissimilarities on subject and object DPs facilitate the comprehension of subject- and object-extracted centre-embedded relative clauses in children with Grammatical Specific Language Impairment (G-SLI). We compared the performance of a group of English-speaking children with G-SLI (mean age: 12;11) with that of two groups of younger typically developing (TD) children, matched on grammar and receptive vocabulary, respectively. All groups were more accurate on subject-extracted relative clauses than object-extracted ones and, crucially, they all showed greater accuracy for sentences with dissimilar number features (i.e., one singular, one plural) on the head noun and the embedded DP. These findings are interpreted in the light of current psycholinguistic models of sentence comprehension in TD children and provide further insight into the linguistic nature of G-SLI.

Type
Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - SA
The online version of this article is published within an Open Access environment subject to the conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike licence . The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013

INTRODUCTION

Children with developmental language disorders, especially those with Specific Language Impairment (SLI), are notoriously impaired in their comprehension of syntactically complex structures (Bishop, Reference Bishop1997; Leonard, Reference Leonard1998). Since at least Stark and Tallal (Reference Stark and Tallal1981), SLI has been considered a heterogeneous disorder and several scholars have identified subgroups of children with a more selective disorder in one or more language components (Conti-Ramsden & Botting, Reference Conti-Ramsden and Botting1999; Conti-Ramsden, Crutchely & Botting, Reference Conti-Ramsden, Crutchely and Botting1997; Friedmann & Novogrodsky, Reference Friedmann and Novogrodsky2011; van der Lely Reference van der Lely1996a, Reference van der Lely2005). This paper continues the investigation into the nature of the deficit in the SLI subgroup known as G(rammatical)-SLI (van der Lely, Reference van der Lely1998, Reference van der Lely2005). We are cautious in extending the linguistic characterization of G-SLI to other subgroups, yet the nature of the deficit in children with G-SLI appears to be consistent with SLI more generally in English and various other languages (Friedmann & Novogrodsky, Reference Friedmann and Novogrodsky2007; Jakubowicz & Tuller Reference Jakubowicz, Tuller and Ayoun2008; Stavrakaki & van der Lely, Reference Stavrakaki and van der Lely2010). We aim to establish whether the performance of children with G-SLI improves as a function of the morphological properties of linguistic constituents that appear in relative clauses (RC, henceforth). Specifically, we focus on number dissimilarity between subject- and object-DPs that occupy argument positions in the main and/or embedded clause.

Recent investigations of unimpaired adults (Gordon, Hendrick & Johnson, Reference Gordon, Hendrick and Johnson2001) and TD children (Adani, van der Lely, Forgiarini & Guasti, Reference Adani, van der Lely, Forgiarini and Guasti2010; Arnon, Reference Arnon2010; Friedmann, Belletti & Rizzi, Reference Friedmann, Belletti and Rizzi2009) have shown that various types of dissimilarities facilitate object-extracted RC comprehension. Turning to SLI, there is substantial cross-linguistic evidence that comprehension of RCs is impaired in these children (Contemori & Garraffa, Reference Contemori and Garraffa2010; Friedmann & Novogrodsky, Reference Friedmann and Novogrodsky2004; Håkansson & Hansson, Reference Håkansson and Hansson2000; Schuele & Nicholls, Reference Schuele and Nicholls2000; Stavrakaki, Reference Stavrakaki2001), but it is less clear whether nominal and verb morphology is also impaired (Bortolini, Leonard & Caselli, Reference Bortolini, Leonard and Caselli1998; Clahsen, Bartke & Gollner, Reference Clahsen, Bartke and Gollner1997; Leonard, McGregor & Allen, Reference Leonard, McGregor and Allen1992; Oetting & Rice, Reference Oetting and Rice1993). In this paper, we address the question: Are children with G-SLI able to use number morphology to facilitate comprehension of RCs? The answer to this question will (a) provide a more detailed characterization of which linguistic abilities are impaired or spared in G-SLI (and possibly in SLI) and (b) identify those properties that can enhance syntactic knowledge in speech and language rehabilitation. To our knowledge, our study is the first attempt to investigate the interaction of sentence complexity and morphosyntactic features in SLI. A sample of sentences under investigation is given in (1); here and throughout the paper, the position of the head noun as subject or object of the embedded verb is indicated within angled brackets ‘< >’:

  1. (1)

    1. a. The goat that <goat>is washing the cat has climbed onto the stool.

    2. b. The goat that <goat>is washing the cats has climbed onto the stool.

    3. c. The goat that the cat is washing <goat>has climbed onto the stool.

    4. d. The goat that the cats are washing <goat>has climbed onto the stool.

In (1a & b) the RC head (the goat) is the subject of both the main clause and the RC (hereafter, SS) whereas in (1c & d) the RC head is the subject of the main clause and the object of the RC (hereafter, SO). We aim to investigate whether children with G-SLI are sensitive to number feature dissimilarities of subject and object DPs that occupy argument positions in the sentence. We are working within the Computational Grammatical Complexity (CGC) hypothesis developed by van der Lely and colleagues (Marshall & van der Lely, Reference Marshall and van der Lely2007; van der Lely, Reference van der Lely1998, Reference van der Lely2005; inter alia). These authors argue that children with G-SLI are impaired in computing hierarchical structures within syntax, morphology, and phonology. Within syntax, the CGC hypothesis claims that the impairment in G-SLI is restricted to non-local dependencies at the clause level, which are derived through the application of movement operations. Their subsequent deficit includes the use of subordinate clauses (van der Lely, Reference van der Lely1998), wh-question formation (Marinis & van der Lely, Reference Marinis and van der Lely2007; van der Lely & Battel, Reference van der Lely and Battell2003), and the assignment of thematic roles in passive sentences (van der Lely, Reference van der Lely1996a). In contrast to these difficulties, other local syntactic dependencies, such as Specifier–Head agreement (e.g., the relation between determiner–noun within the DP and subject–verb within the clause), are spared (Stavrakaki and van der Lely, Reference Stavrakaki and van der Lely2010; van der Lely Reference van der Lely1998; van der Lely & Stollwerck, Reference van der Lely and Stollwerck1997). As for the current study, the CGC hypothesis predicts impaired comprehension of SS and particularly of SO, since both structures are derived by syntactic movement but the presence of non-canonical word order in SO makes any semantic strategy less likely to lead to success. The CGC hypothesis also predicts that structural dependencies that are not derived by movement are spared. Hence, children with G-SLI will be able to check agreement relations such as person or number within DPs, that is, between the determiner and the head noun in a Specifier–Head configuration. Thus, the presence of mismatching number features on DPs (and related verbs) is predicted to facilitate the correct interpretation of RCs in children with G-SLI.

We now turn to reviewing the relevant psycholinguistic literature about adults and TD children which forms the basis of our study.

Facilitation effects in RC comprehension: converging evidence from children and adults

Current psycholinguistic research with adults indicates that RC processing can be manipulated experimentally, using a variety of language properties. For example, using the self-paced reading technique (Gordon et al., Reference Gordon, Hendrick and Johnson2001), adults were significantly faster and more accurate in RCs such as (2), where the embedded DP was a pronoun (you), a proper name (Joe) or a quantifier (everyone), rather than when it was a definite description (the lawyer):

  1. (2) The barber that the lawyer/you/Joe/everyone admired <barber>climbed the mountain.

The effect found in adult on-line processing is also present in children's off-line comprehension. Hebrew-speaking children demonstrated higher accuracy for object RCs where the head noun was a definite DP (the girl) and the embedded DP was a constituent of a different type, such as a personal pronoun (Arnon, Reference Arnon2010), an interrogative pronoun, or impersonal arbitrary pro (Friedmann et al., Reference Friedmann, Belletti and Rizzi2009) (the following examples are English adaptations of the original Hebrew stimuli of Arnon, Reference Arnon2010):

  1. (3)

    1. a. What color are the shoes of the girl that the nurse is drawing <girl>?

    2. b. What color are the shoes of the girl that I am drawing <girl>?

Four-year-olds were significantly more accurate in (3b) than in (3a). Grillo (Reference Grillo2008) and Friedmann et al. (Reference Friedmann, Belletti and Rizzi2009) interpreted a facilitation in sentential contexts similar to (2) and (3) as the result of a stricter application of Relativized Minimality (Rizzi, Reference Rizzi1990, Reference Rizzi and Belletti2004). They propose that due to an increased language processing load in a context of impaired or immature language systems, adults with aphasia (Grillo, Reference Grillo2008) and young TD children (Friedmann et al., Reference Friedmann, Belletti and Rizzi2009) struggle to compute the syntactic relation between the DP head and its copy, when the intervening embedded subject DP (the lawyer in (2); the nurse in (3)) is structurally similar to the moved constituent. The approach put forward by Grillo and colleagues, on the one hand, and Friedmann and colleagues, on the other, differ in some details. Grillo assumes feature under-specification to be at the origin of this difficulty in aphasia, whereas Friedmann and colleagues capitalize on the internal structure of these constituents. In particular, Friedmann et al. (Reference Friedmann, Belletti and Rizzi2009) showed that four-year-old Hebrew-speaking children perform at chance level in (4a, b) but are significantly more accurate in (4c, d) (the following examples are English adaptations of the original Hebrew stimuli used by Friedmann et al., Reference Friedmann, Belletti and Rizzi2009):

  1. (4)

    1. a. Show me the girl that the nurse is wetting <girl>.

    2. b. Which girl does the nurse wet <girl>?

    3. c. Show me the girl that someone is wetting <girl>.

    4. d. Who does the nurse wet <girl>?

In order to explain this response pattern, Friedmann et al. (Reference Friedmann, Belletti and Rizzi2009) point out that the relative head/wh-phrase and the intervening subject have a set of disjoint features in (4c & d) but not in (4a & b), due to the simultaneous presence of a lexically restricting feature [+NP] on both constituents (e.g., they contain a noun girl and nurse and so they are both specified with the feature [+NP]). Friedmann et al. argue that it is the simultaneous presence of this feature on both constituents which makes (4a) and (4b) hard to interpret. However, this difficulty disappears when only one of the two constituents is specified with the [+NP] feature. In (4c & d), the absence of the lexical restriction on one of the two constituents makes the embedded subject (someone in (4c), who in (4d)) less prone to interpretation as a member of the relation between the RC head (in c) or the wh-pronoun (in d) and the copy.

In a study with Italian-speaking TD children, Adani et al. (Reference Adani, van der Lely, Forgiarini and Guasti2010) challenged the claim that object RCs with two full DPs are all equally difficult for children. Building on Friedmann et al. (Reference Friedmann, Belletti and Rizzi2009), Adani et al.'s (Reference Adani, van der Lely, Forgiarini and Guasti2010) proposal integrates the internal properties of the lexical restriction [+NP]. Italian-speaking children (age: 5–9) were assessed on their comprehension of object RCs that differed according to whether the subject and object DPs had the same (5a) or different (5b) number properties (SG=singular; PL=plural) or the same (5c) or different (5d) gender properties (M=masculine; F=feminine):

  1. (5)

    1. a. Il leone che il gatto sta toccando <leone>è seduto per terra.

      The lion-sg that the cat-sg is touching <lion>is sitting on the ground

    2. b. Il leone che i coccodrilli stanno toccando <leone>è seduto per terra.

      The lion-sg that the crocs-pl are touching <lion>is sitting on the ground

    3. c. Il gatto che il topo sta lavando <leone>è salito sullo sgabello.

      The cat-m that the mouse-m is washing <lion>has climbed onto the stool

    4. d. Il gatto che la capra sta lavando <leone>è salito sullo sgabello

      The cat-m that the goat-f is washing <lion>has climbed onto the stool

Overall, children were significantly more accurate in mismatch-feature-value conditions (5b & d), but it was also shown that the size of this effect was larger in the number condition (5b) than in the gender condition. In order to capture this asymmetry, Adani et al. proposed that it is not [+NP] per se that increases the ORC difficulty but that different features generate dissimilarity effects with different sizes. They argued that the prominence of these effects was related to the structural nature of the features. That is, independent functional heads such as number trigger a stronger distinctive effect, whereas lexicalized features such as gender in Italian contribute to the computation of dissimilarity to a lesser degree (Carminati, Reference Carminati2005).

The current study builds on and extends this body of work by investigating RC comprehension in children with G-SLI. There has been much research investigating inflectional morphology in children with SLI and their ability to comprehend and produce RCs and wh-questions. In the next two sections, we review the existing literature about morphological abilities and comprehension of movement-derived sentences in children with (G-)SLI.

Morphological abilities in children with (G-)SLI

The study of morphological impairments in SLI has received considerable attention in the literature, particularly with respect to tense marking. It is generally agreed that English-speaking children with SLI perform significantly poorly on tense morphology (Leonard et al., Reference Leonard, McGregor and Allen1992; Rice & Wexler, Reference Rice and Wexler1996), with regular verbs being affected more severely than irregular ones (van der Lely & Ullman, Reference van der Lely and Ullman2001). However, the findings regarding another morphological operation, namely agreement, show less consensus. We use the term ‘agreement’ to refer to both subject–verb agreement (e.g., person/number in English) within a clause (the child sleeps vs. the children sleep) and determiner–noun agreement (e.g., number in English) within a DP (a dog vs. some dogs). With respect to subject–verb agreement, some scholars consider that children with SLI have problems with inflected lexical verbs (Clahsen et al., Reference Clahsen, Bartke and Gollner1997; Leonard et al., Reference Leonard, McGregor and Allen1992), although inflection on functional verbs (e.g., auxiliary verbs in French) is less affected (Jakubowicz, Reference Jakubowicz2003). With respect to agreement within the DP, the picture is even more unclear. On the one hand, plural morphology on nominal constituents (plural -s) is considered to be impaired in SLI (Bortolini et al., Reference Bortolini, Leonard and Caselli1998; Clahsen et al., Reference Clahsen, Bartke and Gollner1997; Leonard et al., Reference Leonard, McGregor and Allen1992). On the other hand, however, children with SLI showed a productive plural system (Oetting & Rice, Reference Oetting and Rice1993). Moreover, language rehabilitation was found to be more successful when children had prior knowledge of noun pluralization and subject–verb agreement (Pawlowska, Leonard, Camarata, Brown & Camarata, Reference Pawlowska, Leonard, Camarata, Brown and Camarata2008).

The CGC hypothesis predicts a relative sensitivity in children with G-SLI for agreement relations because they are derived via a specifier–head relation (Stavrakaki & van der Lely, Reference Stavrakaki and van der Lely2010; van der Lely, Reference van der Lely1998). This predicted sensitivity to number agreement is tested, in the current study, against another grammatical operation, namely wh-movement. In contrast to number agreement, syntactic dependencies involving movement of constituents across other phrases are predicted to be impaired by the CGC hypothesis. In the next section, we will provide a detailed characterization of how children with (G-)SLI deal with two types of movement-derived structures, namely relative clauses and wh-questions.

Cross-linguistic studies on comprehension and production of movement-derived sentences in children with (G-)SLI

Friedmann and Novogrodsky tested comprehension (Friedmann & Novogrodsky, Reference Friedmann and Novogrodsky2004) and production (Novogrodsky & Friedmann, Reference Novogrodsky and Friedmann2006) in a group of Hebrew-speaking children with S(yntactic)-SLIFootnote 1 (N=10; age range 7;3–11;2 in the comprehension task and N=18; age range 9;3–14;6, in the production task). The children were tested on right-branching subject and object RCs. Compared to their controls, children with S-SLI were significantly impaired in their production and comprehension of object RCs, but no difference was found in subject RCs. Friedmann and Novogrodsky argue that children with S-SLI fail to assign the correct thematic role to a moved constituent. Furthermore, Levy and Friedmann (Reference Levy and Friedmann2009) describe a therapy case study of a Hebrew-speaking child with S-SLI targeting wh-movement (among other types of movement) using a metalinguistic methodology. The abstract notion of syntactic movement was taught to the child by means of visual illustrations. Then, the intervention moved towards the explicit teaching of syntactic movement in orally presented sentences. Wh-movement was explicitly taught using three structure types: topicalizations, object RCs, and subject RCs. At the end of the therapy, the child showed substantial improvement in all structures compared with the baseline and there was also an improvement in structures that were not explicitly treated, namely wh-questions. This improvement was maintained for at least 10 months.

In a series of studies conducted with Greek-speaking children, Stavrakaki assessed comprehension (N=8; age range: 5;4–9;4) as well as production (N=8; age range: 6;1–10;0) of RCs and wh-questions (Stavrakaki, Reference Stavrakaki2001, Reference Stavrakaki and Fava2002), using an act-out technique. In the comprehension study, children with SLI were tested on seven construction types, which showed different properties in terms of locus of embedding (centre-embedded vs. right-branching), subject position (pre-verbal vs. post-verbal), and presence or absence of clitic pronoun. The results revealed that the performance of children with SLI differed from their language-matched peers' performance on non-canonical word order structures. In a production task, rather than producing RCs, the children with SLI and some TD children produced simple active sentences and coordinated structures. However, only the children with SLI produced RCs with a missing head. Stavrakaki concluded that children with SLI are unable to use syntactic operations such as movement and have a deficit that mainly affects this computational component of language.

Two further studies investigated both the comprehension and the production of RCs in one group of young children with SLI. First, Håkansson and Hansson (Reference Håkansson and Hansson2000) carried out a longitudinal study on RC production and comprehension in a group of Swedish-speaking children with SLI (N=10; age range: 4;0–6;3). They found that these young children with SLI and younger language-matched TD children did not differ in their RC comprehension. However, in production, children with SLI omitted the relative complementizer, whereas the TD children used it. In a recent study, comprehension and production of RCs was assessed in a small group of Italian-speaking children with SLI (N=4; age range: 4:5–5:9) by Contemori and Garraffa (Reference Contemori and Garraffa2010). Production of object RCs was significantly impaired in children with SLI, compared to language- and age-matched controls. While TD children adopted various strategies to avoid the production of object RCs (such as passive subject RCs or object RCs with resumptive pronoun or resumptive DP), children with SLI were unable to construct any RC. Contemori and Garraffa (Reference Contemori and Garraffa2010) attributed the more accurate performance in comprehension shown by children with SLI to the application of a ‘first NP’=‘Agent’ heuristic.

Finally, in a longitudinal case study of three English-speaking children, Schuele and Nicholls (Reference Schuele and Nicholls2000) analyzed spontaneous language samples and an elicited RC task. The three children with SLI omitted obligatory relative markers (that/who/which) in subject RCs (e.g., she's get all the dishes need to be washed) and one of them selected the wrong relative marker in object RCs (e.g., she told everything *what she bought for you, where the star ‘*’ indicates the inappropriate relative marker for Standard American English). Based on these results, they concluded that children with SLI are impaired in both subject and object RCs. They interpreted this as impairment in using grammatical morphemes such as free-standing closed-class forms.

Our study provides the first experimental investigation of RC comprehension in English-speaking children with (G-)SLI. However, another syntactically complex construction, wh-questions, has been extensively investigated in children with G-SLI. As with RCs, wh-questions are also derived via movement, a syntactic operation which establishes a relation between the position in which the wh-constituent (who in (6)) is pronounced (this position is also called filler) and the position where the referent for the wh-constituent (indicated by <rabbit>in (6)) is interpreted (this position is also called gap):

  1. (6) Balloo gives a long carrot to the rabbit. (context)

    Who did Balloo give the long carrot to <rabbit>at the farm?

Given this structural similarity, the acquisition of RCs and wh-questions is often compared in developmental research. In the remaining part of this section, we will summarize previous findings on the on-line processing of wh-questions in children with G-SLI.

Marinis and van der Lely (Reference Marinis and van der Lely2007) conducted an on-line study to assess the processing of wh-questions. A group of children with G-SLI (N=14; age range: 10;2–17;2) and two groups of TD controls (receptive vocabulary/memory and chronological age) participated. Children with G-SLI were significantly slower than their age-controls, but not slower than their vocabulary-controls. Furthermore, whereas both the age- and vocabulary-controls showed reactivation of the moved wh-constituent at the gap position, indicating that they had processed wh-questions through syntactic filler–gap dependencies, the children with G-SLI showed a reactivation of the moved wh-constituent only at the verb (give in (6)). The authors argue that children with G-SLI fail to establish a syntactic filler–gap dependency and that they interpret wh-questions via lexical–thematic information. A recent electro-physiological investigation further supports this view. Fonteneau and van der Lely (Reference Fonteneau and van der Lely2008) investigated the processing of syntactic and semantic violations in a group of children and teenagers with G-SLI (N=18; age range: 10–21) and in three groups of language-controls, chronological age-controls, and adults, using the ERP technique. They found that the structural relations between the wh-word (who/what) and a DP that filled the first potential gap were unexpected and elicited an Early Left Anterior Negativity (ELAN) in all the control groups. However, such negativity was not activated in the children with G-SLI. The ELAN is reputedly specific to syntactic violations (Friederici, Hahne & Saddy, Reference Friederici, Hahne and Saddy2002). In contrast to the controls, the syntactic violation elicited a significant negativity around 400 ms with a right posterior distribution in the children with G-SLI. These electro-physiological brain responses were interpreted as revealing a selective impairment to the neural circuitry that is specific to grammatical processing in G-SLI. Furthermore, the participants with G-SLI appeared to be partially compensating for their syntactic deficit by using neural circuitry associated with semantic processing.

In summary, difficulties in RC comprehension are attested cross-linguistically in children with SLI. Children with G-SLI have not yet been tested on RCs, but given the hierarchical syntactic dependencies required for their understanding, as with wh-questions, the CGC hypothesis predicts that children with G-SLI will be impaired on them. Furthermore, the grammatical properties of syntactic constituents influence the comprehension of RCs in adults and TD children, but these properties have not been manipulated in a study of children with (G-)SLI. In the remaining part of the paper we will present how the role of number feature was tested in the comprehension of RCs in children with G-SLI.

METHOD

Participants

The group of children with G-SLI consisted of twelve participants (6 boys), aged 9;5–16;0 (M=12;11; SD=25·88 in months).

To be classified as G-SLI, the children had to first meet the criteria for SLI, and second show persistent deficits in syntax and morphology from the age of nine onwards. The selection is based on three specialized tests that probe core aspects of morphosyntax (passives, agreement, and tense). Children with G-SLI have to make 20% or more errors on these specialized tests, where TD children rarely make any after age six (van der Lely, Reference van der Lely2005).

Hence, the children included in this study all had a professional diagnosis of SLI and were attending special language schools or language units. Their non-verbal cognitive abilities fell within the normal range as measured by the Raven's Progressive Matrices (Raven, Reference Raven1998) (mean non-verbal IQ was 95·75; range: 85–111), and they had normal hearing, no articulation difficulties, and appropriate emotional and social behaviour. All children had a z-score of at least −1·5 (corresponding to −1·5 SD) on two or more of the following standardized language tests: Test for Reception of Grammar (Bishop, Reference Bishop2003), British Picture Vocabulary Scales (Dunn, Dunn, Whetton & Pintilie, Reference Dunn, Dunn, Whetton and Pintilie1982), the sentence repetition subtest of the Clinical Evaluations of Language Fundamentals (Semel, Wiig & Secord, Reference Semel, Wiig and Secord1995), Test of Word Finding (German, Reference German2000). An overview of scores of standardized tests for each participant is reported in Table 1.

Table 1. Scores on standardized (in z-scores) and non-standardized (in % correct) tests for children with G-SLI (the grey shade highlights a z-score of −1·5 (or lower) and an accuracy of 80% (or lower))

notes: RPM: Raven's Progressive Matrices (Raven, Reference Raven1998); TROG-2: Test for Reception of Grammar (Bishop, Reference Bishop2003); BPVS: British Picture Vocabulary Scales (Dunn et al., Reference Dunn, Dunn, Whetton and Pintilie1982); CELF-3: Sentence Repetition subtest of Clinical Evaluations of Language Fundamentals (Semel, Wiig & Secord, Reference Semel, Wiig and Secord1995); TWF-2: Test of Word Finding (German, Reference German2000); VATT: Verb Agreement and Tense Test (van der Lely, Reference van der Lely2000); A-STOP: Advanced Syntactic Test of Pronominal Reference (van der Lely, Reference van der Lely1997); TAPS: Test of Active and Passive Sentences (van der Lely, 1996b).

In addition to these standardized tests, all participants in this study have persistent problems on a range of non-standardized tests that tap into specific grammatical aspects. These include the Verb Agreement and Tense Test (van der Lely, Reference van der Lely2000), the Test of Active and Passive Sentences (van der Lely, Reference van der Lely1996b), and the Advanced Syntactic Test of Pronominal Reference (van der Lely, Reference van der Lely1997). Each participant made 20% or more errors on at least one specialized grammatical test, as illustrated in Table 1.

In line with a great deal of previous work by van der Lely and colleagues, we selected two groups of younger children who are matched on raw scores on either sentence understanding (hereafter grammar control group) or on receptive vocabulary (hereafter vocabulary control group). The use of two control groups allows one to independently determine the effects of vocabulary (an overall measure of language development) and grammar (a specific language component) on RC test performance. A summary of the group's details are given in Table 2.

Table 2. Mean ages, raw scores, and SD (in months) for each group

notes: TROG: Test Receptive of Grammar (Bishop, Reference Bishop2003); BPVS: British Picture Vocabulary Scale (Dunn et al., Reference Dunn, Dunn, Whetton and Pintilie1982).

The grammar control group consisted of twelve children (5 boys, mean age=6;9; SD=5·91 in months) who were matched to the children with G-SLI on the raw scores of a TROG test (Bishop, Reference Bishop2003) (independent sample t-test: p=·69). A second group of twelve children (3 boys, mean age=8;2; SD=5·12 in months) was matched to the group of children with G-SLI on raw scores obtained on BPVS (Dunn et al., Reference Dunn, Dunn, Whetton and Pintilie1982) (independent sample t-test: p=·55). The Grammar control group was significantly younger than the Vocabulary control group (independent sample t-test: p<·001). Receptive grammar and vocabulary in both language control groups were within the expected range of abilities for their ages, as shown in Table 2. The grammar level of the Vocabulary control group was significantly more advanced than that of children with G-SLI (independent sample t-test: p=·007), whereas the vocabulary level of the Grammar control group did not differ from that of children with G-SLI (independent sample t-test: p=·08).

Parental consent was obtained and the children were happy to take part in this study.

Design

Two experimental factors were manipulated: Sentence Type (SS vs. SO) and number Match (Match [M] vs. Mismatch [MM]). Table 3 illustrates the experimental design. For each sentence type (SS and SO), the RC head was singular and in the other half it was plural in the Match and Mismatch trials. Hence, plurality of the RC head was counterbalanced in each condition.

Table 3. Experimental design

notes: SS: subject relative clause; SO: object relative clause; M: Match; MM: Mismatch.

Materials

For each of these four conditions, there were twelve trials (6 with singular RC head and 6 with plural RC head), making forty-eight sentences in total. Eight verbs were used as main verbs (fall, sit, climb, jump, fly, enter, lie, slip) and they were paired with another eight transitive verbs in the embedded clause (pull, splash, wash, stroke, wave, push, comb, touch). All these verbs and some of the pictures used to depict them had been extensively piloted and used in previous work (Fonteneau & van der Lely, Reference Fonteneau and van der Lely2008; Stavrakaki & van der Lely, Reference Stavrakaki and van der Lely2010). We ensured that the same pair of verbs did not co-occur with the same pair of nouns. Nouns for subject and object DPs were all names of animals. The frequency of nouns used in Match or Mismatch conditions were compared using the Celex database and analysis revealed that there was no significant difference between conditions (SS sentences: p=·967; SO sentences: p=·857). All sentences were prerecorded by a female native speaker of British English. In addition to the experimental sentences, a further twenty-four SS and twenty-four SO with DPs matching in number were administered. Hence, in total, there were seventy-two sentences with two singular constituents and twenty-four sentences with one singular and one plural constituent.Footnote 2 From the first list of ninety-six sentences, a second list was then constructed in which the subject and object DPs were reversed. Thus, half of the participants were presented with List 1, the other half were presented with List 2. The creation of two lists with reversed noun order produced a cross-over design which enabled us to control for potential confounding effects, e.g., for a particular noun being a more suitable subject (or object) over the others. The trial order was pseudo-randomized. Each sentence was paired with four pictures that represented all combinations between the actors in the sentence (subject and object DP) and the two actions (embedded and main verbs). The same characters and actions were depicted in each set of pictures. A sample of condition 1 and condition 2 is given in Figure (1a) and (1b), respectively.

Fig. 1. A sample of the experimental pictures.

One of the pictures was correct and there were three distractor pictures. The distractor pictures were designed to provide insight into misinterpretations (see Response type coding section). The distractor and target picture positions on the screen were randomized.

Procedure

The experiment was programmed with E-Prime (Schneider, Eschman & Zuccolotto, Reference Schneider, Eschman and Zuccolotto2002) and administered using a Dell Latitude D510 laptop computer and SONY MDR-7509 headphones. Participants were instructed to look at the four pictures on the screen, listen carefully to the sentence, and then press one out of four keys on the keyboard. Each picture was linked to a number from one to four, which corresponded to the same number reported on each response key. Each response key was highlighted with bright-coloured stickers which corresponded to a picture number. Preliminary instructions emphasized the importance of looking carefully at all pictures and being accurate rather than fast.

Four practice trials preceded the presentation of test trials. A break was planned halfway through the trials and the children were free to have more breaks if needed. In order to maximize the children's attention to trials, the experimenter controlled the presentation of the next item after each response by clicking the mouse. The administration of the whole experiment took approximately 30–40 minutes per participant.

Response type coding

Children's responses were scored according to one of four categories, one of which was the target response and the remaining were errors. Examples in (7) are target responses for SS and SO respectively, and the relevant pictures are illustrated in Figure 2.

  1. (7)

Fig. 2. Target responses.

The arrows in (7) and in the examples below indicate which DP is the subject of each verb it is connected to, as they were depicted in the pictures. They are meant to help the reader to understand how we constructed distracters, but they do not indicate a relation between moved constituents and their copy in the original position.

Examples in (8) through (10) illustrate the three error categories. For both sentence types (SS and SO), three error categories are created on the basis of how children interpret the relation between subject and object DPs and verbs. The first error category is main clause error (=mce), where only the subject–verb relation in the embedded verb is interpreted correctly. A non-target response is categorized as MCE, if the chosen picture depicted the following relations, (8a) for SS and (8b) for SO (see Figure 3 for the corresponding pictures).

  1. (8)

Note that, in the SS sentence, the MCE means that participants rely on the linear order of constituents to interpret subject–verb relations.

Fig. 3. Main Clause Error (MCE).

The second error category was relative clause error (=RCE), where only the subject–verb relation in the main clause is interpreted correctly. A non-target response was categorized as RCE if the chosen picture depicted the following DP/verb relations, (9a) for an SS and (9b) for an SO (see Figure 4 for the corresponding pictures).

  1. (9)

The last category was double clause error (=DCE), where both subject–verb relations are erroneously interpreted. An error was categorized as DCE, if the chosen picture depicted the following relations, (10a) for an SS and (10b) for an SO (see Figure 5).

  1. (10)

Fig. 4. Relative Clause Error (RCE).

Fig. 5. Double Clause Error (DCE).

Data analysis

The mean frequencies for the different response types for each condition and each group are presented in Table 4.Footnote 3 We used a repeated measure logistic regression model to analyze the results. The dependent variable (Response type) is a categorical factor with four levels (Correct, Main Clause Error (MCE), Relative Clause Error (RCE), Double Clause Error (DCE)), which are not independent as only one of the four categories can be chosen in each trial. Because of the statistical dependency of responses, we obtained the logistic regression parameters and the associated inferential tests with GEE methodology (Liang & Zeger, Reference Liang and Zeger1986), as implemented by the SAS system. First, we compared the response accuracy between the two lists. The factor List did not yield any significant difference (χ2(1, N=1728)=0·23, p=0·63). Hence, we can safely assume that the responses of the subjects did not differ in the two lists. Hence, we collapsed the factor List in the following analyses.

Table 4. Mean frequencies (expressed in percentages) for the experimental conditions for the three subject groupsa

notes: C: correct; MCE: Main Clause Error; RCE: Relative Clause Error; DCE: Double Clause Error; SS: subject relative clauses; SO: object relative clauses; M: Match; MM: Mismatch; a Within each group, C+MCE+RCE+DCE add up to 100%.

Accuracy analysis

In order to determine if the groups' responses differed on SS and SO and on the Match and Mismatch conditions, the first model contrasted target and non-target responses (which add up to 100% of subjects' responses). Table 5 reports the estimated coefficient and its level of significance for each fixed factor (in odds ratios).

Table 5. Parameter values for fixed effects in mixed logistic regression model of accurate response proportions, expressed in odds ratios

NOTE: None of the other main effects or 2nd or 3rd order interactions is significant.

The significant parameter estimates associated with the fixed factors Sentence and Match indicate that SS conditions and mismatch conditions significantly increased the odds of a correct response. The main effect of Group was also significant. The follow-up group contrasts showed that the performance of the G-SLI group was significantly less accurate than the Vocabulary control groups, but there was no significant difference between G-SLI/grammar control groups or between grammar control/vocabulary control groups.

In summary, the accuracy analysis revealed that SS sentences are more accurate than SO sentences. Thus, our findings support the subject–object asymmetry previously reported in the literature. Moreover, the mismatch conditions are more accurate than match conditions, in both sentence types and for all groups. The group of children with G-SLI performed less accurately on this task than the vocabulary control group. However, the difference did not reach the significance level (p=·66) when comparing the G-SLI and grammar control groups.

Non-target response analysis

In order to determine if there were group differences in non-target responses, we compared the following three models, i.e., Main Clause Error (MCE)/Relative Clause Error (RCE), Main Clause Error (MCE)/Double Clause Error (DCE), and Relative Clause Error (RCE)/Double Clause Error (DCE). Table 6 shows the estimated coefficient and its level of significance for each fixed factor (in odds ratios).

Table 6. Parameter values for fixed effects in mixed logistic regression model of non-target response proportions, expressed in odds ratios

notes: MCE: Main Clause Error; RCE: Relative Clause Error; DCE: Double Clause Error; a one grammar-matched child is excluded due to absence of MCE or RCE responses; b one grammar-matched child is excluded due to absence of MCE or DCE responses; c two grammar-matched children and one vocabulary-matched child are excluded due to absence of RCE or DCE responses; none of the other main effects or 2nd or 3rd order interactions is significant.

First, we calculated the change of odds for choosing an MCE response rather than an RCE response, for each factor and each group. Table 6 (left panel) shows a main effect of the factor Sentence, which indicates that SS conditions significantly increased the odds of an MCE response. Thus, when non-target responses occurred, the different sentence types were associated with different response strategies. Namely, MCE responses occurred significantly more often in SS sentences, whereas RCE responses occurred significantly more often in SO sentences. The factor Group did not modulate these effects. Second, we calculated the odds change for choosing an MCE rather than a DCE, for each factor and each group. We found significant main effects of the factors Sentence and Match (see Table 6, central panel). These two main effects indicated that SS conditions and mismatch conditions significantly increased the odds of an MCE response. We also found a significant interaction of Sentence×Group. Multiple contrasts revealed that children with G-SLI had a greater probability of choosing a DCE response in SO conditions, whereas the Grammar and Vocabulary control group did not. Thus, in contrast to grammar and vocabulary controls, the children with G-SLI chose significantly more MCE responses in SS and significantly more DCE responses in SO. Third, we calculated the odds change of choosing an RCE response rather than a DCE response, for each factor and each group. As the right panel of Table 6 shows, only the main effect of the fixed factor Match is significant. This main effect indicates that mismatch conditions significantly decreased the probability of a DCE response.

Individual performance

In addition to the statistical analysis for groups, individual performance is shown in Figure 6 to verify whether our group results are representative of performance across individuals within the group. The top-centre and top-right panels in Figure 6 show that the grammar and vocabulary control participants were overall very accurate in SS. Three out of twenty-four children were exceptions to this pattern (S13 and S16 in the grammar control group and S28 in the vocabulary control group) and showed at least 20% MCE responses. In contrast, MCE responses in SS were consistently attested in nine out of twelve children with G-SLI, as indicated in the top-left panel in Figure 6 (the remaining 3 children reach 90% correct in SS conditions). Turning to SO sentences (bottom panels in Figure 6), four children in the Grammar control group and two in the Vocabulary control group were around 90% accurate. None of the children in the G-SLI group reached this performance level. Among the grammar control participants that were below a cut-off point of 90% accurate, six out of eight chose predominantly RCE responses, one child chose RCE and DCE responses to the same extent, and one child (S25) chose MCE responses. As for the vocabulary control group, among the ten children who are less than 90% accurate, six children consistently chose RCE responses; one child (S31) chose RCE and DCE responses to the same extent; another child (S34) chose RCE/MCE/DCE responses to the same extent; and only two children out of ten showed a preference for MCE responses, when they failed to choose the target picture. Hence, the majority of TD children who choose a wrong answer tend to misinterpret the grammatical relation within the RC and choose the reversed animal character(s) as agent(s) of the embedded verb. In contrast, within the group of children with G-SLI, it is not possible to identify a response strategy that children prefer over other strategies. As shown in the bottom-left panel in Figure 6, each child with G-SLI performs very differently from the other ones in the group.

Fig. 6. Individual performances on SS (top panel) and SO (bottom panel).

In summary, a detailed inspection of individual performances revealed that a minority of the children did not behave in accordance with the group results. Importantly, this holds for children with G-SLI as well as TD children. However, we think that this divergent behaviour of some participants is ultimately the manifestation of an expected heterogeneity that affects these populations, and this is exactly the reason why inferential statistics are useful in these cases.

DISCUSSION

Comprehension of SS and SO with matching and mismatching number features on subject and object DPs was assessed in a group of children with G-SLI and two groups of younger TD children. Our main finding is that correct RC interpretation was facilitated by DP number dissimilarities for children with G-SLI, just as for TD children. That is, where one DP was singular and the other one was plural, performance was significantly more accurate than in those conditions where both DPs had the same number, for all groups of children. Furthermore, children with G-SLI performed significantly less accurately than TD vocabulary controls on both SS and SO, a response pattern that is often reported in the existing cross-linguistic literature. Analysis of non-target responses revealed that, if children with G-SLI fail to choose the target answer, they often rely on the linear word order of constituents when interpreting SS (a strategy used significantly less often by TD children). However, they do not show a preferred strategy in SO. In contrast to the children with G-SLI, both groups of TD children tend to choose the RCE error in SO (i.e., they tend to interpret SO as SS).

We will now discuss our findings in the light of the existing psycholinguistic approaches to RC difficulty and to language impairment in (G-)SLI presented at the beginning of the paper.

Number dissimilarity effects in RC comprehension in children with G-SLI

A number of recent psycholinguistic studies investigating on-line processing, production, or comprehension abilities in adults and TD children have shown that RC difficulty can be modulated in experimental contexts. As discussed in the ‘Introduction’, these effects can be traced back to the nature of linguistic constituents that appear in the sentence. It is now established that object RCs with two full DPs are harder to interpret than object RCs where one argument is a full DP and the other is a pronoun, a proper name, a quantified expression, or another nominal constituent of a different structural type (Arnon, Reference Arnon2010; Friedmann et al., Reference Friedmann, Belletti and Rizzi2009; Gordon et al., Reference Gordon, Hendrick and Johnson2001) and that feature dissimilarity also plays a role (Adani et al., Reference Adani, van der Lely, Forgiarini and Guasti2010). These results are supported by our findings on TD children. Furthermore, we discovered that the same facilitation was evident in children with G-SLI.

Difficulties in RC comprehension in children with SLI are well documented in the literature. Some studies capitalize on the fact that object RCs impose a particular difficulty for children with SLI (Friedmann & Novogrodsky, Reference Friedmann and Novogrodsky2004), but most studies also show that subject RC comprehension and production are not intact in SLI (Håkansson & Hansson, Reference Håkansson and Hansson2000; Schuele & Nicholls, Reference Schuele and Nicholls2000; Stavrakaki, Reference Stavrakaki and Fava2002). In our study, children with G-SLI (mean age: 12;11) performed significantly less accurately on both SS and SO when compared to receptive-vocabulary TD controls (mean age: 8;1), but the difference did not reach significance level with the grammar control group (mean age: 6;9). We found that despite a general overall poorer performance, children with G-SLI are above chance on both SS and SO sentences, and they used number information in a similar way to TD children. The two factors Match and Group never interacted in the analysis of accuracy or in the analysis of non-target responses. Therefore, despite their clear difficulties in computing the movement operations that are necessary to interpret RCs, children with G-SLI are able to make use of other morphological cues that facilitate their comprehension. So, what are the consequences of these results?

We argue that the ability to compute movement-derived dependencies is defective in children with G-SLI, but the computations of relations that are established via specifier–head agreement (within the DP or within the clause) are spared. Furthermore, the ability to compute agreement relations within the DP and via spec–head can actually be used by children with G-SLI to overcome some of their difficulties with movement. Thus, our results support the CGC hypothesis (van der Lely, Reference van der Lely2005) and they also corroborate existing findings by Jakubowicz (Reference Jakubowicz2003) and Stavrakaki and van der Lely (Reference Stavrakaki and van der Lely2010) as knowledge of subject–auxiliary verb agreement is not affected in (G-)SLI.

The above-chance performance on both SS and SO is explained using the notion of ‘movement optionality’ (van der Lely, Reference van der Lely1998), according to which ‘movement is a free choice’ (p. 180) in the grammar of children with G-SLI. This contrasts with adult grammar and the grammar of TD children, where checking features via movement is an obligatory operation. In this respect, our proposal differs from both Stavrakaki's (Reference Stavrakaki and Fava2002) and Friedmann and Novogrodsky's (Reference Friedmann and Novogrodsky2004, Reference Friedmann and Novogrodsky2007) proposals on the underlying nature of syntactic deficits in children with SLI. Stavrakaki (Reference Stavrakaki and Fava2002) observed below-chance performance on the production of both object wh-questions and RCs, which is explained as absence of movement operations in the grammar of Greek-speaking children with SLI. Such chance performance on SS and SO is not found in our results, thus indicating the optional availability of this grammatical operation in English-speaking children with G-SLI. It is possible that Stavrakaki's young subjects had yet to learn such compensation, giving the appearance of the absence rather than the optionality of movement. Friedmann and Novogrodski (Reference Friedmann and Novogrodsky2004, Reference Friedmann and Novogrodsky2007) argue that the syntactic deficit on movement-derived sentences affects the ability of Hebrew-speaking children with S-SLI to assign thematic roles correctly. The prediction that this hypothesis would make in our experiment is that children with G-SLI should choose the RCE response in SO (thus, interpreting SO as SS). However, this response behaviour was mostly found in the two TD groups. Recent data, however, suggest a different explanation for this so-called ‘optionality’. Fonteneau and van der Lely's (Reference Fonteneau and van der Lely2008) ERP data indicated that G-SLI children use different neural mechanisms to interpret sentences; specifically those associated with semantic rather than syntactic processing. They suggested that their teenage participants with G-SLI were thus compensating for their syntactic deficit semantically. A hint that this might also be what is happening in our data comes from the inspection of non-target responses that children with G-SLI gave when they interpreted SS sentences incorrectly. As is discussed later in this section (cf. (11) below and the discussion around this error), when children with G-SLI fail to interpret an SS sentence, the majority of them seem to interpret subject–verb relations locally, thus relying on the linear order of constituents. This error might come about because children with G-SLI rely on verb semantics (rather than sentence structure) while interpreting an SS sentence.

Importantly, children with G-SLI's demonstrated ability to use number information correctly might have a valuable implication for therapy: morphological feature dissimilarities among them, number dissimilarities on nouns and verbs can be made prominent in order to facilitate sentence comprehension. In the light of the promising results reported by Levy and Friedmann (Reference Levy and Friedmann2009), we would like to suggest that number dissimilarities on nouns and verbs could also be used to make the relations between a verb and its arguments more explicit in children where implicit knowledge of verb argument structure is significantly impaired. Furthermore, number dissimilarities could also be introduced in a more advanced phase of the therapy, for instance when explicit teaching of wh-movement takes place. This could be used with centre-embedded RCs, similar to the ones that we have investigated in our study of children with G-SLI. These sentences present two lexical verbs (one in the main clause and one in the embedded one) and two potential candidates as arguments (the subject and the object DPs). Centre-embedded RCs are notoriously harder than right-branching RCs for adults (Gibson, Reference Gibson1998) and for children (Corrêa, Reference Corrêa1995). In these harder cases, we therefore suggest that the use of additional morphological markers to identify the grammatical relations between sentential constituents might be fruitful in early therapeutic stages. Further intervention studies are needed to test whether such therapy is effective. If this prediction is supported, this intervention concept could be applied to the comprehension of other types of constructions (object wh-questions, cleft sentences) in the future.

In the rest of this section we will discuss the patterns of non-target response that were identifiable through our four-picture experimental set-up, and how these response patterns elucidate the existing knowledge on RC comprehension difficulties in TD children and children with SLI. These commentaries are based on the results of the group statistical analysis, although intra-individual differences were identified in the three groups of children, as discussed above.

The non-target response that both groups of TD children predominantly chose when they failed to pick the target picture for an SO was the RCE response. This picture depicted an action where the subject of the embedded verb was misinterpreted as its object. Thus, the RC head was interpreted as the subject of the main verb as well as subject of the embedded verb. This error ultimately corresponds to interpreting an SO as an SS. The tendency of young TD children to interpret object RCs as subject RCs is a robust finding attested across studies, both in production (Belletti & Contemori, Reference Belletti and Contemori2010; Diessel & Tomasello, Reference Diessel and Tomasello2000; Guasti & Cardinaletti, Reference Guasti and Cardinaletti2003; Novogrodsky & Friedmann, Reference Novogrodsky and Friedmann2006; Stavrakaki, Reference Stavrakaki and Fava2002; Zukowski, Reference Zukowski2008) and in binary-picture-sentence-matching tasks (Arosio, Adani & Guasti, Reference Arosio, Adani, Guasti, Brucart, Gavarrò and Sola2009; Contemori & Garraffa, Reference Contemori and Garraffa2010; Friedmann & Novogrodsky, Reference Friedmann and Novogrodsky2004; Friedmann et al., Reference Friedmann, Belletti and Rizzi2009). Our study shows that TD children tend to interpret an object RC as a subject RC even when more alternative pictures are available to them, thus indicating that this is not a task-dependent response but a genuine strategy. Following Grillo (Reference Grillo2008) and Friedmann et al. (Reference Friedmann, Belletti and Rizzi2009), we interpret the difficulty with object RC as due to the requirement to interpret the relationship between the RC head and its copy in merge position, crossing the embedded subject DP. The embedded subject is a nominal constituent of a structural type similar to the moved constituent and thus it acts as a potential candidate of the movement relationship (Rizzi, Reference Rizzi and Belletti2004). The effect of the intervening subject in object RC is usually observed up to age four years, whereas older children perform significantly more accurately (Adani, Reference Adani2011). All TD children included in our sample were older than five (their ages ranged between 6;0 and 8;11), so they are not expected to make such errors. However, in contrast with most of the cited articles, in our study only centre-embedded RCs were tested. Our finding concurs with other published work showing that older children (8–10 years) are not yet performing at ceiling on RCs (Arosio, Guasti & Stucchi, Reference Arosio, Guasti and Stucchi2011; Guasti & Cardinaletti, Reference Guasti and Cardinaletti2003; MacDaniel, McKee & Bernstein, Reference McDaniel, McKee and Bernstein1998). Hence, it is not surprising that older children show difficulties which are analogous to those of younger children when tested on the most taxing structure.

Moving to G-SLI, children with this impairment chose the MCE 24% of the time (on average for the match and mismatch conditions) on SS sentences. This response only appears around 8% of the time in the two groups of TD children. Furthermore, inspection of individual performances revealed that nine out of twelve children with G-SLI choose the MCE response consistently in SS, whereas only three out of twenty-four TD children showed a similar response pattern. The example in (11) illustrates the structural relations in the MCE response in SS:

  1. (11)

Hence, the attempt to interpret an SS sentence results in a coordinated structure, such as (12):

  1. (12) The cat is touching the goat and the goat has climbed onto the stool

Similar heuristic strategies are attested in other studies of children with SLI as well. For example, children with SLI produce simple declarative (Contemori & Garraffa, Reference Contemori and Garraffa2010; Levy & Friedmann, Reference Levy and Friedmann2009; Stavrakaki, Reference Stavrakaki and Fava2002) and coordinated sentences (Levy & Friedmann, Reference Levy and Friedmann2009; Stavrakaki, Reference Stavrakaki and Fava2002) instead of the more complex RCs. Stavrakaki (Reference Stavrakaki and Fava2002) relates this non-target response to a last resort processing strategy. We suggest that this error represents an attempt to interpret subject–verb relations as locally as possible on the basis of the linear word order of constituents, thus ignoring the presence of clause boundaries.

Similar to the case of SS sentences, there is also a divergence between children with G-SLI and TD children in non-target SO responses. Whereas TD children tend to choose the RCE, the responses of children with G-SLI are distributed across the three error categories: Main Clause Error (MCE: 17%), Relative Clause Error (RCE: 20%), and Double Clause Error (DCE: 20%). As we have seen, in TD children, the prominence of RCE responses was linked to the intervening embedded subject, which disrupts the interpretation of SO. Most children with G-SLI do not show this preference for the RCE response. Indeed, it is not possible to identify a specific strategy that is used by children with G-SLI when they fail to interpret an SO. Therefore, we conclude that teenage children with G-SLI are significantly impaired in computing hierarchical structural dependencies for RCs, and moreover do not show any one alternative strategy to resolve this impairment. This can be inferred because they do not show a preference for RCE responses; rather, they all show a different response pattern. On the basis of the results obtained in our task it is not possible to say more about the underlying processes that children with G-SLI are using while parsing an RC. In order to shed more light on these processes, the use of on-line as well as brain-imaging measures is warranted.

We would like to conclude this section with a remark regarding the interaction between comprehension and production data to describe language abilities in children with SLI. Contemori and Garraffa (Reference Contemori and Garraffa2010) used production data to argue that ‘children with SLI do not use the same facilitative strategies as TD children’ (p. 1954), although the accuracy scores between the two groups (SLI and TD) did not differ significantly in comprehension. We would like to reinforce and extend the scope of this observation by suggesting that comprehension data can also provide a detailed level of analysis into the potential alternative strategies that children use when comprehension of syntactically complex sentences fails. This is made possible by constructing the experimental material to allow the participant to choose from a wider set of alternative responses, as in this study.

CONCLUSIONS

Our results reveal that RC comprehension is significantly impaired in children with G-SLI in relation to younger language-matched TD children. However, children with G-SLI show a similar sensitivity as TD children to number manipulation. When one DP was singular and the other one was plural, all children were significantly more accurate than in those conditions where both DPs had the same number. We argue that children with G-SLI are able to make use of low-level distinctions (such as number marking on DPs) and to compute local grammatical relations (such as subject–verb agreement). These findings support the CGC hypothesis, which predicts a deficit in the computation of movement derived sentences but a relative strength in the computations of other grammatical relations that do not involve movement of a constituent, such as noun–determiner agreement and subject–verb agreement. We also suggest that this spared ability could be used in therapy to enhance the comprehension of syntactically complex sentences such as RCs and possibly other related constructions (e.g., wh-questions and clefts). Due to the intrinsic limitation of our behavioural method and to the apparent intra-individual variability within the three groups of children, it is clear that more investigations, especially those on brain imagining, are warranted in order to more specifically characterize the nature of the deficit in (G-)SLI.

Footnotes

[*]

We would like to thank the children who took part in the study, their parents, and the staff from the following schools: Dawn House School, Moor House School, and Radlett Primary School. We are grateful to Nichola Gallon, Angela Pozzuto, and Chloë Marshall for their help in designing the experiment and helping out with testing, to Mike Coleman for sharing his technical expertise, and to Chloë Marshall and Outi Tuomainen for their comments on a previous version of the manuscript. All remaining errors are of course our own. FA was supported by a PhD scholarship awarded by the University Milano-Bicocca; HvdL was supported by The Wellcome Trust Grant (063713); MTG, MF, and HvdL were supported by the grant Cross-linguistic Language Diagnosis (CLAD, 135295-LLP-1-2007-UK-Ka1-Ka1SCR). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, the decision to publish or the preparation of the manuscript.

1 Whereas both S-SLI and G-SLI subgroups have deficits in syntax and morphology, children with G-SLI typically have deficits in phonology too, whereas S-SLI do not (van der Lely, Reference van der Lely2005).

2 A reviewer raises the question whether such an imbalance might have had an effect on the results. We think this is not the case as, if it were so, the most frequent singular-only items should have been facilitated, something that definitely does not happen in our results.

3 Standard deviations do not appear in the table, given that our dependent variable is categorical (rather than continuous). This also justifies the use of a logistic regression model to estimate the response frequencies.

References

REFERENCES

Adani, F. (2011). Rethinking the acquisition of relative clauses in Italian: towards a grammatically based account. Journal of Child Language 38(1), 141–65.Google Scholar
Adani, F., van der Lely, H. K. J., Forgiarini, M. & Guasti, M. T. (2010). Grammatical feature dissimilarities make relative clauses easier: a comprehension study with Italian children. Lingua 120(9), 2418–66.Google Scholar
Arnon, I. (2010). Rethinking child difficulty: the effect of NP type on children's processing of relative clauses in Hebrew. Journal of Child Language 37(1), 2757.Google Scholar
Arosio, F., Adani, F. & Guasti, M. T. (2009). Processing grammatical features by Italian children. In Brucart, J. M., Gavarrò, A. & Sola, J. (eds.), Merging features: computation, interpretation, and acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Arosio, F., Guasti, M. T. & Stucchi, N. A. (2011). Disambiguating information and memory resources in children's processing of Italian relative clauses. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 40(2), 137–54.Google Scholar
Belletti, A. & Contemori, C. (2010). Intervention and attraction. On the production of subject and object relatives by Italian (young) children and adults. Paper presented at Language Acquisition and Development: Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1997). Uncommon understanding: development and disorders of language comprehension in children. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). Test for Reception of Grammar (TROG-2). London: Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
Bortolini, U., Leonard, B. L. & Caselli, M. C. (1998). Specific language impairment in Italian and English: evaluating alternative accounts of grammatical seficits. Language and Cognitive Processes 13(1), 120.Google Scholar
Carminati, M. N. (2005). Processing reflexes of the Feature Hierarchy (Person>Number>Gender) and implications for linguistic theory. Lingua 115, 259–85.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., Bartke, S. & Gollner, S. (1997). Formal features in impaired grammars: a comparison of English and German SLI children. Journal of Neurolinguistics 10(2/3), 151–71.Google Scholar
Contemori, C. & Garraffa, M. (2010). Comparison of modalities in SLI syntax: a study on the comprehension and production of non-canonical sentences. Lingua 120(8), 1940–55.Google Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G. & Botting, N. (1999). Classification of children with Specific Language Impairment: longitudinal considerations. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 42, 1195–204.Google Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G., Crutchely, A. & Botting, N. (1997). The extent to which psychometric tests differentiate subgroups of children with SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 40, 765–77.Google Scholar
Corrêa, L. (1995). An alternative assessment of children's comprehension of relative clauses. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 24(3), 183203.Google Scholar
Diessel, H. & Tomasello, M. (2000). The development of relative clauses in spontaneous child speech. Cognitive Linguistics 11(1/2), 131–51.Google Scholar
Dunn, L., Dunn, L., Whetton, C. & Pintilie, D. (1982). The British Picture Vocabulary Scales. Windsor: NFERNelson.Google Scholar
Fonteneau, E. & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2008). Electrical brain responses in language-impaired children reveal grammar-specific deficit. Plos One 3(3), 16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A. D., Hahne, A. & Saddy, D. (2002). Distinct neurophysiological patterns reflecting aspects of syntactic complexity and syntactic repair. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 31(1), 4663.Google Scholar
Friedmann, N., Belletti, A. & Rizzi, L. (2009). Relativized relatives. Types of intervention in the acquisition of A-bar dependencies. Lingua 119, 6788.Google Scholar
Friedmann, N. & Novogrodsky, R. (2004). The acquisition of relative clause comprehension in Hebrew: a study of SLI and normal development. Journal of Child Language 31, 661–81.Google Scholar
Friedmann, N. & Novogrodsky, R. (2007). Is the movement deficit in syntactic SLI related to traces or to thematic role transfer? Brain & Language 101(1), 5063.Google Scholar
Friedmann, N. & Novogrodsky, R. (2011). Which questions are most difficult to understand?: The comprehension of Wh questions in three subtypes of SLI. Lingua 121(3), 367–82.Google Scholar
German, D. J. (2000). Test of Word Finding, Second Edition (TWF-2): Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition 68, 176.Google Scholar
Gordon, P., Hendrick, R. & Johnson, M. (2001). Memory interference during language processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 27, 1411–23.Google Scholar
Grillo, N. (2008). Generalized Minimality: syntactic underspecification in Broca's aphasia. PhD dissertation distributed by LOT, University of Utrecht, the Netherlands.Google Scholar
Guasti, M. T. & Cardinaletti, A. (2003). Relative clause formation in Romance child's production. Probus 15, 4789.Google Scholar
Håkansson, G. & Hansson, K. (2000). Comprehension and production of relative clauses: a comparison between Swedish impaired and unimpaired children. Journal of Child Language 27, 313–33.Google Scholar
Jakubowicz, C. (2003). Computational complexity and the acquisition of functional categories by French-speaking children with SLI. Linguistics 41(2), 175211.Google Scholar
Jakubowicz, C. & Tuller, L. (2008). Specific language impairment in French. In Ayoun, D. (ed.), Studies in French applied linguistics, 97133. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Leonard, B. L. (1998). Children with Specific Language Impairment. Cambridge: MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Leonard, B. L., McGregor, K. K. & Allen, G. D. (1992). Grammatical morphology and speech perception in children with Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 35, 1076–85.Google Scholar
Levy, H. & Friedmann, N. (2009). Treatment of syntactic movement in syntactic SLI: a case study. First Language 29(1), 1550.Google Scholar
Liang, K. Y. & Zeger, S. L. (1986). Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models. Biometrika 73, 1322.Google Scholar
Marinis, T. & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2007). On-line processing of wh-questions in children with G-SLI and typically developing children. International Journal of Language Communication Disorders 42(5), 557–82.Google Scholar
Marshall, C. R. & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2007). The impact of phonological complexity on past tense inflection in children with Grammatical-SLI. Advances in Speech-Language Pathology 9, 191203.Google Scholar
McDaniel, D., McKee, C. & Bernstein, J. B. (1998). How children's relatives solve a problem for Minimalism. Language 74(2), 308–34.Google Scholar
Novogrodsky, R. & Friedmann, N. (2006). The production of relative clauses in syntactic-SLI: a window to the nature of the impairment. Advances in Speech-Language Pathology 8(4), 364–75.Google Scholar
Oetting, J. B. & Rice, M. L. (1993). Plural acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36, 1236–48.Google Scholar
Pawlowska, M., Leonard, L. B., Camarata, S. M., Brown, B. & Camarata, M. N. (2008). Factors accounting for the ability of children with SLI to learn agreement morphemes in intervention. Journal of Child Language 35, 2553.Google Scholar
Raven, J. C. (1998). Raven's progressive matrices. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L. & Wexler, K. (1996). Toward tense as a clinical marker of Specific Language Impairment in English-speaking children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 39 1239–57Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. (1990). Relativized Minimality (Vol. 16): Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. (2004). Locality and left periphery. In Belletti, A. (ed.), Structure and beyond, 223–51. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Schneider, W., Eschman, A. & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user's guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc.Google Scholar
Schuele, C. M. & Nicholls, L. M. (2000). Relative clauses: evidence of continued linguistic vulnerability in children with specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 14(8), 563–85.Google Scholar
Semel, E., Wiig, E. H. & Secord, W. A. (1995). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF): San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Stark, R. E. & Tallal, P. (1981). Selection of children with Specific Language Deficits. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 46, 114–22.Google Scholar
Stavrakaki, S. (2001). Comprehension of reversible relative clauses in Specifically Language Impaired and normally developing Greek children. Brain and Language 77, 419–31.Google Scholar
Stavrakaki, S. (2002). A-bar movement constructions in Greek children with SLI: evidence for deficits in the syntactic component of language. In Fava, E. (ed.), Clinical linguistics: theory and applications in speech pathology and therapy, 131–53. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Stavrakaki, S. & van der Lely, H. (2010). Production and comprehension of pronouns by Greek children with specific language impairment. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 28(1), 189216.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J. (1996a). Specifically language impaired and normally developing children: verbal passive vs. adjectival passive sentence interpretation. Lingua 98, 243–72.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J. (1996b). The Test of Active and Passive Sentences (TAPS). Available at: <www.dldcn.org>.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J. (1997). Advanced Syntactic Test of Pronominal Reference (A-STOP). Available at: <www.dldcn.org>.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J. (1998). SLI in children: movement, economy, and deficits in the computational-syntactic system. Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics 7(2–4), 161–92.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J. (2000). Verb Agreement and Tense Test (VATT). Available at: <www.dldcn.org>.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J. (2005). Domain-specific cognitive systems: insight from grammatical specific language impairment. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(2), 5359.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J. & Battell, J. (2003). WH-movement in children with Grammatical-SLI: a test of the RDDR hypothesis. Language 79, 153–79.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J. & Stollwerck, L. (1997). Binding principles and grammatical specific language impairment in children. Cognition 62, 245–90.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J. & Ullman, M. (2001). Paste tense morphology in specifically language impaired children and normally developing children. Language and Cognitive Processes 16, 177217.Google Scholar
Zukowski, A. (2008). Elicited production of relative clauses in children with Williams syndrome. Language and Cognitive Processes 24(1), 143.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Scores on standardized (in z-scores) and non-standardized (in % correct) tests for children with G-SLI (the grey shade highlights a z-score of −1·5 (or lower) and an accuracy of 80% (or lower))

Figure 1

Table 2. Mean ages, raw scores, and SD (in months) for each group

Figure 2

Table 3. Experimental design

Figure 3

Fig. 1. A sample of the experimental pictures.

Figure 4

Fig. 2. Target responses.

Figure 5

Fig. 3. Main Clause Error (MCE).

Figure 6

Fig. 4. Relative Clause Error (RCE).

Figure 7

Fig. 5. Double Clause Error (DCE).

Figure 8

Table 4. Mean frequencies (expressed in percentages) for the experimental conditions for the three subject groupsa

Figure 9

Table 5. Parameter values for fixed effects in mixed logistic regression model of accurate response proportions, expressed in odds ratios

Figure 10

Table 6. Parameter values for fixed effects in mixed logistic regression model of non-target response proportions, expressed in odds ratios

Figure 11

Fig. 6. Individual performances on SS (top panel) and SO (bottom panel).