Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:37:44.721Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Novel word retention in sequential bilingual children*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 May 2013

PUI FONG KAN*
Affiliation:
University of Colorado at Boulder
*
Address for correspondence: University of Colorado at Boulder – Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, 2501 Kittredge Loop Road 409, UCB Boulder Colorado 80309, United States. e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Children's ability to learn and retain new words is fundamental to their vocabulary development. This study examined word retention in children learning a home language (L1) from birth and a second language (L2) in preschool settings. Participants were presented with sixteen novel words in L1 and in L2 and were tested for retention after either a 2-month or a 4-month delay. Results showed that children retained more words in L1 than in L2 for both of the retention interval conditions. In addition, children's word retention was associated with their existing language knowledge and their fast-mapping performance within and across language. The patterns of association, however, were different between L1 and L2. These findings suggest that children's word retention might be related to the interactions of various components that are operating within a dynamic system.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

Manuscript preparation was supported by funds from the University of Colorado. A portion of these data was presented at the International Symposium of Bilingualism 7 in Utrecht, the Netherlands. I am grateful to the following research assistants for help with data collection, scoring, and analysis: Jy Xiong, Kia Xiong, Chao Khang, Kristy Benoit, Marisa Irwin, Alaina Kelley, Lacey Thomas, and Margie Southward. I appreciate the valuable contribution of the participants and their families. I extend my sincere thanks to teachers and administrators at the Southeast Asian Preschool Program in Minneapolis, MN for their support in coordinating the testing. I thank the reviewers and the editor for their comments and suggestions, which led to significant improvements in this article.

References

REFERENCES

American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (1997). Guidelines for audiologic screening. Rockville, MD: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.Google Scholar
Archibald, L. M. D., Gathercole, S. E. & Joanisse, M. F. (2009). Multisyllabic nonwords: more than a string of syllables. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 125, 1712–22.Google Scholar
Bates, E. & Goodman, J. (1997). On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: evidence from acquisition, aphasia and real-time processing. In Altmann, G. (ed.), Special issue on the lexicon: Language and Cognitive Processes 12(5/6), 507–86.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology–Learning Memory and Cognition 34, 859–73.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge, MA. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Borovsky, A. & Elman, J. (2006). Language input and semantic categories: a relation between cognition and early word learning. Journal of Child Language 33, 759–90.Google Scholar
Capone, N. C. & McGregor, K. K. (2005). The effect of semantic representation on toddlers' word retrieval. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 48, 1468–80.Google Scholar
Carey, S. & Bartlett, E. (1978). Acquiring a single new word. Proceedings of the Stanford Child Language Conference 15, 1729.Google Scholar
de Bot, K., Lowie, W. & Verspoor, M. (2007). A Dynamic Systems Theory approach to second language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 10, 721.Google Scholar
Elman, J. L. (1995). Language as a dynamical system. In Port, R. & van Gelder, T. (eds.), Mind as motion, 195225. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Francis, W. S. (1999). Cognitive integration of language and memory in bilinguals: semantic representation. Psychological Bulletin 125, 193222.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: the nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics 27, 513–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M. (2007). Miami and North Wales, so far and yet so near: a constructivist account of morphosyntactic development in bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 10, 224–47.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Bailey, L. M. & Wenger, N. R. (1992). Young-children and adults use lexical principles to learn new nouns. Developmental Psychology 28, 99108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, J. C., Dale, P. S. & Li, P. (2008). Does frequency count? Parental input and the acquisition of vocabulary. Journal of Child Language 35, 515–31.Google Scholar
Gray, S. (2003). Word-learning by preschoolers with specific language impairment: What predicts success? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 46, 5667.Google Scholar
Gupta, P. & Tisdale, J. (2009). Word learning, phonological short-term memory, phonotactic probability and long-term memory: towards an integrated framework. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B–Biological Sciences 364, 3755–71.Google Scholar
Heibeck, T. H. & Markman, E. M. (1987). Word learning in children – an examination of fast mapping. Child Development 58, 1021–34.Google Scholar
Horst, J. S. & Samuelson, L. K. (2008). Fast mapping but poor retention by 24-month-old infants. Infancy 13, 128–57.Google Scholar
Jarrold, C., Thorn, A. S. C. & Stephens, E. (2009). The relationships among verbal short-term memory, phonological awareness, and new word learning: evidence from typical development and Down syndrome. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 102, 196218.Google Scholar
Kan, P. F. & Kohnert, K. (2005). Preschoolers learning Hmong and English: lexical–semantic skills in L1 and L2. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 48, 372–83.Google Scholar
Kan, P. F. & Kohnert, K. (2008). Fast mapping by bilingual preschool children. Journal of Child Language 35, 495514.Google Scholar
Kan, P. F. & Kohnert, K. (2012). A growth curve analysis of novel word learning by sequential bilingual preschool children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 15, 452–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kan, P. F. & Windsor, J. (2010). Word learning in children with primary language impairment: a meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 53, 739–56.Google Scholar
Kohnert, K., Kan, P. F. & Conboy, B. T. (2010). Lexical and grammatical associations in sequential bilingual preschoolers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 53, 684–98.Google Scholar
Kormi-Nouri, R., Moniri, S. & Nilsson, L. G. (2003). Episodic and semantic memory in bilingual and monolingual children. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 44, 4754.Google Scholar
Li, P., Farkas, I. & MacWhinney, B. (2004). Early lexical acquisition in a self-organizing neural network. Neural Networks 17, 1345–62.Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A. & Fernald, A. (2008). Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science 11(3), F9F16.Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A., Fernald, A. & Hurtado, N. (2010). How vocabulary size in two languages relates to efficiency in spoken word recognition by young Spanish–English bilinguals. Journal of Child Language 37, 817–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markson, L. & Bloom, P. (1997). Evidence against a dedicated system for word learning in children. Nature 385, 813–15.Google Scholar
Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? New York: Dial Press.Google Scholar
McGregor, K. K., Newman, R. M., Reilly, R. M. & Capone, N. C. (2002). Semantic representation and naming in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 45, 9981015.Google Scholar
Messer, M. H., Leseman, P. P. M., Boom, J. & Mayo, A. Y. (2010). Phonotactic probability effect in nonword recall and its relationship with vocabulary in monolingual and bilingual preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 105, 306–23.Google Scholar
Miller, J. F. & Chapman, R. S. (1981). The relation between age and mean length of utterance in morphemes. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 24, 154–61.Google Scholar
Miller, J. & Iglesias, A. (2006). Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT), English & Spanish (Version 9). Madison, WI: Language Analysis Lab, University of Wisconsin-Madison.Google Scholar
Molenaar, P., Sinclair, K., Rovine, M., Ram, N. & Corneal, S. (2009). Analyzing developmental processes on an individual level using nonstationary time series modeling. Developmental Psychology 45, 260–71.Google Scholar
Neuman, S. B., Newman, E. H. & Dwyer, J. (2011). Educational effects of a vocabulary intervention on preschoolers' word knowledge and conceptual development: a cluster-randomized trial. Reading Research Quarterly 46, 249–72.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z. (2007). Social factors in childhood bilingualism in the United States. Applied Psycholinguistics 28, 399410.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, S. C., Lewedeg, V. & Oller, D. K. (1997). The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics 18, 4158.Google Scholar
Peña, E. D. (2007). Lost in translation: methodological considerations in cross-cultural research. Child Development 78, 1255–64.Google Scholar
Peña, E. D., Bedore, L. M. & Zlatic-Giunta, R. (2002). Category-generation performance of bilingual children: the influence of condition, category, and language. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 45, 938–47.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L., Oetting, J. B., Marquis, J., Bode, J. & Pae, S. Y. (1994). Frequency of input effects on word comprehension of children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 37, 106–22.Google Scholar
Roid, G. H. & Miller, L. J. (1997). Leiter International Performance Scale – Revised: examiner's manual. In Roid, G. H. and Miller, L. J., Leiter International Performance Scale – Revised. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting Co.Google Scholar
Smalley, W. A. (1990). Mother of writing: the origin and development of a Hmong messianic script. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Smith, L. B. & Thelen, E. (2003). Development as a dynamic system. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7, 343–48.Google Scholar
Storkel, H. L. (2001). Learning new words: phonotactic probability in language development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 44, 1321–37.Google Scholar
Thorn, A. S. C., Gathercole, S. E. & Frankish, C. R. (2002). Language familiarity effects in short-term memory: the role of output delay and long-term knowledge. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A–Human Experimental Psychology 55, 1363–83.Google Scholar
van Geert, P. (2011). The contribution of complex dynamic systems to development. Child Development Perspectives 5, 273–78.Google Scholar
Vlach, H. A. & Sandhofer, C. M. (2012). Fast mapping across time: memory processes support children's retention of learned words. Frontiers in Psychology. Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3286766/?tool=pubmed.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, K. M. & Mazzitelli, K. (2003). The effect of ‘missing’ information on children's retention of fast-mapped labels. Journal of Child Language 30, 4773.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodward, A. L., Markman, E. M. & Fitzsimmons, C. M. (1994). Rapid word learning in 13-month-olds and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology 30, 553–66.Google Scholar