Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:24:57.600Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Morphological priming in child German*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 November 2013

HARALD CLAHSEN*
Affiliation:
Potsdam Research Institute for Multilingualism (PRIM), University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
ELISABETH FLEISCHHAUER
Affiliation:
Potsdam Research Institute for Multilingualism (PRIM), University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
*
Address for correspondence: Harald Clahsen, University of Potsdam, Potsdam Research Institute for Multilingualism, Karl-Liebknecht-Strasse 24–25, 14476 Potsdam, Germany. tel.: +49 (0)331/ 977-2751; +49 (0)331/ 977-2687; e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Regular and irregular inflection in children's production has been examined in many previous studies. Yet, little is known about the processes involved in children's recognition of inflected words. To gain insight into how children process inflected words, the current study examines regular -t and irregular -n participles of German using the cross-modal priming technique testing 108 monolingual German-speaking children in two age groups (group I, mean age: 8;4, group II, mean age: 9;9) and a control group of 72 adults. Although both age groups of children had the same full priming effect as adults for -t forms, only children of age group II showed an adult-like (partial) priming effect for -n participles. We argue that children (within the age range tested) employ the same mechanisms for regular inflection as adults but that the lexical retrieval processes required for irregular forms become more efficient when children get older.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

The research reported here was supported by an Alexander-von-Humboldt-Professorship to HC and a Cusanuswerk postgraduate studentship to EF. We are grateful to the members of the Potsdam Research Institute for Multilingualism, particularly João Veríssimo, Gunnar Jacob, and Christiane Wotschack, for detailed and helpful comments on the present work.

References

REFERENCES

Allen, M. & Badecker, W. (2002). Inflectional regularity: probing the nature of lexical representation in a cross-modal priming task. Journal of Memory and Language 46, 705–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amenta, S. & Crepaldi, D. (2012). Morphological processing as we know it: an analytical review of morphological effects in visual word identification. Frontiers in Psychology 3(232), 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R. & Gulikers, L. (1995). The CELEX lexical database (version release 2) [CD-ROM]. Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Wurm, L. H. & Aycock, J. (2007). Lexical dynamics for low-frequency complex words: a regression study across tasks and modalities. The Mental Lexicon 2, 419–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barbour, J. S. (1982). Productive and non-productive morphology: the case of the German strong verbs. Journal of Linguistics 18, 331–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beedham, C. (1994). The role of consonants in marking strong verb conjugation in German and English. Folia Linguistica 28, 279–96.Google Scholar
Beedham, C. (1995/1996). Vowel+consonant and consonant+vowel sequences in the strong verbs of German and English. Cahiers Ferdinand de Saussure 49, 139–63.Google Scholar
Bittner, A. (1996). Starke “schwache” Verben, schwache “starke” Verben. Tübingen: Narr, Stauffenburg-Verlag.Google Scholar
Burani, C. & Thornton, A. (2003). The interplay of root, suffix and whole-word frequency in processing derived words. In Baayen, H. & Schreuder, R. (eds), Morphological structure in language processing, 157208. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. (1995). Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes 10, 425–55.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. & White, M. N. (1973). Age-of-acquisition norms for 220 picturable nouns. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 12, 563–76.Google Scholar
Casalis, S., Dusautoir, M., Colé, P. & Ducrot, S. (2009). Morphological effects in children's word reading: a priming study in fourth graders. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 27, 761–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H. (1997). The representation of participles in the German mental lexicon: evidence for the dual-mechanism model. Yearbook of Morphology 1996, 7395.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. (1999). Lexical entries and rules of language: a multidisciplinary study of German inflection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22, 9911013.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., Eisenbeiss, S., Hadler, M. & Sonnenstuhl, I. (2001). The mental representation of inflected words: an experimental study of adjectives and verbs in German. Language 77, 510–43.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., Hadler, M. & Weyerts, H. (2004). Speeded production of inflected words in children and adults. Journal of Child Language 31, 683712.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., Prüfert, P., Eisenbeiss, S. & Cholin, J. (2002). Strong stems in the German mental lexicon: evidence from child language acquisition and adult processing. In Kaufmann, I. & Stiebels, B. (eds), More than words: a festschrift for Dieter Wunderlich, 91112. Berlin: Akademie-Verlag.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. & Rothweiler, M. (1993). Inflectional rules in children's grammars: evidence from German participles. Yearbook of Morphology 1992, 134.Google Scholar
Davis, C. & Bowers, J. (2006). Contrasting five different theories of letter position coding: evidence from orthographic similarity effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 32, 535–57.Google Scholar
Deacon, S. H., Campbell, E., Tamminga, M. & Kirby, J. (2010). Seeing the harm in harmed and harmful: morphological processing by children in Grades 4, 6, and 8. Applied Psycholinguistics 31, 759–75.Google Scholar
Drasdowski, G. (1993). Duden. Mannheim: Brockhaus.Google Scholar
Durrell, M. (1980). Morphophonologische und morpholexische Regelmäßigkeiten im deutschen Ablautsystem. Jahrbuch für internationale Germanistik 8, 1928.Google Scholar
Durrell, M. (2001). Strong verb ablaut in the West Germanic languages. In Watts, S., West, J. & Solms, H. (eds), Zur Verbmorphologie germanischer Sprachen, 518. Tübingen: Niemeyer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elsen, H. (1998). The acquisition of past participles: one or two mechanisms. In Fabri, R., Ortmann, A. & Parodi, T. (eds), Models of inflection, 134–51. Tübingen: Niemeyer.Google Scholar
Faust, M. E., Balota, D. A., Spieler, D. H. & Ferraro, F. R. (1999). Individual differences in information-processing rate and amount: implications for group differences in response latency. Psychological Bulletin 125, 777–99.Google Scholar
Feldman, L. B., Rueckl, J., DiLiberto, K., Pastizzo, M. & Vellutino, F. R. (2002). Morphological analysis by child readers as revealed by the fragment completion task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 9, 529–35.Google Scholar
Fleischhauer, E. & Clahsen, H. (2012). Generating inflected word forms in real time: evaluating the role of age, frequency, and working memory. In Chung, E. & Kimball, A. (eds), Proceedings of BUCLD 36 Vol. 1, 164–76. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Forster, K. (1998). The pros and cons of masked priming. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 27, 203–33.Google Scholar
Forster, K. I. & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: a Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods 35, 116–24.Google Scholar
Gonnerman, L. M., Seidenberg, M. S. & Andersen, E. S. (2007). Graded semantic and phonological similarity effects in priming: evidence for a distributed connectionist approach to morphology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 136, 323–45.Google Scholar
Grimm, J. & Grimm, W. (1991). Deutsches Wörterbuch. Munich: DTV.Google Scholar
Harm, M. & Seidenberg, M. (1999). Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: insights from connectionist models. Psychological Review 106(3), 491528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Köpcke, K. M. (1998). Prototypisch starke und schwache Verben der deutschen Gegenwartssprache. Germanistische Linguistik 141, 4560.Google Scholar
Küspert, P. & Schneider, W. (1998). Würzburger Leise Leseprobe. Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Lindner, K. (1998). Overgeneralization revisited: the case of German past participles. In Fabri, R., Ortmann, A. & Parodi, T. (eds) Models of inflection, 152–74. Tübingen: Niemeyer.Google Scholar
Marcus, G., Brinkmann, U., Clahsen, H., Wiese, R. & Pinker, S. (1995). German inflection: the exception that proves the rule. Cognitive Psychology 29, 189256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marslen-Wilson, W. (2007). Morphological processes in language comprehension. In Gaskel, G. (ed.), The Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics, 175–93. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W., Tyler, L. K., Waksler, R. & Older, L. (1994). Morphology and meaning in the English mental lexicon. Psychological Review 101, 333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meunier, F. & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2004). Regularity and irregularity in French verbal inflection. Language and Cognitive Processes 19, 561–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrison, C. M., Chappell, T. D. & Ellis, A. W. (1997). Age of acquisition norms for a large set of object names and their relation to adult estimates and other variables. Quartely Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology 50, 528–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neubauer, K. & Clahsen, H. (2009). Decomposition of inflected words in a second language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 31, 403–35.Google Scholar
Paul, H. (1992). Deutsches Wörterbuch. Tübingen: Max Niemeyer Verlag.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1999). Words and rules: the ingredients of language. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Plag, I. & Baayen, R. H. (2009). Suffix ordering and morphological processing. Language 85, 109–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollatsek, A. & Well, A. D. (1995). On the use of counterbalanced designs in cognitive research: a suggestion for a better and more powerful analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 21, 785–94.Google Scholar
Quémart, P., Casalis, S. & Colé, P. (2011). The role of form and meaning in the processing of written morphology: a priming study in French developing readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 109, 478–96.Google Scholar
Rabin, J. & Deacon, H. (2008). The representation of morphologically complex words in the developing lexicon. Journal of Child Language 35, 453–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ravid, D. D. (2011). Spelling morphology: the psycholinguistics of Hebrew spelling (Vol. 3). Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Rosa, J. M. & Nunes, T. (2008). Morphological priming effects on children's spelling. Reading and Writing 21, 763–81.Google Scholar
Schiff, R., Raveh, M. & Kahta, S. (2008). The developing mental lexicon: evidence from morphological priming of irregular Hebrew forms. Reading and Writing 21, 719–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schröder, A., Gemballa, T., Ruppin, S. & Wartenburger, I. (2012). German norms for semantic typicality, age of acquisition, and concept familiarity. Behavioural Research 44, 380–94.Google Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S. & Gonnerman, L. M. (2000). Explaining derivational morphology as the convergence of codes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4, 353–61.Google Scholar
Silva, R. & Clahsen, H. (2008). Morphologically complex words in L1 and L2 processing: evidence from masked priming experiments in English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 11, 245–60.Google Scholar
Smolka, E., Zwitserlood, P. & Rösler, F. (2007). Stem access in regular and irregular inflection: evidence from German participles. Journal of Memory and Language 57, 325–47.Google Scholar
Sonnenstuhl, I., Eisenbeiss, S. & Clahsen, H. (1999). Morphological priming in the German mental lexicon. Cognition 72, 203–36.Google Scholar
Szagun, G. (2011). Regular/irregular is not the whole story: the role of frequency and generalization in the acquisition of German past participle inflection. Journal of Child Language 38, 731–62.Google Scholar
Tewes, U. (1983). Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligenztest für Kinder (HAWIK-R). Bern: Huber.Google Scholar
Tewes, U. (1991). Hamburg-Wechsler-Intelligenztest für Erwachsene (HAWIE-R). Bern: Huber.Google Scholar
Wahrig, G. (1997). Deutsches Wörterbuch. Gütersloh: Bertelsmann.Google Scholar
Weyerts, H. & Clahsen, H. (1994). Netzwerke und symbolische Regeln im Spracherwerb: experimentelle Ergebnisse zur Entwicklung der Flexionsmorphologie. Linguistische Berichte 154, 430–60.Google Scholar
Wiese, R. (1996). Phonological versus morphological rules: on German umlaut and ablaut. Journal of Linguistics 32, 113–35.Google Scholar
Wunderlich, D. (1996). Minimalist morphology: the role of paradigms. Yearbook of Morphology 1995, 93114.Google Scholar
Wunderlich, D. & Fabri, R. (1995). Minimalist morphology: an approach to inflection. Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft 14, 236–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar