Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T05:22:36.842Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The moderating effect of Korean preschoolers’ receptive and expressive language skills on the link between Korean PA and English PA

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 June 2019

Yea-Ji HONG
Affiliation:
Department of Child Development and Family Studies, Seoul National University
Soon-Hyung YI*
Affiliation:
Department of Child Development and Family Studies, Seoul National University
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The purpose of the current study was to examine whether oral language skills moderate the effect of Korean phonological awareness (PA) on English PA for Korean preschoolers in the initial stage of learning English as a second language. The study participants comprised 81 five- to six-year-old Korean preschoolers attending Korean-medium preschools in South Korea. The findings demonstrated that Korean PA was significantly associated with English PA. In addition, Korean receptive and expressive language skills had moderating effects on the relationship between Korean PA and English PA, respectively. This study is discussed not only in terms of cross-language PA transfer in processing two phonologically and orthographically different languages but also in light of the importance of native language skills interacting with native PA in the second-language PA development of preschool children in the initial stage of learning a second language.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Majumder, S., & Martin, M. M. (2003). Developing phonological awareness: Is there a bilingual advantage? Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(1), 2744.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., McBride-Chang, E., & Luk, G. (2005). Bilingualism, language proficiency, and learning to read in two writing systems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97(4), 580–90.Google Scholar
Branum-Martin, L., Mehta, P. D., Fletcher, J. M., Carlson, C. D., Ortiz, A., Carlo, M., & Francis, D. J. (2006). Bilingual phonological awareness: multilevel construct validation among Spanish-speaking kindergarteners in transitional bilingual education classrooms. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 170–81.Google Scholar
Campbell, R., & Sais, E. (1995). Accelerated metalinguistic (phonological) awareness in bilingual children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13(1), 61–8.Google Scholar
Chaney, C. (1992). Language development, metalinguistic skills, and print awareness in 3-year-old children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 13(4), 485514.Google Scholar
Chaney, C. (1994). Language development, metalinguistic awareness, and emergency literacy skills of 3-year-old children in relation to social class. Applied Psycholinguistics, 15(3), 371–94.Google Scholar
Chen, X., Xu, F., Nguyen, T.-K., Hong, G., & Wang, Y. (2010). Effects of cross-language transfer on first-language phonological awareness and literacy skills in Chinese children receiving English instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(3), 712–28.Google Scholar
Cho, J.-R., & McBride-Chang, C. (2005). Levels of phonological awareness in Korean and English: a 1-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97(4), 564–71.Google Scholar
Cho, J.-R., McBride-Chang, C., & Park, S.-G. (2008). Phonological awareness and morphological awareness: differential associations to regular and irregular word recognition in early Korean Hangul readers. Reading and Writing, 21(3), 255–74.Google Scholar
Cisero, C. A., & Royer, J. M. (1995). The development and cross-language transfer of phonological awareness. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 20(3), 275303.Google Scholar
Coulmas, F. (2003). Writing systems: an introduction to their linguistic analysis. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cummins, J. (1979). Linguistic interdependence and the educational development of bilingual children. Review of Educational Research, 49(2), 222–51.Google Scholar
D’Angiulli, A., Siegel, L. S., & Serra, E. (2001). The development of reading in English and Italian in bilingual children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22(4), 479507.Google Scholar
Dickinson, D. K., McCabe, A., Clark-Chiarelli, N., & Wolf, A. (2004). Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness in low-income Spanish and English bilingual preschool children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25(3), 323–47.Google Scholar
Dixon, L. Q., Zhao, J., & Joshi, R. M. (2010). Influence of L1 orthography on spelling English words by bilingual children: a natural experiment comparing syllabic, phonological, and morphosyllabic first languages. Learning Disability Quarterly, 33, 211–21.Google Scholar
Durgunoğlu, A. Y., Nagy, W. E., & Hancin-Bhatt, B. J. (1993). Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(3), 453–65.Google Scholar
Goswami, U. (1991). Learning about spelling sequences: the role of onsets and rimes in analogies in reading. Child Development, 62(5), 1110–23.Google Scholar
Gottardo, A., & Mueller, J. (2009). Are first- and second-language factors related in predicting second language reading comprehension? A study of Spanish-speaking children acquiring English as a second language from first to second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(2), 330–44.Google Scholar
Kang, J. Y. (2012). Do bilingual children possess better phonological awareness? Investigation of Korean monolingual and Korean–English bilingual children. Reading and Writing, 25(2), 411–31.Google Scholar
Kim, A.-Y., Park, A., & Lust, B. (2018). Simultaneous vs. successive bilingualism among preschool-aged children: a study of four-year-old Korean–English bilinguals in the USA. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 21(2), 164–78.Google Scholar
Kim, Y.-S. (2007). Phonological awareness and literacy skills in Korean: an examination of the unique role of body–coda units. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(1), 6994.Google Scholar
Kim, Y.-S. (2008). Cat in the hat or cat in the cap? An investigation of the developmental trajectories of phonological awareness for Korean children. Journal of Research in Reading, 31(4), 359–78.Google Scholar
Kim, Y.-S. (2009a). Crosslinguistic influence on phonological awareness for Korean–English bilingual children. Reading and Writing, 22, 843–61.Google Scholar
Kim, Y.-S. (2009b). The foundation of literacy skills in Korean: the relationship between letter name knowledge and phonological awareness and their relative contribution to literacy skills. Reading and Writing, 22, 907–31.Google Scholar
Kim, Y. T., Seong, T. J., & Lee, Y. K. (2003). Preschool receptive–expressive language scale (PRES). Seoul: Seoul Community Rehabilitation Center.Google Scholar
Kuo, L.-J., Uchikoshi, Y., Kim, T.-J., & Yang, X. (2016). Bilingualism and phonological awareness: re-examining theories of cross-language transfer and structural sensitivity. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 46, 19.Google Scholar
Loizou, M., & Stuart, M. (2003). Phonological awareness in monolingual and bilingual English and Greek five-year-olds. Journal of Research in Reading, 26(1), 318.Google Scholar
Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., Anthony, J. L., & Barker, T. A. (1998). Development of phonological sensitivity in 2- to 5-year old children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(2), 294311.Google Scholar
Lopez, L. M., & Greenfield, D. B. (2004). The cross-language transfer of phonological skills of Hispanic head start children. Bilingual Research Journal, 28(1), 118.Google Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Cho, J.-R., Liu, H., Wagner, R. K., Shu, H., Zhou, A., Cheuk, C. S.-M., & Muse, A. (2005). Changing models across cultures: associations of phonological awareness and morphological structure awareness with vocabulary and word recognition in second graders from Beijing, Hong Kong, Korea, and the United States. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92(2), 140–60.Google Scholar
Metsala, J. L. (1999). Young children's phonological awareness and nonword repetition as a function of vocabulary development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(1), 319.Google Scholar
Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In Metsala, J. L. & Ehri, L. C. (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89120). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nuske, H. J., & Bavin, E. L. (2010). Narrative comprehension in 4–7-year-old children with autism: testing the Weak Central Coherence account. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorder, 46(1), 108–19.Google Scholar
Pae, H. K., Sevcik, R. A., & Morris, R. D. (2010). Cross-language correlates in phonological awareness and naming speed: evidence from deep and shallow orthographies. Journal of Research in Reading, 33(4), 374–91.Google Scholar
Park, H. W., Kwak, K. J., & Park, K. B. (1995). Korean-Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence. Seoul: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Sattler, J. M., & Ryan, J. J. (1999). Assessment of children: revised and updated third edition, WAIS–III supplement. San Diego, CA: Jerome M. Sattler.Google Scholar
Scarpino, S. E., Lawrence, F. R., Davison, M. D., & Hammer, C. S. (2011). Predicting bilingual Spanish–English children's phonological awareness abilities from their preschool English and Spanish oral language. Journal of Research in Reading, 34(1), 7793.Google Scholar
Stahl, S. A., & Murray, B. A. (1994). Defining phonological awareness and its relationship to early reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(2), 221–34.Google Scholar
Sun-Alperin, M. K., & Wang, M. (2011). Cross-language transfer of phonological and orthographic processing skills from Spanish L1 to English L2. Reading and Writing, 24(5), 591614.Google Scholar
Tabors, P. (1997). One child, two languages. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Taylor, I., & Taylor, M. M. (1995). Writing and literacy in Chinese, Korean, and Japanese. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Treiman, R. (1987). On the relationship between phonological awareness and literacy. Current Psychology of Cognition, 7(5), 524–9.Google Scholar
Treiman, R., & Zukowski, A. (1996). Children's sensitivity to syllables, onsets, rimes and phonemes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61(3), 193215.Google Scholar
Uchikoshi, Y. (2013). Predictors of English reading comprehension: Cantonese-speaking English language learners in the U.S. Reading and Writing, 26(6), 913–39.Google Scholar
Wang, M., Park, Y., & Lee, K. R. (2006). Korean–English biliteracy acquisition: cross-language phonological and orthographic transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 148–58.Google Scholar
Yoon, H.-K., Bolger, D. J., Kwon, O.-S., & Perfetti, C. A. (2002). Subsyllabic units in reading: a difference between Korean and English. In Verhoeven, L., Elbro, C., & Reitsma, P. (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 139–64). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Yoon, Y. B., & Derwing, B. L. (2001). A language without a rhyme: syllable structure experiments in Korean. Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 46(3/4), 187237.Google Scholar
Yopp, R. H., & Yopp, H. K. (2000). Sharing informational text with young children. Reading Teacher, 53(5), 410–23.Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 329.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Hong and Yi supplementary material

Appendices A-B

Download Hong and Yi supplementary material(File)
File 26.5 KB