Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:12:39.623Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mechanisms linking phonological development to lexical development – a commentary on Stoel-Gammon's ‘Relationships between lexical and phonological development in young children’*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 October 2010

ERIKA HOFF*
Affiliation:
Florida Atlantic University
MARISOL PARRA
Affiliation:
Florida Atlantic University
*
Address for correspondence: Erika Hoff, Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Davie, FL 33314. e-mail: [email protected]

Extract

When Roger Brown selected Adam, Eve and Sarah to be the first three participants in the modern study of child language, one of the criteria was the intelligibility of their speech (Brown, 1973). According to the prevailing view at the time, accuracy of pronunciation was a peripheral phenomenon that had nothing to do with the development of language qua language. So why not study children who were easy to transcribe? One reason why not, according to Stoel-Gammon (SG; this issue), is that the difficulty of accurately producing sounds influences the words children acquire and the rate at which they acquire them. (It's true that Roger Brown's focus was on the child's acquisition of morphosyntax, but articulation was assumed to be peripheral to everything back then.) This interaction between the articulatory skill of children and phonological properties of words is just one of the mutual influences between phonology and the lexicon SG describes. In her target article, SG brings together data from a wide range of investigations to build an account of how phonology and the lexicon interact in development.

Type
Review Article and Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This work was partially supported by grant R21HD060718 to the first author from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.

References

REFERENCES

Beckman, M. E. & Edwards, J. (2000). The ontogeny of phonological categories and the primacy of lexical learning in linguistic development. Child Development 71, 240–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berko, J. & Brown, R. (1960). Psycholinguistic research methods. In Mussen, P. H. (ed.), Handbook of research methods in child development, 517–57. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, S. (1920/1980). The tum phenomenon. Journal of Child Language 7, 428–29 [originally published 1920].CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coady, J. A. & Evans, J. L. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non-word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 43, 140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eilers, R. E. & Oller, D. K. (1976). The role of speech discrimination in developmental sound substitutions. Journal of Child Language 3, 319–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: the nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics 27, 513–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geirut, J. A. (1998). Production, conceptualization and change in distinctive featural categories. Journal of Child Language 25, 321–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E., Core, C. & Bridges, K. (2008). Nonword repetition assesses phonological memory and is related to vocabulary development in 20- to 24-month-olds. Journal of Child Language 35, 903916.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parra, M., Hoff, E. & Core, C. (in press). Relations among language exposure, phonological memory, and language development in Spanish-English bilingually-developing 2-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.Google Scholar
Rvachew, S., Ohberg, A., Grawburg, M. & Heyding, J. (2003). Phonological awareness and phonemic perception in 4-year-old children with delayed expressive phonology skills. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 12, 463–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, R. & Leonard, L. (1982). Do children pick and choose? An examination of phonological selection and avoidance in early lexical acquisition. Journal of Child Language 9, 319–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shatz, M. (1987). Bootstrapping operations in child language. In Nelson, K. E. & Van Kleeck, A. (eds), Children's language, vol. 6, 122. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Swingley, D. (2005). 11-month-olds' knowledge of how familiar words sound. Developmental Science 8, 432–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swingley, D. (2007). Lexical exposure and word-form encoding in 1·5-year-olds. Developmental Psychology 43, 454–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tardif, T., Fletcher, P., Liang, W. & Kaciroti, N. (2009). Early vocabulary development in Mandarin (Putonghua) and Cantonese. Journal of Child Language 36, 1115–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorn, S. C. & Gathercole, S. E. (1999). Language-specific knowledge and short-term memory in bilingual and non-bilingual children, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology 52, 303324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walley, A. C. (1988). Spoken word recognition by young children and adults. Cognitive Development 3, 137–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar