We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
References
REFERENCES
Bruner, J. S. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. JChLang2. 1–19.Google Scholar
Chapin, G., Smith, T. S. & Abrahamson, A. A. (1972). Two factors in perceptual segmentation of speech. JVLVB11. 164–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. & Clark, E. (1977). Psychology and language. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. & Bever, T. G. (1965). The psychological reality of linguistic segments. JVLVB4. 414–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fromkin, V. A. (1971). The non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances. Lg47. 27–52.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (1978). Structural parallels between language and action in development. In Lock, A. J. (ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: the emergence of language. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1975). Learning how to mean. London: Edward Arnold.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarvella, R. J. (1971). Syntactic processing of connected speech. JVLVB10. 409–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lock, A. J. (1980). The guided reinvention of language. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Menn, L. (1973). On the origin and growth of phonological and syntactic rules. Papers from the 9th Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, 378–85.Google Scholar