Hostname: page-component-68945f75b7-6cjkg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-04T23:31:25.242Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Linguistic experience modifies lexical stress perception*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

George D. Allen
Affiliation:
Purdue University

Abstract

Sensitivity to differences in lexical stress pattern was examined in 4- and 5-year-old monolingual French-, German- and Swedish-speaking children. For most stimulus discriminations, the 5-year-olds outperformed their 4-year-old comparison groups. For a discrimination involving a trisyllabic distinction not found in French, however, the French 5-year-olds performed worse than their 4-year-old compatriots, suggesting that the older children had ‘learned’ not to hear the trisyllabic distinction. In follow-up testing of the French 4-year-olds six months later, half of them showed a similar decrease in performance specific to the trisyllabic stimuli. These data support an ‘attunement’ theory of language acquisition, in which potentially relevant abilities that are already partially or fully developed at birth may become attenuated or completely lost if they are inappropriate or irrelevant for the child's language.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1983

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Allen, G. D. (in press). The TAKI task: cross-linguistic comparisons of rhythmic constraints in children's developing phonology. Salzburger Beiträge zur Linguistik.Google Scholar
Allen, G. D. & Hawkins, S. (1978). The development of phonological rhythm. In Bell, A. & Hooper, J. B. (eds), Syllables and segments. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Allen, G. D. (1980). Phonological rhythm: definition and development. In Yeni-Komshian, G. H., Kavanagh, J. F. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Child phonology. Vol. 1: Production. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Aslin, R. N. & Pisoni, D. B. (1980). Some developmental processes in speech perception. In Yeni-Komshian, G. H., Kavanagh, J. F. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Child phonology. Vol 1: Production. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Atkinson-King, K. (1973). Children's acquisition of phonological stress contrasts. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics, 25.Google Scholar
Bannert, R. (1976). Mittelbairische Phonologie auf akustischer und perzeptorischer Grundlage. Lund: Gleerup.Google Scholar
Blasdell, R. & Jensen, P. (1970). Stress and word position as determinants of imitation in first-language learners. JSpHearRes 13. 193202.Google ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S. P., Tucker, G. R. & Lambert, W. E. (1967). The comparative skills of monolinguals and bilinguals in perceiving phoneme sequences. L & S 10. 159–68.Google ScholarPubMed
Crystal, D. (1973). Non-segmental phonology in language acquisition: a review of the issues. Lingua 32. 145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crystal, D. (1975). The English tone of voice. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Davine, M., Tucker, G. R. & Lambert, W. E. (1967). The perception of phoneme sequences by monolingual and bilingual elementary school children. CanJBehavSci 3. 72–6.Google Scholar
Dell, F. (1978). Certains corrélats de la distinction entre morphologie dérivationnelle et morphologic flexionnelle dana la phonologic de français. In Morin, Y. C. & Querido, A. A. (eds), Recherches Linguistiques à Montréal 7. Montreal: McGill University, University of Montreal, and University of Quebec at Montreal.Google Scholar
Diller, A-M. (1978). The mute e in French as a sociolinguistic variable. In Morin, Y. C. & Querido, A. A. (eds), Recherches Linguistiques à Montréal 7. McGill University, University of Montreal, and University of Quebec at Montreal.Google Scholar
Elert, C.-C. (1970). Ljud och ord i svenskan. Stockholm: Almqvist & Wiksell.Google Scholar
Fónagy, I. (1972). A propos de la genèse de la phrase enfantine. Lingua 30. 3171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garde, P. (1968). L'accent. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1976 a). Conceptions of prenatal development: behavioral embryology. PsychRev 83. 215–34.Google ScholarPubMed
Gottlieb, G. (1976 b). The roles of experience in the development of behavior and the nervous system. In Gottlieb, G. (ed.), Neural and behavioral specificity. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hadding-Koch, K. (1961). Acoustico-phonetic studies in the intonation of southern Swedish. Lund: Gleerup.Google Scholar
Hyman, L. M. (1977). On the nature of linguistic stress. In Hyman, L. M. (ed.), Studies in stress and accent. Southern California Occasional Papers in Linguistics 4.Google Scholar
Konopczynski, G. (1975). Étude expérimentale de quelques structures prosodiques employées par les enfants français entre 7 et 22 mois. Travaux de l'Institut de Phonétique de Strasbourg 7. 171205.Google Scholar
Lehiste, I. (1970). Suprasegmentals. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Locke, J. L. (1978). Selective loss of phonetic production and perception: an index to the child's acquisition of phonology. Journal of the National Student Speech and Hearing Association 6. 311.Google Scholar
Malikouti-Drachman, A. & Drachman, G. (1975). The acquisition of stress in modern Greek. Salzburger Beiträge zur Linguistik 2. 277–89.Google Scholar
Mowrer, O. H. (1952). Speech development in the young child. I. The autism theory of speech development and some clinical applications. JSpHearDis 17. 263–8.Google Scholar
Pike, K. L. & Scott, G. (1963). Pitch accent and non-accented phrases in Fore (New Guinea). Zeitschrift für Phonetik 16. 179–89.Google Scholar
Pilch, H. (1970). Pike and Scott's analysis of Fore suprasegmentals. Kivung 3. 133–42.Google Scholar
Reiffenstein, I.Salzburgische Dialektgeographie: Die sudmittelbairischen Mundarten zwischen Inn und Enns. Giessen: Wilhelm Schmitz Verlag.Google Scholar
Sigurd, B. (1965). Phonotactic structures. Unpublished dissertation, University of Lund.Google Scholar
Stampe, D. (1979). A dissertation on natural phonology. Bloomington: Indiana University Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
Vanderslice, R. & Ladefoged, P. (1972). Binary suprasegmental features and transformational word-accentuation rules. Lg 48. 819–38.Google Scholar
Winer, B. J. (1962). Statistical principles in experimental design. New York: McGraw-Hill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar